Skip to content
Publicly Available Published by De Gruyter July 16, 2020

Pain perception in chronic knee osteoarthritis with varying levels of pain inhibitory control: an exploratory study

  • Paulo E. P. Teixeira EMAIL logo , Hanan I. Zehry , Swapnali Chaudhari , Laura Dipietro and Felipe Fregni

Abstract

Background and aims

Pain is a disabling symptom in knee osteoarthritis (KOA) and its underlying mechanism remains poorly understood. Dysfunction of descending pain modulatory pathways and reduced pain inhibition enhance pain facilitation in many chronic pain syndromes but do not fully explain pain levels in chronic musculoskeletal conditions. The objective of this study is to explore the association of clinical variables with pain intensity perception in KOA individuals with varying levels of Conditioned Pain Modulation (CPM) response.

Methods

This is a cross-sectional, exploratory analysis using baseline data of a randomized clinical trial investigating the effects of a non-invasive brain stimulation treatment on the perception of pain and functional limitations due to KOA. Sixty-three subjects with KOA were included in this study. Data on pain perception, mood perception, self-reported depression, physical function, quality of life, and quantitative sensory testing was collected. Multiple linear regression analysis was performed to explore the association between the clinical variables with pain perception for individuals with different levels of CPM response.

Results

For KOA patients with limited CPM response, perception of limitations at work/other activities due to emotional problems and stress scores were statistically significantly associated with pain scores, F(2, 37) = 7.02, p < 0.01. R-squared = 0.275. For KOA patients with normal CPM response, general health perception scores were statistically significantly associated with pain scores, F(1, 21) = 5.60, p < 0.05. R-squared = 0.2104. Limitations of this study include methodology details, small sample size and study design characteristics.

Conclusions

Pain intensity perception is associated differently with clinical variables according to the individual CPM response. Mechanistic models to explain pain perception in these two subgroups of KOA subjects are discussed.

1 Introduction

Knee osteoarthritis (KOA) is a form of arthritis that impairs functional abilities and decreases quality of life in patients by producing pain, stiffness, and limitation in range of motion of the knee joint [1], [2]. Recent epidemiology data suggest that KOA has been a leading contributor to global disability [3], [4] affecting approximately 27 million American adults [5] and an estimated 10% of the world’s population over the age of 60 years [6]. KOA also poses a significant economic burden accounting for more than $27 billion in health care expenditures annually in the US [7].

Pain is the most disabling symptom in KOA [8] and its underlying mechanisms have for long been poorly understood [9], [10]. Contemporary research have suggested that it may result from a combination of joint nociception (e.g. structural changes and synovitis), increased responsiveness of peripheral nociceptors during inflammation of the knee (i.e. peripheral sensitization), pathological neural signals from the joint causing central nervous system changes (i.e. central sensitization), in addition to having specific mediators and being influenced by contextual aspects such as psychosocial factors [11]. Clinical research reveals that the alterations of excess nociceptive ascending and deficient inhibitory descending control present in KOA, influences in the development and maintenance of central sensitization and may contribute to the transition from acute to chronic pain [12], [13], [14], [15], [16]. Central and peripheral sensitization can result in increased pain response and sensitivity to nociceptive stimuli at sites distant from the knee leading to relevant limitations [17], [18], [19]. These changes characterize impairments in the endogenous central pain control mechanisms, such as altered Conditioned Pain Modulation (CPM), that affects descending pain regulatory pathways and are present in knee osteoarthritis patients [20], [21].

CPM can be defined as an endogenous analgesic mechanism that occurs “when a painful stimulus (test stimulus) is inhibited by a second painful stimulus (conditioning stimulus) delivered at a different body location” [22], [23]. This neural control is triggered by noxious stimulation coming from widespread areas of the body and exert an inhibitory influence on wide-dynamic-range neurons [24]. This mechanism has been reported in many clinical studies and can be properly quantified mainly by two dynamic test paradigms that involve pain assessments, namely temporal summation and CPM testing [25], [26], [27]. Abnormal CPM has been demonstrated in many chronic pain syndromes including fibromyalgia, KOA, rheumatoid arthritis, neuropathy, and headaches [28]. Evidence suggests that peripheral nociceptive signals (e.g. originating from KOA) are strongly correlated with dysfunction of the descending pain modulatory pathways and reduced pain inhibition [15], [18], [20], [28].

Previous research have showed that differences in CPM have been consistent predictors of clinical pain and health-related variables in healthy and in musculoskeletal pain cohorts [29], [30], [31], [32]. In KOA, variability in CPM can also predict response to treatment [31]. However, questions remain whether different CPM levels are clinically relevant in characterizing chronic pain patient characteristics. Therefore, the aim of this study is to explore the association between clinical variables with pain perception in individuals with KOA with varying levels of endogenous pain inhibitory control as indexed by CPM function. We hypothesize that the pain perception associate with distinct clinical variables for those who have limited CPM response as compared to those who have normal CPM response.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Design overview

This study analyzes baseline cross-sectional data of an ongoing randomized controlled trial investigating non-invasive brain stimulation for the treatment of KOA (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02723929). All assessments and variables were collected before any randomization or stimulation was given.

2.2 Setting

This study was conducted in the Spaulding Rehabilitation Hospital (SRH) in Charlestown, MA, USA. Participants were recruited from the SRH Network greater Boston metropolitan area through online and local newspaper advertising, community flyers, and patient registries designed to promote research. All study procedures were approved by the Partner’s Healthcare Institutional Review Board (IRB number 2014P002496).

2.3 Participants

Sixty-nine people with KOA (35 women, 34 men) participated in the analysis. Demographic, socioeconomic, and disease-related data were collected from all subjects.

Inclusion criteria for the clinical trial were age between 18 and 85 years; confirmed diagnosis of KOA by medical records or physician clinical assessment; existing knee pain of at least 3 on a 0–10 Visual Analog Scale (VAS) on average over the past 6 months; pain of at least 3 on a 0–10 VAS scale on average over the past week; pain resistant to common analgesics and medications for chronic pain used as initial pain management. For more details about inclusion/exclusion criteria, please refer to clinicaltrials.gov identifier: NCT02723929. All individuals signed a written, informed consent form approved by the Partner’s Healthcare IRB prior to participation in the study.

3 Assessments

This study implemented a set of assessments for KOA subjects with currently acceptable levels of sensitivity, reliability and validity based on their applicability to the non-invasive brain stimulation trial that we were conducting and/or as recommended by the ACTTION and IMMPACT [33], [34] guidelines for conducting chronic pain trials. Blinded raters performed the following test procedures.

3.1 Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) for Pain

Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) for Pain was used to ask subjects to self-reportedly rate the pain on their most affected knee on a 0–10 (0=no pain, 10=very painful) VAS. The time period for the pain reporting was for the moment of the assessment. If there was bilateral knee pain, participants were instructed to rate the pain on their most affected knee. The VAS for pain have been studied in chronic musculoskeletal pain and have pain acceptable psychometric properties [35], [36].

3.2 Visual Analog Mood Scale (VAMS)

Visual Analog Mood Scale (VAMS) [37] is a self-assessment scale, with acceptable psychometric properties [38], in which subjects rate their own emotions, including anxiety, depression, stress, and sleepiness using a VAS similar to the described for the VAS for pain. Anchor words were used at either end of the line which described the extremes of the symptom in question. The time period for the mood reporting was for the moment of the assessment.

3.3 Mechanical Detection Threshold

This technique evaluates light touch in small cutaneous regions [39]. We used the von Frey monofilament von Frey hairs (von Frey monofilament, Touch-Test Sensory Evaluator, North Coast Medical, Morgan Hill, CA, USA) and the procedure was conducted as in reference [39], [40]. The threshold was taken as the lowest force that caused a light touch sensation.

3.4 Mechanical Pain Thresholds

Similar to the mechanical detection, the threshold to produce pain was recorded in this test. The subjects were asked to say when they sensed pain. The test was performed in the same body region as the mechanical detection test. The smallest monofilament that produced pain was recorded. The subject was asked to keep their eyes closed during the application of the fibers for both detection and pain threshold tests.

3.5 Pain pressure threshold (PPT)

Pain pressure threshold (PPT) was assessed using blunt pressure delivered by a 1-cm2 hard-rubber probe using an FDA approved device (Commander algometer – JTECH medical) as in Rolke et al. [41]. The algometer probe was applied at 90° to the skin at a constant rate of 2 lbs./s until the PPT was reached, as indicated by the patient’s verbal notification. This procedure was repeated 3 times, and the mean of the recorded values was used for analysis.

3.6 Continuous Pain Modulation (CPM)

Continuous Pain Modulation (CPM) was evaluated using pressure as the test stimulus and cold water as the conditioning stimulus as in reference [42], [43]. CPM was induced approximately 2-min later by having subjects immerse their hand into a water bath maintained at 10–12 °C for approximately 1 min. After 30 s of CPM conditioning (cold water immersion), the same PPT test was performed. The subject was asked to have their eyes closed during the both the PPT and the CPM tests and were not aware of their test results. CPM was defined as the difference in the pressure threshold from the CPM trials and the pressure threshold recorded in the PPT trials alone. Positive values revealed subjects that tolerated higher pressure values during the cold-water conditioning stimulus as compared to the PPT trials alone.

3.7 Beck Depression Inventory

Beck Depression Inventory was used to evaluate depression symptoms. This is a 21-item self-reported scale commonly used to measure depression severity with multiple choice answer options and with well-established psychometric properties [44].

3.8 Quality of life

Self-reported generic assessments of physical and mental functioning were assessed using SF-36 survey. The SF-36 Health Survey has acceptable psychometric properties as documented in studies with arthritis patients [35], [45].

3.9 Western Ontario and McMaster Universities Osteoarthritis Index (WOMAC)

Western Ontario and McMaster Universities Osteoarthritis Index (WOMAC) was as a disease specific self-reported physical function measure. The WOMAC [36] psychometric properties in people with KOA are excellent [46], [47].

4 Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to characterize the sample. Dichotomous and categorical variables were described in frequency and respective percentages. Continuous variables central tendency and variability were described by means and standard deviation or medians and interquartile ranges according to data distribution. Normality of distribution was checked by a combination of visual inspection of the histogram of data points and the Shapiro-Wilk normality test [48].

For our analysis, the CPM variable was transformed into a dichotomous variable that was used to discriminate KOA subjects with distinct levels of endogenous pain inhibitory control as indexed by conditioned pain modulation (CPM) function, namely subjects with “normal CPM response” and with “limited CPM response”. The absolute standard error of the measurement (SEM) value calculated from the pre-conditioning PPT trials was used to classify the continuous CPM variable into the dichotomous variable. This is a valid distribution-based statistical approach used to determine true change in CPM that has been used in previous studies [49], [50] to identify inhibitory CPM effects. An inhibitory CPM effect was defined as a CPM value greater than 1SEM. Subjects with CPM>1SEM were categorized as the “normal CPM response” group, referring to those who were able to modulate the pain at least above the SEM. Subjects with CPM<1SEM defined the “limited CPM response” group, referring to those who were not able to modulate pain above the SEM and consequently having a more limited or absent ability to modulate pain after the conditioning stimulus. The SEM was calculated using the formula (SD * √1-r) where r is the reliability coefficient and SD is the standard deviation of all data from the 3 PPT trials. Independent sample t-test and Mann-Whitney U test were used to check for differences in the descriptive statistics between the two subgroups. All the subjects were blinded to their CPM scores or CPM response classification.

A multiple linear regression analysis was used to investigate the association between the clinical variables and pain perception in each of the two KOA subgroups. VAS for Pain was used as the dependent variable while all other variables were used as the exploratory independent variables. All the variables were treated as continuous variables.

Outliers were defined as cases with values greater than 3 SDs above or below the mean scores of the dependent and independent variables. For each independent variable, the assumption of linearity was assessed by visually comparing the scatterplot of the variable against the plot of a superimposed regression line. The assumption of homoscedasticity was checked by visually comparing the scatterplots of the standardized predicted values and those of the standardized residuals [51]. Normality of the residuals was checked by a combination of visual inspection of the histogram of the residuals and the Shapiro-Wilk normality test [48]. Durbin Watson estimate, tolerance values, Cook’s distance, variance inflation factor and Standardized DFBeta values were requested for analysis of regression diagnostics such as multicollinearity and influential cases.

To define the best models, univariate linear models were created with each independent variable to determine the significant covariates based on the regression coefficient significance p-value using a cutoff of p<0.2. If variables were not significant, they would be automatically excluded from the final model. Using a backward elimination approach, we included all variables that were significant in the univariate regression analysis in the full models. The regression coefficients were then checked for significance in the full model and those with a p-value <0.05 were excluded from the model. The Akaike’s information criteria was also used to determine the model with the best fit. In addition, if any variables revealed to be significantly different among the KOA subgroups in the descriptive analysis, such variable would be considered for the final models. Cohen’s ƒ2 effect size was calculated for each model as it is an appropriate effect size measure for multiple regression [52]. All analysis were performed using statistical software STATA version 15 (StataCorp. 2017. Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX, USA: StataCorp LLC).

5 Results

Sixty-three subjects with KOA participated in this exploratory analysis study. The SEM for PPT based on the pre-conditioning PPT trials was 1.45 pound-force/square centimeter (64.5 kPa) which is similar to SEM values reported in the literature [49]. An inhibitory CPM effect (>+1SEM) was elicited in 36.5% (23) of subjects in response to cold stimulus (normal CPM response group) while 63.5% (40) of subjects had limited CPM response. Tables 1 and 2 summarize the sample.

Table 1:

Descriptive for categorical variables for all the participants.

Normal CPM response (n=23)
Limited CPM response (n=40)
Frequency % Frequency %
Gender
 Male 10 43.5 21 52.5
 Female 13 56.5 19 47.5
Race
 Black 10 43.5 10 25
 Caucasian 9 39.1 28 70
 American Indian or Alaska Native 1 4.3 0 0
 Asian 2 8.7 0 0
 Unknown or not reported 1 4.3 2 5
Education level
 Unknown or unreported 5 21.7 12 30
 Comprehensive school or less 6 26.1 3 7.5
 Upper secondary/vocational school or more 12 52.2 25 62.5
Employment status
 Unknown or unreported 3 13 3 7.5
 Full time 4 17.4 14 35
 Part time 4 17.4 7 17.5
 Unemployed 8 34.8 9 22.5
 Retired 4 17.4 7 17.5
Marital status
 Unknown or unreported 1 4.3 1 2.5
 Single 13 56.5 20 50
 Married 4 17.4 9 22.5
 Divorced 5 21.7 8 20
 Widowed 0 0 2 5
Table 2:

Descriptive for continuous variables for all the participants.

Normal CPM response (n=23)
Limited CPM response (n=40)
p-Value
n Central tendency Variability n Central tendency Variability
Age 23 58.9 9.2 40 62.3 9.1 0.155
Beck Depression Inventorya 23 4.0 9.0 40 3.0 4.0 0.146
Body Mass Indexa 22 30.0 11.5 39 27.1 11.2 0.279
SF36 Paina 23 45.0 25.0 39 55.0 22.5 0.590
SF36 Emotional Well Beinga 23 76.0 60.0 38 80.0 22.0 0.881
SF36 Energy Fatiguea 23 50.0 20.0 40 60.0 28.8 0.225
SF36 General Health 23 62.4 22.1 40 61.3 20.7 0.838
SF36 Physical Function 23 43.5 26.7 38 46.2 23.8 0.526
SF36 Social Functioninga 20 62.5 37.5 40 75.0 46.9 0.209
SF36_Role Limitations Emotional Problemsa 23 66.7 66.7 40 100.0 33.3 0.078
SF36_Role Limitations Physical Healtha 23 25.0 100.0 40 25.0 75.0 0.596
Mechanical Detection Threshold (knee)a 23 0.6 1.2 40 0.4 0.7 0.774
Mechanical Detection Threshold (thenar)a 23 0.2 0.3 40 0.2 0.4 0.303
Mechanical Pain Threshold (knee)a 23 60.0 162.0 38 80.0 166.3 0.575
Mechanical Pain Threshold (thenar)a 23 100.0 120.0 38 100.0 120.0 0.925
Pain Pressure Threshold 23 12.5 5.3 40 11.7 6.4 0.584
CPMa 23 2.8 67.0 40 −0.3 2.1 0.00b
VAMS Depressiona 23 1.0 3.0 40 0.0 2.0 0.669
VAMS Anxietya 23 1.0 3.0 40 2.0 4.0 0.341
VAMS Sleepinessa 23 1.3 2.9 40 1.0 3.0 0.813
VAMS Stressa 23 2.0 4.0 40 1.0 3.0 0.199
VAS Now 23 4.4 2.2 40 4.7 2.3 0.592
Womac Total 23 41.7 19.4 40 46.3 16.5 0.350
  1. aCentral tendency presented as median and variability as interquartile range; CPM = Continuous Pain Modulation; VAS = Visual Analog Scale.

  2. b p<0.001.

The analysis did not show any significant difference between the two subgroups of normal CPM response versus limited CPM response, except for the CPM variable (p<0.05). CPM was statistically significantly different between the two subgroups (median=2.8 and median=−.3 for the subgroup with normal CPM response and limited CPM response, respectively), U=920, z=6.567, p<0.001. This significant difference supports that our subgroups are indeed distinct on their CPM function.

Two different multiple linear regression models were generated, one for the KOA subjects with normal CPM Response (model 1) and one for KOA subjects with “limited CPM response (model 2). No outliers were identified. Visual analysis of the scatterplots as well as the residuals confirmed the assumption of linearity and homoscedasticity for all models. For multicollinearity, analysis of the correlation matrix between the predictors showed no values above 0.7 (strong correlation). Also, analysis of the variance inflation factor and tolerance values obtained from the collinearity diagnosis output showed no values >10 or <0.2, respectively, assuring the assumption of no multicollinearity. For influential cases, we found no Cook’s distance values >1 and no DFBeta values >|1|, indicating no influential cases. The Durbin Watson statistic showed no value <1 or >3 for all 2 models, indicating that the assumption of independent errors was met for all 2 models.

The final regression equation for the subgroup of KOA subjects with normal CPM response (model 1) was: VASPain=7.247 – 0.046*SF36 General Health. The multiple regression model statistically significantly predicted VASPain, F(1, 21)=5.60, p<0.05, R-squared=0.2104. General Health measured by the SF36 General Health subscale was significantly associated to VASPain scores. Model coefficients suggested that as SF36 General Health score decreased (poorer general health), VAS Pain increased. The respective Cohen’s ƒ2 effect size was 0.27 (medium) [52]. Regression coefficients and standard errors are detailed in Table 3.

Table 3:

Model 1 – “normal CPM response” group.

Normal CPM response
Variable Model 1
B SE p-Value
Intercept 7.247 1.287 0.000a
SF36 General Health −0.046 0.019 0.028b
R-squared 0.2104
F 5.60
  1. Multivariate linear regression for VAS pain scores.

  2. n=23 ap<0.001, bp<0.05.

  3. B=unstandardized coefficient.

For KOA subjects with limited CPM response (model 2), the final regression equation was: VASPain=5.441 – (0.019*SF36 Role Limitation due to Emotional Problems)+(0.330* Stress VAMS). The multiple regression model statistically significantly predicted VASPain, F(2, 37)=7.02, p<0.01, R-squared=0.275. The SF36 Role Limitation due to Emotional Problems as well as the perception of stress at the moment of assessment measured by the Stress VAMS was significantly associated to VASPain scores. The direction of the coefficients suggested that as limitations due to emotional problems scores diminishes (meaning more disability), VASPain increases; and as Stress VAMS score increased, VASPain also increased. The respective Cohen’s ƒ2 effect size was 0.37 (large) [52]. Regression coefficients and standard errors are detailed in Table 4.

Table 4:

Model 2 – “limited CPM response” group.

Limited CPM response
Variable Model 2
B SE p-Value
Intercept 5.441 0.884 0.042a
SF36 Role Limit. due to Emotional Problems −0.019 0.008 0.029a
Stress VAMS 0.330 0.157 0.000b
R-squared 0.2751
F 7.02
  1. Multivariate linear regression for VAS pain scores.

  2. Dependent variable: VAS Pain.

  3. n=40 ap<0.05, bp<0.01.

  4. B=unstandardized coefficient.

  5. Stress VAMS = Visual Analog Mood Scale for Stress.

6 Discussion

The aim of this study was to explore the association of clinical variables and pain perception in KOA individuals with varying levels of CPM function. We have found that general health perception is associated with pain perception in KOA subjects with normal CPM response, while perception of limitations at work/other activities due to emotional problems and self-reported stress is associated with pain perception in those with limited CPM response. We believe this innovative approach can contribute to unveil the factors contributing to pain in KOA subjects and ultimately help guide treatment strategies.

The model for KOA subjects with normal CPM response (model 1) included only the SF36 General Health variable and was able to explain 21% of the variability in pain in that subgroup. There was a significant association between the model and the VASPain. This result supports the idea that in a functional endogenous pain inhibitory control system, pain perception is associated with general health perception. This association is not surprising as it is largely accepted that pain is a determinant of quality of life and general health due to its effects on mental health, well-being and activities of daily living [53], [54], [55]. Other [43] research has shown that general health status is explained partially by aspects of pain, for example, pain catastrophizing. Using a large community sample, this study showed that older people with chronic pain who report higher levels of pain tended to rate their general health status more negatively. This reinforces recent evidence that suggest a greater multidimensional negative impact of chronic pain on quality of life as compared to general population [56].

In model 2, the SF36 “Role Limitation Due to Emotional Problems” subscale variable was significant in the model. To thoroughly understand what this variable represents in our model, we must consider how it is determined. It is a combined result of three questions inside the SF36 questionnaire that asks if, over the past 4 weeks, the subject has spent less time at work or performing other activities, accomplished less than they would like and didn’t do work/other activities as carefully as usual due to any emotional problems. As these three questions specifically ask about the past 4 weeks, this variable likely represents the individual’s function prior to the pain variable being collected. Our results suggest that on those subjects who have limited CPM response, the perception of limitations at work/other activities due to emotional problems in the past month can significantly explain a portion of the variability in pain perception. The direction of this association reflects that the worst the subjects’ perception of how limited their work/other activities were in the past month because of emotional problems, the higher the pain. Although our results are based on analysis of cross-sectional data and no causality conclusions can be made, one may suggest this model to perhaps be considered as a potential prediction model due to how the questions were asked referring to the past 4 weeks. All the other collected variables such as Stress VAMS, Depression VAMS, Beck Depression Index, WOMAC, did not significantly add to the model.

In the group with limited CPM response (model 2), the final model also included Stress VAMS. In this model, higher stress perception was correlated with higher levels of pain perception. The stress variable was collected by asking the subject to rate their stress level at the moment of the assessment, which would reflect the subject’s current or acute level of stress. Our findings contradicts the concept of stress-induced analgesia [57], [58], [59]. We attribute this disagreement to the fact that, although our Stress VAMS variable is collected by specifically asking subjects to rate their stress at the moment of evaluation using a visual analog scale, it is possible that subjects rated instead their overall stress. Thus, rather than acute stress, our data might reflect chronic stress which has been shown to increase pain sensitivity in both animal and clinical research [60], [61], [62], [63], [64]. Evidence that stress increases pain sensitivity has been found in several studies, and the reasons for suspecting such a relationship are well-founded theoretically [65]. Stress have also been found to be highly prevalent in subject with severe KOA who are waiting for knee replacement [66]. It has also been reported that therapies that focus on cognitive byproducts of stress reduce pain, which provides further evidence that these psychological factors play an important role in chronic pain [67].

The comparison of model 1 and 2 suggests that differently that in subjects with normal CPM response, in subjects with limited CPM response, the perception of limitations at work/other activities due to emotional problems and the stress felt at the moment of the assessment are relevant factors that may explain differences in pain levels. Determining if the impact of emotional problems on work/other activities is a significant predictor of pain for KOA subjects with normal CPM response but not for subjects with limited CPM response may have direct implications for clinical practice. Although this result may suggest that interventions targeting emotional problems and its impact on social/work activities may contribute to address pain perception on KOA subjects with a well-functioning pain modulatory system, we cannot infer that the same type of intervention would not affect pain perception on those with a limited-functioning pain modulatory system. On the other hand, general health perception was found to be significantly associated with pain level in subjects with normal CPM response but not in those with limited CPM response. This finding may suggest that general health perception may not have as much impact on explaining pain levels on subjects with limited CPM response compared to those with normal CPM response and perhaps should not be emphasized as an intervention in this subgroup of KOA subjects. To confirm the different characteristics of these subgroups of KOA patients may be relevant to better determine treatment plans and achieve better clinical results.

Previous research [29] has explored the relationship between clinical pain and CPM measures in healthy subjects; specifically, hierarchical regression models have shown that CPM response has the greatest impact on pain response, with greater CPM responses related to less pain. Our analysis did not consider CPM as an independent variable, but instead, used CPM response to identify distinct groups based on their CPM function. However, the two regression models developed in this study suggested that pain is associated differently according to the individual CPM response. Edwards et al. [29] found that younger participants showed adequate CPM response (suppression of pain rating with conditioning stimulus) more commonly than older participants. Our results are consistent with Edwards et al.’s results as, although not significant, age was found to be different between our subgroups of KOA subjects with the subgroup with normal CPM response being slightly younger that with limited CPM response. We tested is “age” was a confounding factor in our sample and found that it did not contribute significantly to VASPain scores in any of the separate models.

Our study has limitations. First, our data for pain and stress were collected using visual analog scales. Some may argue the subjectiveness of these types of scales and its limitations [68]. However, its vast use in research and clinically allow easy interpretation and direct comparison with related research. Second, our small sample size hampers the generalizability of our results. Although our sample has characteristics that are similar to those of other reported OA samples, it has a similar distribution between men and women, where KOA is typically predominant in women [69]. Thirdly, we have dichotomized the level of endogenous pain inhibitory control as indexed by CPM function and information on individual differences may have been lost in the dichotomization process causing other subgroups with dysfunctional CPM levels to not be detected. Previous research has discussed the impact of the dichotomization of quantitave measurements [70], [71]. Ultimately, this study has an exploratory nature and it is a secondary analysis of a clinical trial. Our analysis used only cross-sectional data and, although we discussed the possibility of some of the models to be seen as prediction models, causation inferences are not appropriate. All inferences about any results should consider these limitations and be interpreted accordingly.

7 Conclusions

Our study suggests that pain perception may be associated with different factors in KOA subjects that have different levels CPM function. This information may help understand the factors contributing to pain in KOA and assist clinical practice. The exploratory nature of our analysis implies that our inferences should be considered only with an understanding of the limitations of our methodology. Future research should explore other clinical variables to optimize the best predictor/association model for pain in KOA subjects with different levels of CPM function. We also encourage researchers to explore treatment strategies focused on subgroups of KOA subjects based on their different levels of endogenous pain inhibitory control.

  1. Research funding: Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Center for Complementary & Integrative Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R44AT008637. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

  2. Conflict of interest: All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

  3. Informed consent: Written informed consent was obtained for all participants.

  4. Ethical approval: All study procedures were approved by the Partner’s Institutional Review Board at the Partners Human Research Committee (protocol approval number: 2014P002496). In addition, this research complies with all the relevant national regulations, institutional policies and was performed in accordance with the tenets of the Helsinki Declaration.

  5. Author contributions:The authors PEPT, HIZ and SC contributed equally to this work in regards to intellectual and technical assistance, data collection, analysis, writing and editing assistance. Author FF has contributed with intellectual and editing assistance and author LD with analysis and editing assistance. All named authors meet the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE) criteria for authorship for this article, take responsibility for the integrity of the work as a whole, and have given their approval for this version to be published.

References

1. Martel-Pelletier J, Barr AJ, Cicuttini FM, Conaghan PG, Cooper C, Goldring MB, Goldring SR, Jones G, Teichtahl AJ, Pelletier JP. Osteoarthritis. Nat Rev Dis Primer 2016;2:16072.10.1038/nrdp.2016.72Search in Google Scholar PubMed

2. Fernandes L, Hagen KB, Bijlsma JWJ, Andreassen O, Christensen P, Conaghan PG, Doherty M, Geenen R, Hammond A, Kjeken I, Lohmander LS, Lund H, Mallen CD, Nava T, Oliver S, Pavelka K, Pitsillidou I, da Silva JA, de la Torre J, Zanoli G, et al. EULAR recommendations for the non-pharmacological core management of hip and knee osteoarthritis. Ann Rheum Dis 2013;72:1125–35.10.1136/annrheumdis-2012-202745Search in Google Scholar PubMed

3. James SL, Abate D, Abate KH, Abay SM, Abbafati C, Abbasi N, Abbastabar H, Abd-Allah F, Abdela J, Abdelalim A, Abdollahpour I, Abdulkader RS, Abebe Z, Abera SF, Abil OZ, Abraha HN, Abu-Raddad LJ, Abu-Rmeileh NME, Accrombessi MMK, Acharya D, et al. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet 2018;392:1789–58.10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32279-7Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

4. Cross M, Smith E, Hoy D, Nolte S, Ackerman I, Fransen M, Bridgett L, Williams S, Guillemin F, Hill CL, Laslett LL, Jones G, Cicuttini F, Osborne R, Vos T, Buchbinder R, Woolf A, March L. The global burden of hip and knee osteoarthritis: estimates from the Global Burden of Disease 2010 study. Ann Rheum Dis 2014;73:1323–30.10.1136/annrheumdis-2013-204763Search in Google Scholar PubMed

5. Lawrence RC, Felson DT, Helmick CG, Arnold LM, Choi H, Deyo RA, Gabriel S, Hirsch R, Hochberg MC, Hunder GG, Jordan JM, Katz JN, Kremers HM, Wolfe F, National Arthritis Data Workgroup. Estimates of the prevalence of arthritis and other rheumatic conditions in the United States: part II. Arthritis Rheum 2008;58:26–35.10.1002/art.23176Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

6. Pereira D, Peleteiro B, Araújo J, Branco J, Santos RA, Ramos E. The effect of osteoarthritis definition on prevalence and incidence estimates: a systematic review. Osteoarthritis Cartilage 2011;19:1270–85.10.1016/j.joca.2011.08.009Search in Google Scholar PubMed

7. Losina E, Paltiel AD, Weinstein AM, Yelin E, Hunter DJ, Chen SP, Klara K, Suter LG, Solomon DH, Burbine SA, Walensky RP, Katz JN. Lifetime medical costs of knee osteoarthritis management in the United States: impact of extending indications for total knee arthroplasty. Arthritis Care Res 2015;67:203–15.10.1002/acr.22412Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

8. Rannou F. Pathophysiology of osteoarthritis. In: Arden N, Blanco F, Cooper C, Guermazi A, Hayashi D, Hunter D, Javaid MK, Rannou F, Roemer FW, Reginster J-Y, editors. Atlas of Osteoarthritis. Tarporley: Springer Healthcare Ltd., 2014:37–54.Search in Google Scholar

9. Felson DT. The sources of pain in knee osteoarthritis. Curr Opin Rheumatol 2005;17:624–8.10.1097/01.bor.0000172800.49120.97Search in Google Scholar PubMed

10. Szebenyi B, Hollander AP, Dieppe P, Quilty B, Duddy J, Clarke S, Kirwan JR. Associations between pain, function, and radiographic features in osteoarthritis of the knee. Arthritis Rheum 2006;54:230–5.10.1002/art.21534Search in Google Scholar PubMed

11. Fu K, Robbins SR, McDougall JJ. Osteoarthritis: the genesis of pain. Rheumatology 2018;57(suppl_4):iv43–50.10.1093/rheumatology/kex419Search in Google Scholar PubMed

12. Mease PJ, Hanna S, Frakes EP, Altman RD. Pain mechanisms in osteoarthritis: understanding the role of central pain and current approaches to its treatment. J Rheumatol 2011;38:1546–51.10.3899/jrheum.100759Search in Google Scholar PubMed

13. Liu-Bryan R, Terkeltaub R. Emerging regulators of the inflammatory process in osteoarthritis. Nat Rev Rheumatol 2015;11:35–44.10.1038/nrrheum.2014.162Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

14. Parks EL, Geha PY, Baliki MN, Katz J, Schnitzer TJ, Apkarian AV. Brain activity for chronic knee osteoarthritis: dissociating evoked pain from spontaneous pain. Eur J Pain Lond Engl 2011;15:843.e1–843.e14.10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.12.007Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

15. Lluch E, Torres R, Nijs J, Van Oosterwijck J. Evidence for central sensitization in patients with osteoarthritis pain: a systematic literature review. Eur J Pain Lond Engl 2014;18:1367–75.10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.499.xSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

16. Arendt-Nielsen L. Joint pain: more to it than just structural damage? Pain 2017;158:S66–73.10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000812Search in Google Scholar PubMed

17. Staud R. Evidence for shared pain mechanisms in osteoarthritis, low back pain, and fibromyalgia. Curr Rheumatol Rep 2011;13:513–20.10.1007/s11926-011-0206-6Search in Google Scholar PubMed

18. Fingleton C, Smart K, Moloney N, Fullen BM, Doody C. Pain sensitization in people with knee osteoarthritis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Osteoarthr Cartilage 2015;23: 1043–56.10.1016/j.joca.2015.02.163Search in Google Scholar PubMed

19. Loeser JD, Treede R-D. The Kyoto protocol of IASP basic pain terminology. Pain 2008;137:473–7.10.1016/j.pain.2008.04.025Search in Google Scholar PubMed

20. King CD, Sibille KT, Goodin BR, Cruz-Almeida Y, Glover TL, Bartley E, Riley JL, Herbert MS, Sotolongo A, Schmidt J, Fessler BJ, Redden DT, Staud R, Bradley LA, Fillingim RB. Experimental pain sensitivity differs as a function of clinical pain severity in symptomatic knee osteoarthritis. Osteoarthritis Cartilage 2013;21:1243–52.10.1016/j.joca.2013.05.015Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

21. Petersen KK, Simonsen O, Olesen AE, Mørch CD, Arendt-Nielsen L. Pain inhibitory mechanisms and response to weak analgesics in patients with knee osteoarthritis. Eur J Pain 2019;23:1904–12.10.1002/ejp.1465Search in Google Scholar PubMed

22. Youssef AM, Macefield VG, Henderson LA. Pain inhibits pain; human brainstem mechanisms. NeuroImage 2016;124:54–62.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.060Search in Google Scholar PubMed

23. Knardahl S. Conditioned pain modulation: a robust phenomenon? Scand J Pain 2011;2:161.10.1016/j.sjpain.2011.08.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed

24. Bannister K, Dickenson AH. The plasticity of descending controls in pain: translational probing. J Physiol 2017;595: 4159–66.10.1113/JP274165Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

25. Yarnitsky D. Conditioned pain modulation (the diffuse noxious inhibitory control-like effect): its relevance for acute and chronic pain states. Curr Opin Anesthesiol 2010;23:611–5.10.1097/ACO.0b013e32833c348bSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

26. Lindstedt F, Berrebi J, Greayer E, Lonsdorf TB, Schalling M, Ingvar M, Kosek E. Conditioned pain modulation is associated with common polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter gene. PLoS One 2011;6:e18252.10.1371/journal.pone.0018252Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

27. Popescu A, LeResche L, Truelove E, Drangsholt M. Gender differences in pain modulation by diffuse noxious inhibitory controls: a systematic review. Pain 2010;150:309–18.10.1016/j.pain.2010.05.013Search in Google Scholar PubMed

28. Ossipov MH, Dussor GO, Porreca F. Central modulation of pain. J Clin Invest 2010;120:3779–87.10.1172/JCI43766Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

29. Edwards RR, Ness TJ, Weigent DA, Fillingim RB. Individual differences in diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC): association with clinical variables. Pain 2003;106:427–37.10.1016/j.pain.2003.09.005Search in Google Scholar PubMed

30. Lewis GN, Rice DA, McNair PJ. Conditioned pain modulation in populations with chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain 2012;13:936–44.10.1016/j.jpain.2012.07.005Search in Google Scholar PubMed

31. Edwards RR, Dolman AJ, Martel MO, Finan PH, Lazaridou A, Cornelius M, Wasan AD. Variability in conditioned pain modulation predicts response to NSAID treatment in patients with knee osteoarthritis. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 2016;17:284.10.1186/s12891-016-1124-6Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

32. Campbell CM, Buenaver LF, Raja SN, Kiley KB, Swedberg LJ, Wacnik PW, Cohen SP, Erdek MA, Williams KA, Christo PJ. Dynamic pain phenotypes are associated with spinal cord stimulation-induced reduction in pain: a repeated measures observational pilot study: SCS and dynamic pain phenotyping. Pain Med 2015;16:1349–60.10.1111/pme.12732Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

33. Turk DC, Dworkin RH, Allen RR, Bellamy N, Brandenburg N, Carr DB, Cleeland C, Dionne R, Farrar JT, Galer BS, Hewitt DJ, Jadad AR, Katz NP, Kramer LD, Manning DC, McCormick CG, McDermott MP, McGrath P, Quessy S, Rappaport BA, et al. Core outcome domains for chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain 2003;106:337–45.10.1016/j.pain.2003.08.001Search in Google Scholar PubMed

34. Dworkin RH, Turk DC, Farrar JT, Haythornthwaite JA, Jensen MP, Katz NP, Kerns RD, Stucki G, Allen RR, Bellamy N, Carr DB, Chandler J, Cowan P, Dionne R, Galer BS, Hertz S, Jadad AR, Kramer LD, Manning DC, Martin S, et al. Core outcome measures for chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain 2005;113:9–19.10.1016/j.pain.2004.09.012Search in Google Scholar PubMed

35. Jensen MP, McFarland CA. Increasing the reliability and validity of pain intensity measurement in chronic pain patients. Pain 1993;55:195–203.10.1016/0304-3959(93)90148-ISearch in Google Scholar PubMed

36. Michener LA, Snyder AR, Leggin BG. Responsiveness of the numeric pain rating scale in patients with shoulder pain and the effect of surgical status. J Sport Rehabil 2011;20:115–28.10.1123/jsr.20.1.115Search in Google Scholar PubMed

37. Ahearn EP. The use of visual analog scales in mood disorders: a critical review. J Psychiatr Res 1997;31:569–79.10.1016/S0022-3956(97)00029-0Search in Google Scholar

38. Cella DF, Perry SW. Reliability and concurrent validity of three visual-analogue mood scales. Psychol Rep 1986;59:827–33.10.2466/pr0.1986.59.2.827Search in Google Scholar PubMed

39. Walk D, Sehgal N, Moeller-Bertram T, Edwards RR, Wasan A, Wallace M, Irving G, Argoff C, Backonja MM. Quantitative sensory testing and mapping: a review of nonautomated quantitative methods for examination of the patient with neuropathic pain. Clin J Pain 2009;25:632–40.10.1097/AJP.0b013e3181a68c64Search in Google Scholar PubMed

40. Keizer D, van Wijhe M, Post WJ, Wierda JMKH. Quantifying allodynia in patients suffering from unilateral neuropathic pain using von frey monofilaments. Clin J Pain 2007;23:85–90.10.1097/01.ajp.0000210950.01503.72Search in Google Scholar PubMed

41. Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tölle TR, Treede RD, Beyer A, Binder A, Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Bötefür IC, Braune S, Flor H, Huge V, Klug R, Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Maihöfner C, Rolko C, Schaub C, Scherens A, et al. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain 2006;123:231–43.10.1016/j.pain.2006.01.041Search in Google Scholar PubMed

42. Tavares DRB, Okazaki JEF, Rocha AP, Santana MVA, Pinto ACPN, Civile VT, Santos FC, Fregni F, Trevisani VFM. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on knee osteoarthritis pain in elderly subjects with defective endogenous pain-inhibitory systems: protocol for a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Res Protoc 2018;7:e11660.10.2196/11660Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

43. Lewis GN, Heales L, Rice DA, Rome K, McNair PJ. Reliability of the conditioned pain modulation paradigm to assess endogenous inhibitory pain pathways. Pain Res Manag 2012;17:98–102.10.1155/2012/610561Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

44. Warmenhoven F, van Rijswijk E, Engels Y, Kan C, Prins J, van Weel C, Vissers K. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) and a single screening question as screening tools for depressive disorder in Dutch advanced cancer patients. Support Care Cancer Off J Multinatl Assoc Support Care Cancer 2012;20:319–24.10.1007/s00520-010-1082-8Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

45. Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care 1992;30:473–83.10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002Search in Google Scholar

46. Basaran S, Guzel R, Seydaoglu G, Guler-Uysal F. Validity, reliability, and comparison of the WOMAC osteoarthritis index and Lequesne algofunctional index in Turkish patients with hip or knee osteoarthritis. Clin Rheumatol 2010;29:749–56.10.1007/s10067-010-1398-2Search in Google Scholar PubMed

47. Bruce B, Fries J. Longitudinal comparison of the Health Assessment Questionnaire (HAQ) and the Western Ontario and McMaster Universities Osteoarthritis Index (WOMAC). Arthritis Care Res 2004;51:730–7.10.1002/art.20695Search in Google Scholar PubMed

48. Yap BW, Sim CH. Comparisons of various types of normality tests. J Stat Comput Simul 2011;81:2141–55.10.1080/00949655.2010.520163Search in Google Scholar

49. Marcuzzi A, Wrigley P, Dean C, Adams R, Hush J. The long-term reliability of static and dynamic quantitative sensory testing in healthy individuals. Pain 2017;158:1217–23.10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000901Search in Google Scholar PubMed

50. Kennedy DL, Kemp HI, Wu C, Ridout DA, Rice ASC. Determining real change in conditioned pain modulation: a repeated measures study in healthy volunteers. J Pain 2019;S1526–5900: 30847–8 [ahead of print].Search in Google Scholar

51. Osborne JW, Waters E. Four assumptions of multiple regression that researchers should always test. Pract Assess Res Eval 2002;8:2.Search in Google Scholar

52. Selya AS, Rose JS, Dierker LC, Hedeker D, Mermelstein RJ. A practical guide to calculating Cohen’s f2, a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED. Front Psychol 2012;3:111.10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

53. Katz N. The impact of pain management on quality of life. J Pain Symptom Manage 2002;24:S38–47.10.1016/S0885-3924(02)00411-6Search in Google Scholar

54. Azizabadi Farahani M, Assari S. Relationship between pain and quality of life. In: Preedy VR, Watson RR, eds. Handbook of Disease Burdens and Quality of Life Measures. New York, NY: Springer, 2010:3933–53.10.1007/978-0-387-78665-0_229Search in Google Scholar

55. Wranker LS, Rennemark M, Berglund J, Elmståhl S. Relationship between pain and Quality of Life – Findings from the Swedish National Study on Aging and Care – Blekinge study. Scand J Pain 2014;5:270–5.10.1016/j.sjpain.2014.05.029Search in Google Scholar PubMed

56. Hadi MA, McHugh GA, Closs SJ. Impact of chronic pain on patients’ quality of life: a comparative mixed-methods study. J Patient Exp 2019;6:133–41.10.1177/2374373518786013Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

57. Butler RK, Finn DP. Stress-induced analgesia. Prog Neurobiol 2009;88:184–202.10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.04.003Search in Google Scholar PubMed

58. Ferdousi M, Finn DP. Chapter 4 – Stress-induced modulation of pain: role of the endogenous opioid system. Prog Brain Res 2018;239:121–77.10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.07.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed

59. Tobaldini G, Andersen EOL, Polato JJ, Guilhen VA, Gaspar JC, Lazzarim MK, Sardi NF, Fischer L. Pain and stress: functional evidence that supra-spinal mechanisms involved in pain-induced analgesia mediate stress-induced analgesia. Behav Pharmacol 2020;31:159–67.10.1097/FBP.0000000000000529Search in Google Scholar PubMed

60. Costa A, Smeraldi A, Tassorelli C, Greco R, Nappi G. Effects of acute and chronic restraint stress on nitroglycerin-induced hyperalgesia in rats. Neurosci Lett 2005;383:7–11.10.1016/j.neulet.2005.03.026Search in Google Scholar PubMed

61. Scheich B, Vincze P, Szőke É, Borbély É, Hunyady Á, Szolcsányi J, Dénes Á, Környei Z, Gaszner B, Helyes Z. Chronic stress-induced mechanical hyperalgesia is controlled by capsaicin-sensitive neurones in the mouse. Eur J Pain Lond Engl 2017;21:1417–31.10.1002/ejp.1043Search in Google Scholar PubMed

62. Liu L-Y, Zhang R-L, Chen L, Zhao HY, Cai J, Wang JK, Guo DQ, Cui YJ, Xing GG. Chronic stress increases pain sensitivity via activation of the rACC-BLA pathway in rats. Exp Neurol 2019;313:109–23.10.1016/j.expneurol.2018.12.009Search in Google Scholar PubMed

63. Welte-Jzyk C, Pfau DB, Hartmann A, Daubländer M. Somatosensory profiles of patients with chronic myogenic temporomandibular disorders in relation to their painDETECT score. BMC Oral Health 2018;18:138.10.1186/s12903-018-0601-8Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

64. Egle UT, Egloff N, von Känel R. [Stress-induced hyperalgesia (SIH) as a consequence of emotional deprivation and psychosocial traumatization in childhood: implications for the treatment of chronic pain]. Schmerz Berl Ger 2016;30:526–36.10.1007/s00482-016-0107-8Search in Google Scholar PubMed

65. Jennings EM, Okine BN, Roche M, Finn DP. Stress-induced hyperalgesia. Prog Neurobiol 2014;121:1–18.10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.06.003Search in Google Scholar PubMed

66. Khatib Y, Jenkin D, Naylor JM, Harris IA. Psychological traits in patients waiting for total knee arthroplasty. A cross-sectional study. J Arthroplasty 2016;31:1661–6.10.1016/j.arth.2016.01.053Search in Google Scholar PubMed

67. Lee AC, Harvey WF, Price LL, Morgan LPK, Morgan NL, Wang C. Mindfulness is associated with psychological health and moderates pain in knee osteoarthritis. Osteoarthr Cartilage 2017;25:824–31.10.1016/j.joca.2016.06.017Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

68. Streiner DL, Norman GR, Cairney J. Health measurement scales: a practical guide to their development and use (5th edition). Oxford, UK: Oxford Unversity Press, 2016.10.1093/med/9780199685219.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

69. Plotnikoff R, Karunamuni N, Lytvyak E, Penfold C, Schopflocher D, Imayama I, Johnson ST, Raine K. Osteoarthritis prevalence and modifiable factors: a population study. BMC Public Health 2015;15:1195.10.1186/s12889-015-2529-0Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

70. MacCallum RC, Zhang S, Preacher KJ, Rucker DD. On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychol Methods 2002;7:19–40.10.1037//1082-989X.7.1.19Search in Google Scholar

71. DeCoster J, Iselin A-MR, Gallucci M. A conceptual and empirical examination of justifications for dichotomization. Psychol Methods 2009;14:349–66.10.1037/a0016956Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Received: 2020-01-17
Revised: 2020-04-21
Accepted: 2020-04-27
Published Online: 2020-07-16
Published in Print: 2020-10-25

©2020 Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston. All rights reserved.

Downloaded on 10.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/sjpain-2020-0016/html
Scroll to top button