Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Can eating pleasure be a lever for healthy eating? A systematic scoping review of eating pleasure and its links with dietary behaviors and health

  • Alexandra Bédard,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre Nutrition, santé et société (NUTRISS), Institute of Nutrition and Functional Foods, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada

  • Pierre-Olivier Lamarche,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Sociology, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada

  • Lucie-Maude Grégoire,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre Nutrition, santé et société (NUTRISS), Institute of Nutrition and Functional Foods, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada, School of Nutrition, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada

  • Catherine Trudel-Guy,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre Nutrition, santé et société (NUTRISS), Institute of Nutrition and Functional Foods, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada, School of Nutrition, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada

  • Véronique Provencher,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre Nutrition, santé et société (NUTRISS), Institute of Nutrition and Functional Foods, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada, School of Nutrition, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada

  • Sophie Desroches,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Centre Nutrition, santé et société (NUTRISS), Institute of Nutrition and Functional Foods, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada, School of Nutrition, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada

  • Simone Lemieux

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Simone.Lemieux@fsaa.ulaval.ca

    Affiliations Centre Nutrition, santé et société (NUTRISS), Institute of Nutrition and Functional Foods, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada, School of Nutrition, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada

Abstract

The aims of this review were to map and summarize data currently available about 1) key dimensions of eating pleasure; 2) associations of eating pleasure, and its key dimensions, with dietary and health outcomes and 3) the most promising intervention strategies using eating pleasure to promote healthy eating. Using the scoping review methodology, a comprehensive search of the peer-reviewed literature (Medline, PsycInfo, Embase, ERIC, Web of Science, CINAHL, ABI/Inform global and Sociology Abstract) and of the grey literature (ProQuest Dissertations & Theses and Google) was carried out by two independent reviewers. We included 119 of the 28,908 studies found. In total, 89 sub-dimensions of eating pleasure were grouped into 22 key dimensions. The most frequently found related to sensory experiences (in 50.9% of the documents), social experiences (42.7%), food characteristics besides sensory attributes (27.3%), food preparation process (19.1%), novelty (16.4%), variety (14.5%), mindful eating (13.6%), visceral eating (12.7%), place where food is consumed (11.8%) and memories associated with eating (10.9%). Forty-five studies, mostly cross-sectional (62.2%), have documented links between eating pleasure and dietary and/or health outcomes. Most studies (57.1%) reported favorable associations between eating pleasure and dietary outcomes. For health outcomes, results were less consistent. The links between eating pleasure and both dietary and health outcomes varied according to the dimensions of eating pleasure studied. Finally, results from 11 independent interventions suggested that strategies focusing on sensory experiences, cooking and/or sharing activities, mindful eating, and positive memories related to healthy food may be most promising. Thus, eating pleasure may be an ally in the promotion of healthy eating. However, systematically developed, evidence-based interventions are needed to better understand how eating pleasure may be a lever for healthy eating.

Introduction

Diet quality has been identified as a major determinant of health. However, in developed countries, many individuals still not consume enough fruits, vegetables and wholes grains, while consuming too much processed foods high in energy, sodium, free sugars and saturated fats [1,2]. Despite numerous public health campaigns aimed at improving dietary habits, recent data suggest that diet quality has remained suboptimal over years [3]. These observations are challenging for policymakers and health professionals, underlying the necessity to improve food policies and interventions, and to identify new approaches to promote healthy eating that may have a greater impact on diet quality over time. These efforts could lead to considerable health benefits, along with reducing disease-related costs [4].

Some authors have proposed that the strong focus on the healthiness of food that is typically used to encourage individuals to change their food habits could explain, at least partly, the limited success of strategies used to improve diet quality [58]. Indeed, it is likely that the uneven importance attributed to the health value of food compared with other food values (e.g., related to preferences and human experiences) can nurture the beliefs that the healthiness of food is incompatible with these other food attributes [9]. A good example of this is the well-documented unhealthy equal tasty association. Some studies have shown that food presented as being healthy is often considered as being less tasty than “unhealthy” food [1015]. This perception seems to be influenced by the culture. Previous research observed that, for Americans, unhealthy food is implicitly associated to tastiness [15]. However, the opposite intuition has been observed in France, French people spontaneously associating unhealthy food with bad taste, while linking healthy food to tastiness [16]. Considering that taste is a predominant determinant of food choices [17,18], the unhealthy equal tasty intuition can clearly thwart the adoption of healthy food choices.

A growing number of authors have suggested that pleasure should be emphasized more in the promotion of healthy eating [68,1921]. This is even reflected in the latest dietary guidelines in some countries, including Canada [22] and Brazil [23], but also in intervention strategies using the “intuitive eating” approach (i.e., an approach that implies eating in response to hunger and satiety cues and rediscovering the pleasures of eating) [24]. This new paradigm is supported by a growing body of research that has reported, using different study designs, favorable associations between eating pleasure and dietary and health outcomes. For example, various cross-sectional studies have linked higher eating pleasure to better nutritional status [25], healthier food choices [2628], increased subjective diet-related quality of life [29] and decreased depressive symptoms [30]. Other studies of both short-term and long-term interventions using eating pleasure also observed promising results. For example, Petit et al. [7] found that focusing on the healthiness of food decreased activity in the gustatory and reward brain areas, whereas focusing on the tastiness of food increased brain activity in self-control, gustatory and reward areas, and most importantly increased the percentage of healthy food choices. Overall, the authors concluded that promoting the tastiness of healthy food may lead to more successful self-control and healthy dietary behaviors. In a randomized controlled trial conducted by our research group [31], a sensory-based intervention addressing the topics of relationship with food, reliance on hunger and satiety cues, food tasting and eating pleasure showed that women who were allocated to the intervention decreased their level of disinhibition in comparison to the control group. Sasson et al. [32] did an experiment with US students who explored food and nutrition from historical, cultural and culinary perspectives during a study abroad program in Italy. Six months after the end of the program, it was found that students had increased their use of fresh and locally grown foods, improved their cooking skills and were less worried about calories [32]. Authors suggested that focusing on eating pleasure can nurture lifelong healthy eating habits. However, other studies have observed a negative link between eating pleasure and diet quality [27,33,34] or body-mass index (BMI) [35,36]. In some cross-sectional studies, eating pleasure has been associated with the consumption of convenience food [33], late-night snaking [34], and has been identified as a factor motivating the consumption of sweets [13]. Moreover, results from Cornil & Chandon [37] showed that focusing on sensory pleasure made sated eaters and dieters choose larger portions of hedonic foods, while the opposite was observed for hungry eaters and nondieters. Eating pleasure has also been reported as one of the most important reason for weight gain over years in a sample of women and men living with obesity [35].

The use of different approaches to define eating pleasure may partly explain the inconsistency of these outcomes. Studies aimed at understanding overeating and self-regulation failures [21,3841] mostly describe proper weight management as incompatible with eating pleasure, which they define as visceral impulses triggered by external cues or emotional urges driven by biological preferences for high-sugar, high-fat and high-sodium food. This way of defining eating pleasure is associated with overeating, loss of control and poor diet quality [21,39]. However, other approaches to define eating pleasure have been proposed in the literature [21,4244]. For example, Cornil & Chandon [21] contrast short-term visceral pleasure with what they call epicurean eating pleasure, or “the enduring pleasure derived from the aesthetic appreciation of the sensory and symbolic value of food”. This definition includes not only food characteristics (i.e., taste and appearance) but also other dimensions of the eating experience such as social eating and cooking. They showed that, unlike visceral pleasure, those scoring high on epicurean pleasure preferred smaller portions and displayed a higher level of well-being [21]. Interestingly, epicurean eating pleasure displays similarities with the dimensions of eating pleasure identified in some qualitative studies [4244]. These studies underline the multidimensional nature of eating pleasure, suggesting it includes dimensions related to food characteristics, the individual, the environment and the social context. The wide variety of definitions of eating pleasure could thus lead to inconsistencies in assessments of the relationship between eating pleasure and healthy eating. Establishing a common understanding of eating pleasure therefore requires identifying its key dimensions and using these to analyze in more detail how eating pleasure is associated with dietary behavior and health outcomes.

Considering the increased interest within the scientific and clinical communities in the integration of eating pleasure in the promotion of healthy eating, a comprehensive mapping of current knowledge about this topic is essential to inform health professionals and policymakers whether and how the pleasure of eating can be a lever for healthy eating. This will also help to identify gaps that will guide next research efforts in this area. In this comprehensive scoping review we therefore aimed to (1) identify the key dimensions of eating pleasure; (2) map the data currently available about associations of eating pleasure, and its key dimensions, with dietary behaviors and health outcomes and (3) identify most promising intervention strategies using eating pleasure to promote healthy eating. Scoping review design represents a rigorous methodology that allows comprehensive assessment of emerging evidence, and a first step in research development [45]. This technique aims to map the key concepts, types of evidence available, and gaps in research related to a defined field by systematically searching, selecting, and synthesizing existing knowledge [46,47]. This is particularly relevant to disciplines with emerging evidence, since researchers can incorporate a range of study designs in both published and grey literature, and address questions beyond those related to intervention effectiveness [48]. Accordingly, a scoping review was deemed more appropriate than a systematic review or a meta-analysis in the present context because (1) the objectives address a broad, multidisciplinary topic, (2) this topic has been explored using a range of research designs and (3) this complex and emerging topic has not been reviewed comprehensively before, thereby preventing the identification of more specific objectives that can be the focus of a systematic review or a meta-analysis [49].

Methods

Based on the Arksey and O’Malley methodological framework for scoping reviews [47] and enhanced according to the recommendations of Levac et al. aimed at clarifying each stage of the framework [48], six steps were performed: (1) identifying the research question, (2) identifying relevant studies, (3) selecting eligible studies, (4) charting the data, (5) collating, summarizing, and reporting results and (6) consultation. The study protocol has been registered on Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/5p7b8/) [50].

Step 1: Identifying the research questions and eligibility criteria

Research questions.

The purpose of this review was to assess whether and how eating pleasure can be a lever for healthy dietary behaviors and health. Accordingly, three research questions were addressed:

  1. Q1: How is eating pleasure conceptualized (i.e., key dimensions) in scientific research and organization/government documents?
  2. Q2: What is the current available scientific evidence about associations of eating pleasure, and its key dimensions, with dietary behaviors and health outcomes?
  3. Q3: What are the most promising strategies using eating pleasure to promote healthy dietary behaviors identified through intervention studies?

In parallel, this review sought to identify gaps that research efforts should address to further explore the place of eating pleasure in the promotion of healthy eating, and to inform health professionals and policymakers about how to use eating pleasure in their practical strategies.

Our scoping review was conducted between February 2018 and August 2019. It was conceptualized (SL and AB) and reviewed by coauthors with expertise in scoping review methodology (VP and SD).

Population and context.

The population targeted was individuals of at least five years old, with no specific condition that would interfere with eating pleasure or alter the perception of eating pleasure. We decided not to include individuals under the age of five because we were interested in eating pleasure as experienced or reported by the individuals themselves, and children under five may have some trouble reporting explicit, conscious judgements due to their limited cognitive maturity, and especially language development [51]. As a result, eating pleasure is usually measured in infants and young children using observer-based measures or parent-rated measures [51,52]. Intervention strategies may also be very different at these ages, focusing more on play-based approaches and often related to neophobia and food familiarity [5358]. In addition, as we wanted to identify strategies that could be applied in interventions aiming to prevent nutrition-related chronic diseases (e.g., obesity, type 2 diabetes, cardiovascular diseases), we excluded documents targeting people with conditions requiring a strict diet or increased dietary intake. Finally, since issues related to healthy eating differ between underdeveloped and developed countries [59], only studies conducted in developed countries (i.e., countries with high and very high human development index according to the Human Development Report 2016 [60]) were considered for inclusion.

Concepts.

No a priori definition for eating pleasure was used since we aimed to conceptualize eating pleasure by identifying its key dimensions in this review. Scientific research referred to peer-reviewed literature and theses/dissertations. Given that this is an emerging area in the scientific literature a broad range of articles and study designs were considered, including but not limited to qualitative, cross-sectional, retrospective, prospective, intervention and mixed methods studies. Organization/government documents referred to guidelines, health-promotion tools, reports, and program descriptions published by a health-related organization (i.e., an organization whose ultimate mission is to promote and/or foster the health of individuals/populations) or government. Dietary behavior was defined as any actions associated with the act of eating. Health was defined according to the definition of the World Health Organization (“a state of complete physical, mental and social well-being and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity” [61]). A promising strategy was defined as an intervention method that led to a significant and beneficial impact on dietary behavior and/or health outcomes.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria.

In addition to responding to our research questions, documents considered for inclusion were: 1) mainly focused on eating pleasure dimensions or reporting data about the links between eating pleasure and dietary behavior and/or health outcomes; 2) targeting individuals of at least five years old; 3) written in English or in French (languages spoken by the research team); 4) conducted in developed countries and 5) interested in eating pleasure as experienced or reported by the individuals themselves. Excluded sources were those that 1) reported results in animals, 2) targeted individuals with a specific condition that would require a strict diet or an increased dietary intake (e.g., type 1 diabetes, malnutrition, decreased appetite, neophobia), 3) targeted people with a condition that may interfere with eating pleasure or alter the perception of eating pleasure (e.g., depression, eating disorder pathologies, pain), 4) reported research objectives aiming to evaluate eating pleasure in relation to a specific food/food group (except for vegetables and fruits, as they are considered as the cornerstone of a healthy diet [62]) and 5) described eating pleasure only in terms of the biological reward mechanisms (e.g., with brain imaging). Conference abstracts, proceedings of symposiums or conferences, editorials, expert opinions, blogs, books, book chapters, book reviews and literature reviews were also excluded.

Step 2: Identifying relevant studies

With the collaboration of a librarian specializing in nutrition, we developed a comprehensive search strategy using both specific keywords and controlled vocabulary (i.e., standardized terms used by database indexers to describe and categorize articles based on content) related to eating and pleasure. Using May 6, 2018, as a cut-off date, we searched eight electronic databases of peer-reviewed literature: Medline, PsycInfo, Embase, ERIC, Web of Science, CINAHL, ABI/Inform global and Sociology Abstract. This range of databases allowed us to capture a comprehensive sample from the many disciplines related to eating pleasure, namely medical sciences, nutrition, psychology, business, public health, education and sociology. Search terms were adapted for each database and combined using Boolean operators to narrow the results. No date restrictions were implemented in the search. As an example, the Medline search strategy is presented in Table 1. Strategies for other databases are available in S1 Table. The references of included studies were also scanned to identify other relevant results. To conceptualize eating pleasure in a more comprehensive way and to retrieve unpublished studies, we conducted additional searches in the grey literature using ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global (cut-off date: July 10, 2019) and the search engine “Google” (between July 15, 2019 and August 26, 2019). For Google, simplified search strategies in English and French (when relevant) were developed and validated by the specialized librarian for organization websites (site:.org) and for the government website of each country for which a relevant peer-reviewed article had been identified in peer-reviewed databases (total of 16 government websites; see S1 Table for search strategies). For Google search strategies, the government websites of the United States, France and Canada as well as organization websites were initially searched. For each of these, the first 100 hits were analyzed. Since the Google engine displays results by relative importance using a link analysis algorithm [63] and all relevant websites were in the first Google hits for these first Google searches, for the remaining governments, we decided that if at least one website was identified as relevant in the first group of 20, then the next 20 websites were searched, for up to a maximum of 100 per government search strategy. Finally, suggestions from authors of this scoping review were gathered to identify additional potentially relevant documents from the literature.

Step 3: Selecting eligible studies

All retrieved records were imported into EndNote software (Version X9), and duplicate records were deleted. First, two reviewers (POL and LMG) and the research assistant (AB) tested the selection process with 250 articles to ensure a shared understanding of inclusion and exclusion criteria. Thereafter, the two reviewers independently determined the eligibility of all articles using a two-stage screening process consisting of a title and abstract scan followed by a full-text review. If we were unable to obtain the full text of an article, the research assistant contacted the corresponding author for the article in question. For the Google searches, two reviewers (POL and CTG) determined eligibility after reading each web page in detail. Any discrepancies between the two reviewers were resolved through weekly discussion with the research assistant and, if needed, the principal investigator (SL).

Step 4: Charting the data

A standardized data extraction spreadsheet (Microsoft Excel) was developed by the authors. The following information was extracted: document details, design and methodology, relevant results and authors’ conclusions. Subdomains are outlined in Table 2. A similar but simplified template was used for web pages. The extraction of data was undertaken by two independent coders (POL and CTG).

Prior to data extraction, this form was pilot tested by the two coders with ten selected records that were sufficiently different in terms of design and objectives to make sure that the template would be appropriate for different types of publications. The form was revised as needed during the data extraction to extract relevant information as comprehensively as possible. Each of the two coders extracted data for half of the records. The other coder then revised the extracted data and any discrepancies between the two coders were resolved by discussion and consensus. If consensus was not reached, the research assistant was consulted to resolve the disagreement.

Step 5: Collating, summarizing and reporting results

As recommended by Levac et al. [48], this step was broken into three distinct tasks: (1) analyzing data (including descriptive numerical summary analysis); (2) reporting results and outcomes relevant to the research questions and (3) considering the meaning and implications of the findings for future research, practice and policy. First, a summary of each included document is presented, including the following information: author(s), year of publication, country, publication type, study/document design, journal discipline, first author’s discipline, target population, word(s) used to designate “pleasure”, and objective(s) targeted by the document. For each research question, a quantitative analysis for each relevant characteristic is presented. Second, data outcomes were analyzed and reported according to research questions. For the first research question (Q1: i.e., key dimensions of eating pleasure), documents analyzed were mainly those focusing on the identification of the dimensions of eating pleasure as well as those reporting data about the links between eating pleasure and dietary behavior and/or health outcomes. Each document was uploaded into qualitative NVivo Software (version 10, QSR International, Burlington, MA) and a thematic analysis of the dimensions of eating pleasure was undertaken. A basic tree node structure was built independently by the two coders and the research assistant based on 10 key articles reporting a detailed approach of eating pleasure [8,4244,6469]. These basic tree nodes were then pooled together, and each dimension and sub-dimension of eating pleasure was described based on existing definitions and the literature. The two coders then reviewed all documents independently and extracted all dimensions of eating pleasure. New dimensions and sub-dimensions were created as needed during the extraction process. For peer-reviewed articles and theses, only the methodology and results sections were coded in order not to duplicate findings. The two coders met regularly to compare coding and to discuss all new added nodes and those for which agreement was not perfect (kappa coefficient under 100%). Any discrepancies were discussed and resolved with the research assistant. The tree node structure was discussed with the principal investigator at the beginning, middle and end of the extraction process. For the second research question (Q2: i.e., links between eating pleasure and dietary behaviors and health outcomes), the reported outcome was categorized in line with study authors’ judgement as favorable (i.e., eating pleasure was associated with favorable outcomes), unfavorable (i.e., eating pleasure was associated with unfavorable outcomes), mixed (i.e., eating pleasure was associated with both favorable and unfavorable outcomes) or neutral (i.e., eating pleasure was not significantly associated with any outcomes). Outcomes most frequently studied in association with eating pleasure (i.e., diet quality, food choices, portion size, restrained eating, weight/BMI and depressive symptoms) were analyzed separately. Finally, associations between eating pleasure and outcomes were analyzed according to the ways to conceptualize (i.e., dimensions) eating pleasure as identified by coders. For the third research question (Q3: i.e., most promising intervention strategies using eating pleasure to promote healthy eating), we included studies evaluating the effect of an intervention using eating pleasure and also studies that showed any increase in eating pleasure in response to their intervention. For these studies, we made sure to give enough information about the nature of intervention groups, data analysis and measurement of outcomes in the text and tables in order to fully understand the context in which these studies were carried out. Finally, a table that groups together the dimensions based on their likely favorable or unfavorable impact and ordering the dimensions based on level of evidence was used to summarize the findings. Melnyk & Fineout-Overholt’s guidelines were used to assess the level of evidence for each dimension [70]. In this model, evidence is assigned a score from 1 to 7, with 1 being the highest level of evidence. The meaning and implications of the findings for research, practice, and policy, are addressed in the Discussion, along with gaps identified through this review.

Step 6: Consulting with an expert committee

An expert committee was set up at the beginning of the study, including the authors of this study and two research assistants with expertise in scoping review methodology. This expert committee was consulted for their feedback and deeper understanding of the data at each critical step of our scoping review. In addition, to help to put our findings on the dimensions of eating pleasure into perspective and to interpret them in the context of our population, results were compared to those from focus groups addressing perceptions about eating pleasure in the French-Canadian population conducted earlier by our research team [42].

Results

From the search, 27,974 records were found from the databases and 934 records identified through other sources (hand-searching references, government and organization websites, and expert committee) for a total of 28,908 records imported into Endnote (Fig 1). After exclusion of duplicates, 18,002 records were screened, from which 16,606 were excluded on the basis of Titles/Abstracts. Therefore, 1,396 were reviewed in full and 119 met our eligibility criteria and were included in this review. S2 Table provides the references and characteristics of eligible documents. Some documents addressed more than one of our research questions. Of the 119 records, 110 (92.4%) were used for the identification of dimensions of eating pleasure (Q1), 45 (37.8%) documented the links between eating pleasure and dietary behaviors and health (Q2), and 12 (10.1%) intervention studies were used to identify most promising methods using eating pleasure to promote healthy dietary behaviors (Q3).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Flow chart of the scoping review process.

a For the exclusion criteria, the reason identified for each document is the one that the coders identified first. This does not exclude the possibility that other criteria were not met. b Some documents addressed more than one research question.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244292.g001

Q1—Key dimensions of eating pleasure

Description of included documents.

Of the 110 documents that identified dimensions of eating pleasure, 66 (60.0%) were peer-reviewed articles identified through scientific databases, five (4.5%) were theses and dissertations and 39 (35.5%) were governmental or organizational web pages (Table 3 and S2 Table). The first author’s discipline of peer-reviewed articles as well as of theses and dissertations (n = 71) was most often psychology (n = 18; 25.4%), followed by business, economy and management (n = 16; 22.5%), nutrition (n = 8; 11.3%) and food sciences (n = 7; 9.9%). For peer-reviewed articles (n = 66), one third (n = 26; 39.4%) were published in a journal for which the major discipline was psychology, followed by nutrition (n = 9; 13.6%), public health (n = 8; 12.1%), food sciences (n = 7; 10.6%), business, economy and management (n = 6; 9.1%). For governmental and health-related organizational web pages (n = 39), 13 (33.3%) were dietary guidelines, 11 (28.2%) were healthy eating promotion tools, nine (23.1%) were reports and six (15.4%) were program descriptions. In total, 88 documents (80.0%) were written in English and 22 (20.0%) in French. Most of the documents were published after 2000 (93.6%), and 70.9% after 2010. Most represented countries were France (n = 28; 25.5%), United States of America (n = 24; 21.8%), Canada (n = 16; 14.5%), Australia (n = 10; 9.1%), Finland (n = 8; 7.3%) and United Kingdom (n = 6; 5.5%). The words most often found to designate "pleasure" were “pleasur*” or “plaisir” (French for pleasure) (n = 58; 52.7%), followed by “enjoy*” (n = 26; 23.6%), “hedon*” (n = 5; 4.5%), “fun” (n = 3; 2.7%), “happiness” (n = 1; 0.9%) or a mix of the above words (n = 17; 15.5%). The populations targeted were young and middle-aged adults in half of the documents (n = 56; 50.9%), followed by children (n = 9; 8.2%), older adults (n = 6; 5.5%), adolescents (n = 5; 4.5%), children and adolescents (n = 4; 3.6%), children, adolescents and young and middle-aged adults (n = 3; 2.7%), children and young and middle-aged adults (n = 2; 1.8%), and adolescents and young and middle-aged adults (n = 1; 0.9%). In addition, 24 (21.8%) documents targeted all population strata.

Key dimensions of eating pleasure.

The key dimensions of eating pleasure are presented in Table 4. The dimensions were identified by two independent coders, quoting phrases in documents that could complete the sentence “The pleasure of eating is…”. Eighty-nine eating pleasure sub-dimensions were identified through the literature review and grouped into 22 key dimensions (see S4 Table for the complete description of each sub-dimension). In order of importance, eating pleasure was related to (1) sensory experiences (n = 56; 50.9%; e.g., taste, appearance, texture), (2) social experiences (n = 47; 42.7%; e.g., eating with others, preparing meals with others, respecting shared norms and practices such as culture and traditions), (3) food characteristics besides sensory attributes (n = 30; 27.3%; e.g., healthy, unhealthy, fresh), (4) food preparation process (n = 21; 19.1%; e.g., cooking, gardening, grocery shopping), (5) novelty (n = 18; 16.4%; e.g., discovering new food, dishes and tastes, learning about food), (6) variety (n = 16; 14.5%; e.g., in type of food, flavors and ways that food is prepared), (7) mindful eating (n = 15; 13.6%), (8) visceral eating (n = 14; 12.7%; e.g., eating to reward yourself, to cope with emotions), (9) place where food is consumed (n = 13; 11.8%; e.g., eating at restaurants, eating while travelling, eating at home), (10) memories associated with eating (n = 12; 10.9%), (11) atmosphere where food is consumed (n = 11; 10.0%), (12) psychological and physical state during food intake (n = 11; 10.0%; e.g., experiencing emotions when eating), (13) food anticipation (n = 10; 9.1%), (14) special occasions (n = 10; 9.1%), (15) having the choice (n = 10; 9.1%), (16) food intake structure (n = 9; 8.2%; e.g., diet structure, balanced diet, meal composition), (17) taking time (n = 9; 8.2%), (18) health considerations (n = 8; 7.3%; e.g., balancing pleasure and health, making healthy choices), (19) eating according to food preferences (n = 8; 7.3%), (20) the psychological and physical state after food intake (n = 7; 6.4%; e.g., feeling full), (21) respecting eating habits (n = 4; 3.6%) and (22) ideological considerations (n = 4; 3.6%; e.g., environmental movement).

Q2. Links between eating pleasure and dietary behavior/health outcomes

Description of included documents.

Details about characteristics of scientific documents (peer-reviewed articles and theses; n = 45) linking eating pleasure to dietary behavior and health outcomes are presented in Tables 3 and 5. Most studies were cross-sectional (n = 28; 62.2%), followed by short-term, single exposure intervention studies (n = 8; 17.8%), longer-term, prolonged exposure intervention studies (n = 5; 11.1%), qualitative studies (n = 2; 4.4%), mixed-design studies (n = 1; 2.2%) and prospective studies (n = 1; 2.2%; Table 3). Almost all documents were published after 2000 (n = 42; 93.3%), and two thirds were published after 2010 (n = 31; 68.9%; Table 3). Studies were carried out in the United States of America (n = 13; 28.9%), United Kingdom (n = 6; 13.3%), France (n = 5; 11.1%), Canada (n = 5; 11.1%), Finland (n = 3; 6.7%), Australia (n = 2; 4.4%), Malaysia (n = 2; 4.4%), Germany (n = 1; 2.2%), Japan (n = 1; 2.2%), Greece (n = 1; 2.2%), Italy (n = 1; 2.2%), Taiwan (n = 1; 2.2%), Vietnam (n = 1; 2.2%) or in multiple countries (n = 3; 6.7%; Table 3). Studies included both men (boys) and women (girls) (n = 36; 80.0%) or only women (girls) (n = 6; 13.3%), and in three studies (6.7%), the sex of participants was not specified (Table 5). In total, 126,368 participant units (67.7% women) were included in the analyses of all studies, with sample size ranging from 12 to 50,003 participants. Most studies included young and middle-aged adult participants only (mean age between 18 and 65 years; n = 34; 75.6%) while fewer included adolescents only (n = 4; 8.9%), children only (n = 3; 6.7%), older adults only (n = 2; 4.4%), children and young and middle-aged adults (n = 1; 2.2%), and adolescents and young and middle-aged adults (n = 1; 2.2%). Most studies did not report participants’ BMI (n = 25; 55.6%).

thumbnail
Table 5. Characteristics of included studies (n = 45) reporting links between eating pleasure and dietary/health related outcomes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244292.t005

Measures of eating pleasure.

Tools used in these studies to measure eating pleasure are presented in Table 5. Eating pleasure was measured using a variety of tools, namely interviews (n = 2) [89,137], pairing and categorization tasks (n = 1) [26], single items (n = 11) [28,29,76,109,111,140,141,147,148,152,160], multi-item questionnaires developed by authors for the purpose of the study (n = 8) [21,74,112,136,145,148,150,156], adapted versions of existing multi-item questionnaires (n = 7) [13,3335,75,157,158] or existing multi-item questionnaires (n = 13) [21,25,27,30,36,74,84,86,88,125,126,146,151]. In total, 37 different tools were used to measure eating pleasure: 1 interview process, 2 pairing and categorization tasks, 15 single items and 19 questionnaires. Questionnaires used to measure eating pleasure were (1) the Pleasures Questionnaire (Appleton & McGowan, 2006) [156], (2) the pleasure subscale of the Health and Taste Attitude Scale (Roininen et al., 1999) [25,84,86,88], (3) an international survey on the causes and treatment of obesity (Bray et al., 1992) [35], (4) the Epicurean Eating Pleasure Questionnaire (Cornil & Chandon, 2016) [21], (5) the external eating and emotional eating subscales of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ; van Strien et al., 1986) [21], (6) the pleasure subscale of the Dish Choice Questionnaire (Ducrot et al., 2015) [27,36], (7) the enjoyment of food subscale of the Adult’s Eating Behaviors Questionnaire (Hunot et al., 2016) [157], (8) an emotions questionnaire (Richins, 1997) [151], (9) the pleasure motivation subscale of the Motivation to Eat Questionnaire (Jackson et al., 2003) [145,146], (10) the pleasure subscale of a questionnaire developed by Lindeman et al. (1999) and assessing food choice motives [74], (11) the pleasure subscale (sensory appeal) of The Food Choice Questionnaire (Steptoe et al. 1995) [30,74,75], (12) the enjoyment of food subscale of the self-reported version of the Children’s Eating Behaviors Questionnaire (Loh et al., 2013) [158], (13) the pleasure eating subscale of the Meanings of Eating Index (MEI; McClain et al., 2011) [150], (14) a questionnaire assessing hedonic consumption values (Babin et al. 1994) [33], (15) the pleasure motivation subscale of the Eating Motivation Survey (adapted from Renner et al., 2012) [13,34], (16) the pleasure subscale of a questionnaire assessing consumption attitudes (Remick et al., 2009) [111], (17) the pleasure subscale of a questionnaire developed to explore the role of food in life (Rozin et al., 1999) [125,126], (18) the food enjoyment subscale of a questionnaire assessing children’s relationship with food (Ensaff et al., 2016) [136] and (19) a questionnaire assessing remembered enjoyment (Robinson et al. 2011) [148]. Nine studies [21,26,28,74,76,136,140,148,152] included multiple measures of eating pleasure while six intervention studies [7,31,32,37,159,161] that used eating pleasure in their strategies did not measure the impact of their interventions on eating pleasure.

Dietary behavior and health outcomes.

Dietary behavior and health outcomes for each study are presented in Table 5. Of the 45 studies identified, 31 (68.9%) included only dietary outcomes [7,13,2529,33,34,37,75,76,84,86,88,109,111,125,126,136,140,145148,150152,159,160,161], 10 (22.2%) only health outcomes [30,35,36,74,89,112,137,156158] and 4 (8.9%) included both dietary and health outcomes [21,31,32,141].

The dietary outcomes studied were very diverse, the most studied being diet quality (i.e., food/dietary intakes, balanced diet, adherence to nutritional guidelines) (n = 13) [13,2729,32,33,84,86,109,140,148,150,160], food choices (n = 8) [7,26,84,86,125,147,148,160], portion size (n = 4) [21,32,37,159], and restrained eating (n = 4) [21,31,76,145]. Other dietary outcomes were assessed in two studies or less and comprized emotional eating (n = 2) [76,145], external eating (n = 2) [76,145], intuitive eating (n = 2) [31,126], perceived importance of family meals (n = 2) [75,111], food neophobia/fussiness (n = 2) [136,141], taste description ability (n = 2) [136,161], likelihood to try new food / recipes (n = 2) [32,136], cooking skills (n = 2) [136,160], the amount time spent for cooking/involvement in cooking activities (n = 2) [32,136], the amount of time spent eating meals (n = 2) [32,109], subjective diet-related quality of life (n = 1) [29], perceived level of importance that individuals place on food (n = 1) [88], skipping breakfast habit (n = 1) [29], energy intake (n = 1) [27], behavioral intentions toward healthy foods (n = 1) [151], late-night snacking (n = 1) [34], family meals frequency (n = 1) [111], eating habits to control weight (n = 1) [146], perceived ease of making healthy food choices (n = 1) [160], disinhibition (n = 1) [31], susceptibility to hunger (n = 1) [31], mindful eating (n = 1) [31], worries about calories (n = 1) [32], nutritional status (n = 1) [25], alcohol intake (n = 1) [32], frequency of consuming satisfying meals (n = 1) [32], the number of meals prepared at home (n = 1) [32], the use of fresh ingredients (n = 1) [32], interest in improving cooking skills (n = 1) [32], the variety of foods used to prepare food (n = 1) [32], and the purchases of locally grown food, seasonally and organically grown food and healthy food (n = 1) [32].

The health outcomes were also diverse, the most studied being body weight/BMI (n = 8) [21,31,32,35,36,74,157,158] and depressive symptoms (n = 3) [30,74,156]. Other health outcomes assessed in two studies or less were happiness (n = 2) [89,137], satisfaction with life (n = 2) [112,156], symptoms of eating disorders (n = 2) [30,74], anxiety symptoms (n = 1) [156], well-being (n = 1) [21], health worries (n = 1) [21], awareness of thinness pressures (n = 1) [74], internalization of thinness pressures (n = 1) [74], appearance dissatisfaction (n = 1) [74], weight dissatisfaction (n = 1) [74], self-esteem (n = 1) [74], self-clarity (n = 1) [112], neuroticism (n = 1) [112] and mood (n = 1) [141].

Links between eating pleasure and dietary/health outcomes.

Summary of associations between eating pleasure and dietary and health outcomes are shown in Fig 2.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Links relating eating pleasure with dietary and health outcomes in the scientific literature.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244292.g002

Among the 35 studies that included dietary outcomes, 20 (57.1%) were cross-sectional studies, eight (22.9%) were short-term, single exposure intervention studies, five (14.3%) were longer-term, prolonged exposure intervention studies, one (2.9%) was a mixed-design study and one (2.9%) was a prospective study. Of these, 20 (57.1%) observed favorable associations between eating pleasure and dietary variables [7,25,26,28,29,32,75,88,111,125,126,136,140,147,148,151,159161] while six (17.1%) reported unfavorable associations [33,34,76,86,145,152], five (14.3%) mixed associations (both favorable and unfavorable associations) [13,21,27,37,146] and four (11.4%) neutral association [84,109,141,150]. When only intervention studies were considered, we observed that most of the papers (11/13; 84.6%) reported positive dietary outcomes [7,32,125,136,140,147,148,159161], while only one observed mixed effects [37] and one neutral effect [141].

When examining diet quality specifically among the different dietary outcomes (n = 13), six studies (46.2%) observed favorable associations between eating pleasure and diet quality [28,29,31,32,140,148] while two (15.4%) reported unfavorable associations [33,86], two (15.4%) mixed associations [13,27] and three (23.1%) reported neutral association [84,109,150]. For food choices (n = 8), six studies (75.0%) observed favorable associations [7,26,125,147,148,160] while two (25.0%) reported neutral association [84,86]. For portion size (n = 4), two studies (50.0%) observed favorable associations [32,159] while two (50.0%) reported mixed associations [21,37]. Finally, for restrained eating, two studies (50%) reported unfavorable associations [21,76] while two (50.0%) observed neutral association [31,145]. Other dietary outcomes were assessed in two studies or less, therefore separated analysis was not performed for these variables.

Among the 14 studies that documented health outcomes, nine (64.3%) were cross-sectional studies, two (14.3%) were longer-term, prolonged exposure intervention studies, two (14.3%) were qualitative studies and one (7.1%) was a short-term, single exposure intervention study. Of these, three (21.4%) observed favorable associations between eating pleasure and health variables [30,89,137] while four (28.6%) reported unfavorable associations [35,36,74,157], two (14.3%) mixed associations [21,112] and five (35.7%) neutral association [31,32,141,156,158] (Fig 2). Only three intervention studies measured health outcomes and they reported no impact of the eating pleasure-related interventions on these variables.

Most studies linked eating pleasure to a higher weight/BMI (n = 5/8; 62.5%) [21,35,36,74,157], while three studies reported neutral association (37.5%) [31,32,158]. In addition, most studies reported neutral association between eating pleasure and depressive symptoms (n = 2/3; 66.7%) [74,156] while one study reported a favorable association (33.3%) [30]. Other health outcomes were assessed in two studies or less, therefore separate analysis was not performed for these variables.

Links between dimensions of eating pleasure and dietary/health outcomes.

For each article, up to five dimensions of eating pleasure were identified by coders. In order of importance, dimensions studied in relation to dietary outcomes were sensory experiences (n = 12) [7,21,25,26,33,37,75,76,84,86,88,109], social experiences (n = 6) [27,29,75,111,125,126], visceral eating (n = 5) [13,21,34,145,146], memories associated with eating (n = 4) [125,126,147,148], food anticipation (n = 4) [125,126,151,152], psychological and physical state during food intake (n = 3) [33,150,151], food characteristics besides sensory attributes (n = 2) [27,75], food preparation process (n = 2) [28,136], novelty (n = 2) [27,33], mindful eating (n = 2) [140,141], variety (n = 1) [27], the place (n = 1) [126], food intake structure (n = 1) [28], food preferences (n = 1) [27], and psychological and physical state after food intake (n = 1) [75] (Table 6). No study relating eating pleasure to dietary outcomes conceptualized eating pleasure in terms of atmosphere, eating on special occasions, having the choice, taking time, health considerations, eating habits or ideological considerations.

thumbnail
Table 6. Links between eating pleasure and dietary/health outcomes according to the dimensions used to conceptualize eating pleasure.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244292.t006

In order to evaluate associations between dimensions of eating pleasure and dietary outcomes, we focused on dimensions identified in at least two different documents (Table 6). We found that when eating pleasure was conceptualized using social experiences, 5 out of 6 studies (83.3%) [29,75,111,125,126] showed that eating pleasure was associated with positive dietary outcomes while the percentage of studies showing positive dietary outcomes reached 100% when eating pleasure was conceptualized using food preparation process [28,136] or memories associated with eating [125,126,147,148]. When eating pleasure was conceptualized as food anticipation, 3 out of 4 studies (75.0%) [125,126,151] showed that eating pleasure was associated with positive dietary outcomes. However, when pleasure was conceptualized using the visceral eating dimension, it was mostly associated with negative outcomes (3/5; 60.0%) [21,34,145].

Dimensions of eating pleasure studied in relation to health outcomes were fewer in number than when studying dietary outcomes (Table 6). In order of importance, dimensions were sensory experiences (n = 4) [21,30,74,89], novelty (n = 3) [36,89,137], social experiences (n = 2) [36,112], variety (n = 2) [36,137], memories associated with eating (n = 2) [89,112], food characteristics (n = 1) [36], mindful eating (n = 1) [141], visceral eating (n = 1) [21], psychological and physical state during food intake (n = 1) [137], food anticipation (n = 1) [112], taking time (n = 1) [137], and food preferences (n = 1) [36]. Among those studies, none conceptualized eating pleasure in terms of food preparation process, place, atmosphere, eating on special occasions, having the choice, food intake structure, health considerations, psychological and physical state after food intake, eating habits or ideological considerations.

In order to evaluate associations between dimensions of eating pleasure and health outcomes, we focused on dimensions identified in at least two different documents. When eating pleasure was conceptualized as sensory experiences, 3 out of 4 studies (75%) [21,30,89] showed that eating pleasure was associated with positive health outcomes. When eating pleasure was conceptualized as novelty, 2 out of 3 studies (66.7%) [89,137] showed that eating pleasure was associated with positive health outcomes. No dimension identified in at least two documents was predominantly associated with negative outcomes.

Q. 3 –Most promising intervention strategies using eating pleasure to promote healthy dietary behaviors

Description of included documents.

Among the 13 intervention studies identified in this review (Table 5), one was excluded from this analysis since it did not permit identification of intervention strategies using eating pleasure to promote healthy dietary behaviors [125]. In addition, two studies reported results from the same long-term intervention [31,161]. Therefore, 12 intervention papers were included in this analysis [7,32,37,136,140,141,147,148,159161], reporting results from 11 independent interventions. Seven (63.6%) were short-term, single exposure interventions while four (36.4%) were longer-term, prolonged exposure interventions. Studies were all published after 2007. Interventions were conducted in the United States of America (n = 3; 27.3%), United Kingdom (n = 3; 27.3%), France (n = 1; 9.1%), Canada (n = 1; 9.1%), Germany (n = 1; 9.1%) or in multiple countries (n = 2; 18.2%). In sum, these intervention studies included 10,243 participants, of whom 7,835 (76.5%) were women, 1,569 (15.3%) were men and 839 (8.2%) for whom the sex was not specified. Sample size ranged from 16 to 7,385 participants. Nine were conducted in young and middle-aged adults only (81.8%), one in children only (9.1%) and one in both children and young and middle-aged adults (9.1%).

Intervention strategies using eating pleasure to promote healthy dietary behaviors.

First, two studies investigated whether inducing a pleasure mindset influences portion sizes and food choices. Hege et al. [159] investigated whether different mindsets (healthiness mindset, pleasure mindset, fullness mindset) were associated with altered activity in certain brain areas during the pre-meal selection of portion sizes. Results showed that, compared with a control condition, participants reduced their portion sizes when considering eating for health or pleasure while increasing their portion sizes when considering eating to be full until the next meal. Petit et al. [7] assessed whether drawing attention to the pleasure of eating healthily increased healthy food choices in individuals with a high BMI. Participants evaluated pictures of 64 selected food items in three different “attention conditions” in which participants were either instructed to consider the benefits of eating healthy food (health condition), to consider the pleasure of eating healthily (pleasure condition), or to consider any features of the food that came to their mind (no diet condition). Results showed that participants were more likely to mention that they would eat the pictured healthy foods in the pleasure diet condition than in the no diet condition. Taken together, these results suggest that, compared with a control condition, a pleasure mindset may have favorable effects on portion sizes and food choices.

Four intervention studies investigated whether focusing on sensory sensations influences dietary intakes, eating behaviors and food choices. Gravel et al. [31,161] investigated whether a sensory-based 6-week intervention influenced eating related attitudes and behaviors among restrained women (dieters). Specific themes addressed were the relationship with food, hunger and satiety cues, sensory sensations, and the pleasures associated with eating. Compared with a waiting list control group, women in the sensory-based intervention decreased disinhibition (i.e., the tendency to overeat in response to cognitive or emotional cues), while increasing the intuitive eating subscale related to the unconditional permission to eat and the use of terms associated with the senses and with pleasure to describe food. No effects on restrained eating, susceptibility to hunger, and BMI were observed. Moreover, Cornil and Chandon [37] investigated how multisensory imagery can make people happier with smaller food portions. During the intervention, participants were asked to vividly imagine the multisensory pleasure (taste, smell, and texture) of three hedonic foods before choosing the size of another hedonic food. This short intervention led to mixed results, showing that, compared with several control conditions (e.g., non-food sensory imagery condition), focusing on sensory pleasure made hungry eaters and nondieters to choose and prefer smaller portions of hedonic foods while sated eaters and dieters chose larger portions. Two interventions using mindful eating strategies and focusing on the sensory properties of food were also performed. Arch et al. [140] showed that directing participants’ attention as fully as possible towards the pleasure associated with the sensory experience of eating increased the enjoyment of a commonly pleasurable food (chocolate), and a food with generally more mixed associations (raisins), and led to lower calorie consumption of unhealthy food, an effect mediated by greater eating enjoyment as compared with a control condition. Finally, in a study by Hong et al. [141] participants were randomly assigned to a mindful raisin-eating task condition (i.e., subjects instructed to be nonjudgmental and fully aware, with all senses, of different aspects of the raisin), a non-mindful raisin-eating task (control) condition, or a no-task baseline condition. All participants were thereafter offered different types of food that they could sample. Subjects in the mindful raisin-eating task condition indicated higher levels of enjoyment of the sampled food than did subjects in the two other conditions. However, mindful eating had no effect on the likelihood of trying novel or typically avoided foods. In sum, results suggest that sensory-based interventions can have positive impact on eating behaviors and portion sizes. Mindful eating strategies focusing on the sensory properties of food also seem to increase eating enjoyment and to lower energy intake from unhealthy food while having no impact on the likelihood to sample a novel or typically avoided food.

Two interventions included aspects related to food preparation and/or sharing. Adam et al. [160] assessed the effectiveness of open, online nutrition and cooking instruction in improving eating behaviors of participants. Results showed that, in addition to increase the percentage of participants who found yesterday’s dinner to be very or extremely enjoyable, the intervention resulted in significant changes in eating behaviors and meal composition, mirroring the messaging of the course. Mean overall perceived ease of making healthy food choices and preparing home cooked meals also increased significantly over time. Ensaff et al. [136] evaluated Jamie Oliver’s Kitchen Garden Project (JOKGP). This project aims at developing food and cooking skills in children, increase their willingness to try new foods, and gain a better understanding of where food comes from. In the intervention school, children reported an increase in cooking enjoyment. They also reported an increase in helping with cooking at home and taste description score, and a decrease in food neophobia and fussiness. At follow-up, higher scores were reported for the children in the intervention school compared with those from the control school. Taken together, these results suggest that interventions including cooking lessons and sharing increase the enjoyment of eating and cooking, in addition to improving eating behaviors, diet quality, taste description, and involvement in cooking.

Two studies performed by Robinson et al. [147,148] included aspects related to memories. First, they examined the relationship between remembered enjoyment of vegetable eating, predictions about liking vegetables, and vegetable consumption [147]. Results showed that the recall of positive and enjoyable memories about past vegetable consumption resulted in higher liking for vegetables and the choice of a larger portion size of vegetables when compared with recall of a personal non-food memory, a non-vegetable food memory, or when visualizing someone else enjoying eating vegetables. They then examined if a simple instruction to rehearse what participants found enjoyable about a food immediately after eating it could be used to increase remembered enjoyment of a food (a Mediterranean vegetable quiche) and whether this would result in individuals choosing to eat more of this food at a later date [148]. Compared with controls (i.e., neutral meal rehearsal of the ingredients in the vegetable quiche and how long it took to eat it), participants in the intervention group showed a 90% increase in the amount of low-energy Mediterranean vegetable quiche chosen and eaten. In sum, these results suggest that recalling enjoyable eating memories related to healthy foods may increase the portion size of these foods later.

Finally, Sasson et al. [32] assessed the effects of a 3-week study program conducted in Italy that emphasizes the importance of enjoying the cooking and eating experience, taking time to eat, and valuing high-quality, unprocessed food. The program positively influenced behaviors related to food shopping, preparation, eating, and dieting six months later. More precisely, a majority of participants (i.e., 50% or more) reported increases in purchases of olive oil, locally grown food, seasonally and organically grown food, and food from farmers’ markets. The consumption of more satisfying meals, the number of meals prepared at home, the use of fresh ingredients, the interest in improving cooking skills, the variety of foods used to prepare food, the use of olive oil in cooking and trying new recipes were also all increased by the majority of participants. Decreases in portion sizes and in worrying about calories were also observed. This program had no impact on self-reported weight. These results suggest that a more comprehensive intervention using eating pleasure as the core strategy may have many beneficial effects on dietary behaviors.

A summary of findings is presented in Table 7. This table groups together the dimensions based on their likely favorable or unfavorable impact and presents the dimensions in an order based on the level of evidence. Also, where applicable, intervention strategies that have shown promising effects for each dimension are also presented.

thumbnail
Table 7. Summary of findings: Dimensions of eating pleasure with favorable or unfavorable links with dietary/health outcomes, level of evidence and promising intervention strategies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244292.t007

Discussion

Many authors now claim that eating pleasure is a promising approach to fostering healthy dietary behaviors [19,31,69,142,162] and some dietary guidelines, such as Canada’s Food Guide [22], now include the enjoyment of food among their healthy eating recommendations. In spite of this, until now, no comprehensive review assessing the links between eating pleasure and dietary behaviors was available, making difficult to determine whether and how the pleasure of eating can be a lever for healthy eating and health. Our review contributes to filling some important gaps. First, it provides a more comprehensive overview of the key dimensions of eating pleasure, which may serve as a foundation for researchers interested in studying this concept. This literature review also mapped the evidence to date on the links relating eating pleasure with healthy eating and health, a key step for determining whether the pleasure of eating can be a potential ally in the promotion of healthy eating. This review also identified strategies using eating pleasure that seem promising to include in interventions aimed at promoting healthy eating. However, some major gaps remain in our understanding of how to use eating pleasure in the promotion of healthy eating habits, which we discuss below.

In total, 110 studies were examined for their way to conceptualize eating pleasure, including both scientific and grey literature. The review of the literature suggests that eating pleasure is a multidimensional concept. Indeed, in our qualitative assessment, two independent coders identified 22 different key dimensions used to define eating pleasure. The dimensions most frequently cited were sensory experiences, social experiences, food characteristics besides sensory attributes, food preparation process, novelty, variety, mindful eating, visceral eating, place where food is eaten, and memories associated with eating. This way to conceptualize eating pleasure is much broader than those found in previous reviews, which usually address only one or two dimensions of eating pleasure [163168]. It is interesting to note that several similarities exist between this broader way to conceptualize eating pleasure and key dimensions we gathered in a study we recently conducted. In our focus groups [42], participants mostly defined eating pleasure in terms of taste, aesthetics, cooking, sharing a meal, relaxing, variety, nutritional aspects of food, and discovering new foods. In sum, our conceptual framework of eating pleasure provides evidence-based key dimensions and terminology that researchers may use to build consistency in the field and develop our understanding of the role of eating pleasure in the promotion of healthy eating habits. These results also highlight that eating pleasure may have different meanings, and that it is important for policymakers to clarify what is meant by this term when using it in policies and promotional materials.

The purpose of this scoping review was to assess whether and how eating pleasure can be a lever for healthy dietary behaviors and health. Although results were not uniform across studies, our scoping review highlights that eating pleasure is mostly (57.1%) associated with positive dietary behavior outcomes. This was especially the case for food choices, as six out of eight studies showed favorable links. However, most studies investigating the links between eating pleasure and dietary behavior outcomes were cross-sectional, hindering the assumption of causality between eating pleasure and better dietary behaviors. Nevertheless, when we looked only at intervention studies, we observed even more consistent results. In addition to leading to better diet quality [140,160], healthier portion sizes [32,37,147,148,159] and better food choices [7,160], pleasure-related strategies used in these intervention studies also increased perception of tastiness and the liking of healthy food [147], time for and perceived ease of cooking and eating [32,136,160], proportion of meals prepared at home [32] and interest in improving cooking skills [32], while decreasing disinhibition (i.e., the tendency to overeat in response to cognitive or emotional cues) [31] and food neophobia and fussiness [136]. However, although these intervention studies had promising results concerning the potential impact of using eating pleasure in the promotion of healthy eating, these findings still need to be confirmed since to date, only 11 independent intervention studies have been conducted, and these used different methodologies, evaluated different outcomes and most of the time integrated only one or a few dimensions of eating pleasure. This may be explained by the fact that the integration of eating pleasure in the promotion of healthy eating is still at a nascent stage, all intervention studies being published after 2007.

The link between eating pleasure and health outcomes has been less studied and seems more inconsistent. Only two outcomes were studied in more than two studies: body weight/BMI and depressive symptoms. For body weight and BMI, most studies reported that eating pleasure was associated with a higher weight/BMI. However, it is important to note that the two intervention studies that documented weight/BMI observed a neutral effect. Three cross-sectional studies also assessed the link between eating pleasure and depressive symptoms, with 2 out of 3 studies showing no association between these two variables. This literature review therefore highlights a gap in research on links between eating pleasure and health-related variables, including body weight but also other health indicators less studied such as depression and anxiety symptoms, well-being, self-esteem and body weight dissatisfaction.

Tools used to measure eating pleasure in studies examining the links between eating pleasure and dietary and health outcomes used a great variety of approaches. In total, 37 different tools were used to measure eating pleasure, which greatly compromises comparisons across studies. As there seems to be no universal definition of eating pleasure in the literature, authors could define this concept according to their own perception. Inconsistent approaches and measurement of eating pleasure between studies may be at least partly responsible of divergence in results reported. The development of standardized and validated measurement tools is thus essential to have a better understanding of the links relating eating pleasure, and its key dimensions, with dietary and health outcomes.

To overcome the inconsistency in the measurement of eating pleasure, we decided to describe the associations between eating pleasure and dietary and health outcomes, taking into consideration the different dimensions that defined eating pleasure in each study. On the one hand, when eating pleasure was conceptualized as sensory experiences, mindful eating, memories, social experiences, food preparation process and food anticipation, favorable dietary outcomes were reported in the literature. In addition, findings from intervention studies suggested that sensory-based interventions (e.g., food-tasting activities; multisensory imagery), mindful eating strategies focusing on the sensory experience of eating, the rehearsal of enjoyable eating memories related to healthy foods, cooking lessons/experiences and sharing activities may result in beneficial effects on dietary behaviors. It is worth noting that these findings are consistent with those of other studies that did not specifically focus on eating pleasure, but that observed favorable outcomes using similar intervention strategies as those identified as promising in this review [169172]. On the other hand, visceral eating pleasure, defined as eating pleasure associated with short-term visceral impulses triggered by hunger, external cues or internal emotional urges, was more strongly related to detrimental effects on dietary outcomes according to cross-sectional studies. For health outcomes, most cross-sectional studies found that sensory experiences and novelty were favorably associated with these outcomes. Taken together, these results suggest that the different dimensions of eating pleasure are characterized by a specific pattern of associations with dietary and health outcomes, some being more favorable than others. These promising avenues may then inform whether and how the pleasure of eating can be a lever for healthy eating. However, even if this review was an essential first step to propose impactful strategies in the promotion of healthy eating, it will be essential for health professionals and policymakers to tailor messages and intervention strategies to fit the local context, including social, cultural and socioeconomic factors, to ensure that these strategies are well aligned with community needs and interests. Additional steps, including a comprehensive needs assessment, are therefore needed before using these promising strategies in policies and dietary interventions within a specific population.

It is important to highlight some gaps identified by this review. First, considering the high number of dimensions of eating pleasure (89 sub-dimensions grouped into 22 key dimensions), some of these have been less studied (e.g., place and atmosphere where food is consumed, eating on special occasions, having the choice, taking time). The lack of studies, however, does not necessarily indicate that these dimensions of eating pleasure are not favorably or unfavorably related to dietary behaviors and health outcomes. Rather, these findings highlight the fact that research on the pleasure of eating in the context of promoting healthy eating is still in its infancy and further studies are needed to get a more comprehensive perspective on the place of eating pleasure, and its multiple dimensions, in the promotion of healthy eating. In addition, some limitations of the included intervention studies need to be highlighted. First, intervention studies were more likely to include only one dimension of eating pleasure, to be short-term single exposure interventions, and to be performed with women. However, some reasons may explain these methodological choices. Short-term exposure can be more easily used to test hypotheses at the outset, allowing to identify more efficiently dimensions of eating pleasure that seem to be more promising in the promotion of healthy eating. These promising dimensions can then be tested in larger, longer-term interventions. Also, the testing of one dimension at a time allows for a better assessment of the impact of this dimension on the variables of interest, which can be very useful when designing more complex interventions afterwards. The fact that more women have been included in previous intervention studies is congruent with what is observed in other systematic reviews in nutrition, highlighting most of the time a greater proportion of women than men in these studies [173176]. These gender disparities may be explained by gender-specific differences in many areas of nutrition, including the fact that women have a greater interest in nutrition and seek nutrition counselling more frequently than men do [177]. Finally, no intervention study examined the effectiveness of intervention strategies by sex, age or BMI, and only two comprehensive intervention studies using different dimensions of eating pleasure were reported in the present review [31,32]. Therefore, these findings highlight some research gaps and the need for conducting well-developed and evidence-based intervention studies before reaching a firm conclusion about the place of eating pleasure, and its multiple dimensions, in interventions aiming to promote healthy eating.

Methodological strengths and limitations

This scoping review has many methodological strengths. First, the search strategy was developed with the help of a librarian specializing in nutrition-related research, limiting the risk of missing relevant documents. The search strategy included a wide range of databases, allowing us to capture a comprehensive sample from the many disciplines related to eating pleasure. In addition, the search strategy included both scientific and grey literature, allowing for a more comprehensive approach of eating pleasure. A rigorous methodology comprising two independent coders (one specialized in nutrition and one specialized in social sciences) who reviewed evidence at each step of the scoping review resulted in a substantial reduction in possible errors. In addition, the use of the scoping review per se is a strength, allowing us to address a broad, multidisciplinary topic using a wide range of research designs. Finally, we used the PRISMA extension for scoping reviews checklist to report these results, ensuring methodological and reporting quality [178]. Some limitations of the present review must also be acknowledged. First, we included only studies conducted in developed countries, with participants aged 5 years old and older, limiting the generalization of our results. Second, we reviewed only documents written in English or French, thus we cannot exclude the possibility that we missed some studies. Finally, several documents mentioned dimensions of eating pleasure only very briefly, thus making interpretation difficult for coders. In order not to bias the results of this review, the coders therefore analyzed them as they were described, without over-interpreting them. Accordingly, we do not exclude the possibility that some dimensions can be combined in further analyses.

Conclusion

This comprehensive review of the literature about eating pleasure showed that this concept involves numerous key dimensions. It showed favorable links between eating pleasure and dietary outcomes and identified promising strategies for using eating pleasure in intervention strategies. However, the role occupied by studies evaluating the effects of eating pleasure on healthy eating is still minimal, and additional well-developed and evidence-based intervention studies are needed before reaching a firm conclusion about the place that eating pleasure, and its multiple dimensions, should occupy in the promotion of healthy eating.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Search strategies (other than Medline).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244292.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Characteristics of included documents.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244292.s002

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Key dimensions of eating pleasure, with description of sub-dimensions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244292.s004

(DOCX)

S1 File. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) checklist.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244292.s005

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Mylène Turcotte and Annie Lapointe for their valuable assistance in the scoping review process. They also thank the specialized librarian, Daniela Zavala Mora, for her help in developing the comprehensive search strategy, and Louisa Blair for copyediting the article.

References

  1. 1. World Health Organization. Diet, nutrition and the prevention of chronic diseases: report of a joint WHO/FAO expert consultation [Internet]. 2002 [cited Nobember 1 2020]. Available from: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/924120916X.
  2. 2. World Health Organization. Healthy diet [Internet]. 2020 [cited November 1 2020]. Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/healthy-diet.
  3. 3. Imamura F, Micha R, Khatibzadeh S, Fahimi S, Shi P, Powles J, et al. Dietary quality among men and women in 187 countries in 1990 and 2010: a systematic assessment. Lancet Glob Health. 2015; 3: e132–e142. pmid:25701991
  4. 4. Lieffers JRL, Ekwaru JP, Ohinmaa A, Veugelers PJ. The economic burden of not meeting food recommendations in Canada: The cost of doing nothing. PLoS One. 2018; 13: e0196333. pmid:29702680
  5. 5. Werle COC, Cuny C. The boomerang effect of mandatory sanitary messages to prevent obesity. Mark Lett. 2012; 23: 883–891.
  6. 6. Petit O, Basso F, Merunka D, Spence C, Cheok AD, Oullier O. Pleasure and the control of food intake: An embodied cognition approach to consumer self-regulation. Psychol Mark. 2016; 33: 608–619.
  7. 7. Petit O, Merunka D, Anton J-L, Nazarian B, Spence C, Cheok AD, et al. Health and pleasure in consumers' dietary food choices: Individual differences in the brain's value system. PLoS One; 11(7), 2016, ArtID e0156333. pmid:27428267
  8. 8. Jallinoja P, Pajari P, Absetz P. Negotiated pleasures in health-seeking lifestyles of participants of a health promoting intervention. Health. 2010; 14: 115–130. pmid:20164161
  9. 9. Mayes CR, Thompson DB. What Should We Eat? Biopolitics, Ethics, and Nutritional Scientism. J Bioeth Inq. 2015; 12: 587–599. pmid:26659866
  10. 10. Tuorila H, Cardello AV, Lesher LL. Antecedents and consequences of expectations related to fat-free and regular-fat foods. Appetite. 1994; 23: 247–263. pmid:7726543
  11. 11. Demos KE, McCaffery JM, Thomas JG, Mailloux KA, Hare TA, Wing RR. Identifying the mechanisms through which behavioral weight-loss treatment improves food decision-making in obesity. Appetite. 2017; 114: 93–100. pmid:28315419
  12. 12. Dodds A, Chamberlain K. The problematic messages of nutritional discourse: A case-based critical media analysis. Appetite. 2017; 108: 42–50. pmid:27659870
  13. 13. Phan UT, Chambers EIV. Motivations for choosing various food groups based on individual foods. Appetite. 2016; 105: 204–211. pmid:27235822
  14. 14. Schuldt JP, Hannahan M. When good deeds leave a bad taste. Negative inferences from ethical food claims. Appetite. 2013; 62: 76–83. pmid:23195710
  15. 15. Raghunathan R, Naylor RW, Hoyer WD. The unhealthy equal tasty intuition and its effects on taste inferences, enjoyment, and choice of food products. J Mark. 2006; 70: 170–184.
  16. 16. Werle CO, Trendel O, Ardito G. Unhealthy food is not tastier for everybody: The "healthy = tasty" French intuition. Food Qual Prefer. 2013; 28: 116–121.
  17. 17. Canadian Foundation for Dietetic Research. Tracking Nutrition Trends 2013 [Internet]. 2013 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.cfdr.ca/Downloads/CCFN-docs/CFDR-Tracking-Nutrition-Trends-2013-Report.aspx.
  18. 18. Canadian Foundation for Dietetic Research. Tracking Nutrition Trends 2015 [Internet]. 2015 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.cfdr.ca/Sharing/Tracking-Nutrition-Trends.aspx.
  19. 19. Pettigrew S. Pleasure: An under-utilised 'p' in social marketing for healthy eating. Appetite. 2016; 104: 60–69. pmid:26456411
  20. 20. Jacquier C, Bonthoux F, Baciu M, Ruffieux B. Improving the effectiveness of nutritional information policies: assessment of unconscious pleasure mechanisms involved in food-choice decisions. Nutr Rev. 2012; 70: 118–131. pmid:22300598
  21. 21. Cornil Y, Chandon P. Pleasure as an ally of healthy eating? Contrasting visceral and Epicurean eating pleasure and their association with portion size preferences and wellbeing. Appetite. 2015; 104: 52–59. pmid:26363419
  22. 22. Government of Canada. Canada's Food Guide: Enjoy your food [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://food-guide.canada.ca/en/healthy-eating-recommendations/enjoy-your-food/.
  23. 23. Government of Brazil. Dietary guidelines for the brazilian population [Internet]. 2014 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.paho.org/hq/dmdocuments/2015/dietary-guides-brazil-eng.pdf.
  24. 24. Tribole E, Resch E. Intuitive eating: A recovery book for the chronic dieter: Rediscover the pleasures of eating and rebuild your body image. St. Martin’s Press; 1995.
  25. 25. Bailly N, Maitre I, Van Wymelbeke V. Relationships between nutritional status, depression and pleasure of eating in aging men and women. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2015; 61: 330–336. pmid:26337662
  26. 26. Marty L, Miguet M, Bournez M, Nicklaus S, Chambaron S, Monnery-Patris S. Do hedonic- versus nutrition-based attitudes toward food predict food choices? A cross-sectional study of 6- to 11-year-olds. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017; 14. pmid:29178916
  27. 27. Ducrot P, Mejean C, Fassier P, Alles B, Hercberg S, Peneau S. Associations between motives for dish choice during home-meal preparation and diet quality in French adults: findings from the NutriNet-Sante study. Br J Nutr. 2017; 117: 851–861. pmid:28393743
  28. 28. Crawford D, Ball K, Mishra G, Salmon J, Timperio A. Which food-related behaviours are associated with healthier intakes of fruits and vegetables among women? Public Health Nutr. 2007; 10: 256–265. pmid:17288623
  29. 29. Ainuki T, Akamatsu R, Hayashi F, Takemi Y. Association of enjoyable childhood mealtimes with adult eating behaviors and subjective diet-related quality of life. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2013; 45: 274–278. pmid:23485628
  30. 30. Lindeman M, Stark K. Loss of pleasure, ideological food choice reasons and eating pathology. Appetite. 2000; 35: 263–268. pmid:11073708
  31. 31. Gravel K, Deslauriers A, Watiez M, Dumont M, Dufour Bouchard A-A, Provencher V. Sensory-Based Nutrition Pilot Intervention for Women. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2014; 114: 99–106. pmid:24035461
  32. 32. Sasson L, Black J, Dalton S. Lessons learned about food-related attitudes and behaviors from an Italian Study Abroad Program. Top Clin Nutr. 2007; 22: 357–366.
  33. 33. Olsen SO, Tuu HH. Time perspectives and convenience food consumption among teenagers in Vietnam: The dual role of hedonic and healthy eating values. Food Res Int. 2017; 99: 98–105. pmid:28784552
  34. 34. Phan UT, Chambers EIV. Application of an eating motivation survey to study eating occasions. J Sens Stud. 2016; 31: 114–123.
  35. 35. Cachelin FM, Striegel-Moore RH, Brownell KD. Beliefs about weight gain and attitudes toward relapse in a sample of women and men with obesity. Obes Res. 1998; 6: 231–237. pmid:9618128
  36. 36. Ducrot P, Fassier P, Mejean C, Alles B, Hercberg S, Peneau S. Association between Motives for Dish Choices during Home Meal Preparation and Weight Status in the NutriNet-Sante Study. Nutrients. 2016; 8: 05. pmid:27399764
  37. 37. Cornil Y, Chandon P. Pleasure as a substitute for size: How multisensory imagery can make people happier with smaller food portions. J Mark Res. 2016; 53: 847–864.
  38. 38. Stroebe W, van Koningsbruggen GM, Papies EK, Aarts H. Why most dieters fail but some succeed: A goal conflict model of eating behavior. Psychol Rev. 2013; 120: 110–138. pmid:23230892
  39. 39. Cornil Y, Chandon P. From fan to fat? Vicarious losing increases unhealthy eating, but self-affirmation is an effective remedy. Psychol Sci. 2013; 24: 1936–1946. pmid:23925307
  40. 40. Loewenstein G. Out of Control: Visceral Influences on Behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1996; 65: 272–292.
  41. 41. van Strien T, Frijters JER, Bergers GPA, Defares PB. The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int J Eat Disord. 1986; 5: 295–315.
  42. 42. Landry M, Lemieux S, Lapointe A, Bedard A, Belanger-Gravel A, Begin C, et al. Is eating pleasure compatible with healthy eating? A qualitative study on Quebecers' perceptions. Appetite. 2018; 125: 537–547. pmid:29505810
  43. 43. Macht M, Meininger J, Roth J. The Pleasures of Eating: A Qualitative Analysis. J Happiness Stud. 2005; 6: 137–160.
  44. 44. Guillemin I, Marrel A, Arnould B, Capuron L, Dupuy A, Ginon E, et al. How French subjects describe well-being from food and eating habits? Development, item reduction and scoring definition of the Well-Being related to Food Questionnaire (Well-BFQ(C)). Appetite. 2016; 96: 333–346. pmid:26407804
  45. 45. Peterson J, Pearce PF, Ferguson LA, Langford CA. Understanding scoping reviews: Definition, purpose, and process. J Am Assoc Nurse Pra. 2017; 29: 12–16.
  46. 46. Colquhoun HL, Levac D, Amp, Apos, Brien KK, Straus S, et al. Scoping reviews: time for clarity in definition, methods, and reporting. J Clin Epidemiol. 2014; 67: 1291–1294. pmid:25034198
  47. 47. Arksey H, O'Malley L. Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. Int J Soc Res Methodol. 2005; 8: 19–32.
  48. 48. Levac D, Colquhoun H, O'Brien KK. Scoping studies: advancing the methodology. Implement Sci. 2010; 5: 69–69. pmid:20854677
  49. 49. Peters MDJ, Godfrey C, McInerney P, Munn Z, Tricco AC, Khalil, H. Chapter 11: Scoping Reviews (2020 version). In: Aromataris E, Munn Z (Editors). Joanna Briggs Institute Reviewer's Manual, JBI, 2020. Available from https://reviewersmanual.joannabriggs.org/.
  50. 50. Bédard A, Lamarche P-O, Grégoire L-M, Trudel-Guy C, Provencher V, Desroches S, et al. (2020). Can eating pleasure be a lever for healthy dietary behaviours and health? A Scoping Review Protocol [Internet]. 2020 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://osf.io/5p7b8/.
  51. 51. Nicklaus S. The role of food experiences during early childhood in food pleasure learning. Appetite. 2016; 104: 3–9. pmid:26298009
  52. 52. Wardle J, Guthrie CA, Sanderson S, Rapoport L. Development of the Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001; 42: 963–970. pmid:11693591
  53. 53. Gibson EL, Kreichauf S, Wildgruber A, Vögele C, Summerbell CD, Nixon C, et al. A narrative review of psychological and educational strategies applied to young children's eating behaviours aimed at reducing obesity risk. Obes Rev. 2012; 13: 85–95. pmid:22309067
  54. 54. Salvat E. Education and pleasure: The paradox of food consumption. Young Consum. 2010; 11: 148–152.
  55. 55. Fries LR. Parental Feeding Practices and Associations with Children's Food Acceptance and Picky Eating. Nestle Nutr Inst Workshop Ser. 2019; 91: 31. pmid:30865956
  56. 56. Coulthard H, Williamson I, Palfreyman Z, Lyttle S. Evaluation of a pilot sensory play intervention to increase fruit acceptance in preschool children. Appetite. 2018; 120: 609–615. pmid:29017905
  57. 57. Coulthard H, Thakker D. Enjoyment of Tactile Play Is Associated with Lower Food Neophobia in Preschool Children. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2015; 115: 1134–1140. pmid:25935569
  58. 58. Helland SH, Bere E, Overby NC. Study protocol for a multi-component kindergarten-based intervention to promote healthy diets in toddlers: a cluster randomized trial. BMC Public Health. 2016; 16. pmid:26987876
  59. 59. Wiggins S, Keats S. Future diets in the developing world [Internet]. 2017 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.odi.org/sites/odi.org.uk/files/resource-documents/11739.pdf.
  60. 60. United Nations Development Programme. Human Development report 2016: Human Development for Everyone [Internet]. 2016 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://hdr.undp.org/sites/default/files/2016_human_development_report.pdf.
  61. 61. World Health Organization. Constitution of WHO: principles [Internet] 1948 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://www.who.int/about/mission/en/.
  62. 62. Wallace TC, Bailey RL, Blumberg JB, Burton-Freeman B, Chen CYO, Crowe-White KM, et al. Fruits, vegetables, and health: A comprehensive narrative, umbrella review of the science and recommendations for enhanced public policy to improve intake. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2019: 1. pmid:28799777
  63. 63. Barron P. How Google Works [Internet]. 2011 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://www.internetatschools.com/Articles/Editorial/Features/How-Google-Works-Are-Search-Engines-Really-Dumb-and-Why-Should-Educators-Care-73090.aspx webcite.
  64. 64. Darcel N, Tomé D. Editorial. Appetite. 2016; 104: 1–2. pmid:27058282
  65. 65. Dupuy A, Poulain J-P. Le plaisir dans la socialisation alimentaire. Enfance. 2008; 60: 261–270.
  66. 66. Ekmekcioglu C. The perfect food for perfect health and pleasure? Turk J Med Sci. 2012; 42: 1–8.
  67. 67. Kauppinen-Räisänen H, Gummerus J, Lehtola K. Remembered eating experiences described by the self, place, food, context and time. Br Food J. 2013; 115: 666–685.
  68. 68. Lupton D. The heart of the meal: food preferences and habits among rural Australian couples. Sociol Health Illn. 2000; 22: 94–109.
  69. 69. Rozin P, Fischler C, Imada S, Sarubin A, Wrzesniewski A. Attitudes to food and the role of food in life in the U.S.A., Japan, Flemish Belgium and France: Possible implications for the diet-health debate. Appetite. 1999; 33: 163–180. pmid:10502362
  70. 70. Melnyk B, Fineout-Overholt E. Evidence-Based Practice in Nursing and Healthcare: A Guide to Best Practice. Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins; 2011.
  71. 71. Andersen BV, Hyldig G. Consumers' view on determinants to food satisfaction. A qualitative approach. Appetite. 2015; 95: 9–16. pmid:26119808
  72. 72. De Iulio S, Kovacs S. Between Pleasures and Risks: The Circulation of Knowledge about Food in French Primary Schools. IJSAF. 2014; 21: 327–345.
  73. 73. Larson JS, Redden JP, Elder RS. Satiation from sensory simulation: Evaluating foods decreases enjoyment of similar foods. J Consum Psychol. 2014; 24: 188–194.
  74. 74. Lindeman M, Stark K. Pleasure, pursuit of health or negotiation of identity? Personality correlates of food choice motives among young and middle-aged women. Appetite. 1999; 33: 141–161. pmid:10447986
  75. 75. Marquis M, Shatenstein B. Food choice motives and the importance of family meals among immigrant mothers. Can J Diet Pract Res. 2005; 66: 77–82. pmid:15975196
  76. 76. Mason TB, Pacanowski CR, Lavender JM, Crosby RD, Wonderlich SA, Engel SG, et al. Evaluating the Ecological Validity of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire Among Obese Adults Using Ecological Momentary Assessment. Assessment. 2017: 1073191117719508. pmid:28703009
  77. 77. Moore DJ. Interrupted anticipation after a service failure: The role of olfactory sensation on expected pleasure, taste enjoyment, consumption, and repatronage intentions. Mark Lett. 2013; 24: 399–408.
  78. 78. Newcombe MA, McCarthy MB, Cronin JM, McCarthy SN. "Eat like a man". A social constructionist analysis of the role of food in men's lives. Appetite. 2012; 59: 391–398. pmid:22683804
  79. 79. Onwezen MC, Bartels J. Development and cross-cultural validation of a shortened social representations scale of new foods. Food Qual Prefer. 2013; 28: 226–234.
  80. 80. Rappoport LH, Peters GR, Huffcorzine L, Downey RG. Reasons for eating—an exploratory cognitive analysis. Ecol Food Nutr. 1992; 28: 171–189.
  81. 81. Rappoport L, Peters GR, Downey R, McCann T, Huff-Corzine L. Gender and age differences in food cognition. Appetite. 1993; 20: 33–52. pmid:8452376
  82. 82. Regnier F. Diet Between Pleasure(s) and Necessity(ies) in France and in the United-States: Some Variations in Women's Magazines since the 1930's. Sociologie et Societes. 2014; 46: 85–108.
  83. 83. Roininen K, Lahteenmaki L, Tuorila H. Quantification of consumer attitudes to health and hedonic characteristics of foods. Appetite. 1999; 33: 71–88. pmid:10447981
  84. 84. Roininen K, Tuorila H. Health and taste attitudes in the prediction of use frequency and choice between less healthy and more healthy snacks. Food Qual Prefer. 1999; 10: 357–365.
  85. 85. Roininen K, Lahteenmaki L, Tuorila H. An application of means-end chain approach to consumers' orientation to health and hedonic characteristics of foods. Ecol Food Nutr. 2000; 39: 61–81.
  86. 86. Roininen K, Tuorila H, Zandstra EH, de Graaf C, Vehkalahti K, Stubenitsky K, et al. Differences in health and taste attitudes and reported behaviour among Finnish, Dutch and British consumers: a cross-national validation of the Health and Taste Attitude Scales (HTAS). Appetite. 2001; 37: 33–45. pmid:11562156
  87. 87. Rozin P, Bauer R, Catanese D. Food and life, pleasure and worry, among American college students: Gender differences and regional similarities. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003; 85: 132–141. pmid:12872889
  88. 88. Somers J, Worsley A, McNaughton SA. The association of mavenism and pleasure with food involvement in older adults. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014; 11. pmid:24885765
  89. 89. Yang ECL, Khoo-Lattimore C. Food and the Perception of Eating: The Case of Young Taiwanese Consumers. Asia Pac J Tour Res. 2015; 20: 1545–1564.
  90. 90. Potestio KG. Is Healthy Happy? The Affective Impact of the Renton Menu Labeling Project in an Adolescent Population. M.Sc. Thesis. Ann Arbor: University of Washington; 2012.
  91. 91. Ray Chaudhury S. Painstaking pleasurable consumption: Countering the elitism of pleasurable consumption through civic-minded consumer practice. Ph.D. Ann Arbor: New Mexico State University; 2010.
  92. 92. West D. Sex differences in images of pleasure: A study of food preferences and consumption behavior M.Sc. Thesis. Ann Arbor: Concordia University (Canada); 2004.
  93. 93. Centre de recherche et d'informations nutritionnelles (CERIN). Bien manger—plaisir et équilibre [Internet]. 2018 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://data.over-blog-kiwi.com/1/39/08/08/20181002/ob_40f683_bien-manger-plaisir-equilibre.pdf.
  94. 94. Gouvernement du Canada. Tendances mondiales de consommation: Expériences sensorielles des aliments [Internet]. 2011 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://publications.gc.ca/collections/collection_2011/agr/A74-2-12-2011-fra.pdf.
  95. 95. Victoria State Government. Children's diet—fruit and vegetables [Internet]. 2011 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.betterhealth.vic.gov.au/health/healthyliving/childrens-diet-fruit-and-vegetables.
  96. 96. Victoria State Government. Fruit and vegetables [Internet]. 2011 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.betterhealth.vic.gov.au/health/healthyliving/fruit-and-vegetables.
  97. 97. Australian Government. How to understand food labels [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.eatforhealth.gov.au/eating-well/how-understand-food-labels.
  98. 98. Victoria State Government. Seniors celebrating good eating: Food for enjoyment and good health [Internet]. 2012 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.seniorsonline.vic.gov.au/-/media/seniors/files/services-and-information/seniors-celebrating-good-eating-guide—october-2012—web-version-pdf.pdf.
  99. 99. République Française. Inégalités sociales et alimentation [Internet]. 2014 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://agriculture.gouv.fr/telecharger/71709?token=8fb7e768a877834611579a7fe822eb37.
  100. 100. République Française. Atelier cuisine et alimentation durable [Internet]. 2010 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://www.loire.gouv.fr/IMG/pdf/guide_methodo_cuisines_pedagogiques.pdf.
  101. 101. République Française. Éducation alimentaire de la jeunesse [Internet]. 2018 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://agriculture.gouv.fr/telecharger/95650?token=3f088313ab7594465027345a615e8cd8.
  102. 102. République Française. Alimentation et insertion: Guide d'accompagnement du calendrier "Tous à table!" [Internet]. 2005 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://solidarites-sante.gouv.fr/IMG/pdf/Guide_d_accompagnement_du_calendrier_Tous_a_table.pdf.
  103. 103. République Française. Outils d'aide à la valorisation du service de restauration collective des établissements scolaires de la maternelle au lycée [Internet]. 2013 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://draaf.auvergne-rhone-alpes.agriculture.gouv.fr/IMG/pdf/KitDeComDRAAFAuv_RestoScol_01_Guide_VF2_cle8c7fd2.pdf.
  104. 104. République Française. Qualité gustative des aliments et environnement des repas: restauration scolaire, hospitalière et aide alimentaire [Internet]. 2010 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://agriculture.gouv.fr/telecharger/44370?token=69608087556a212b7e4851f2a8516c85.
  105. 105. Gouvernement du Québec. Bien manger pour mieux grandir [Internet]. 2009 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.mapaq.gouv.qc.ca/SiteCollectionDocuments/Mesaliments/FasciculeE.pdf.
  106. 106. Institut national de la recherche agronomique (INRA). Les comportements alimentaires: Quels en sont les déterminants? Quelles actions, pour quels effets? [Internet]. 2010 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www6.paris.inra.fr/depe/content/download/3441/33431/version/1/file/Comportements+Alimentaires-Rapport+Complet+276+pages.pdf.
  107. 107. Government of Canada. Eat well to age well [Internet]. 2004 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://publications.gc.ca/collections/collection_2011/sc-hc/H71-4-1-17-3-eng.pdf.
  108. 108. United States Department of Agriculture. Enjoy your food, but eat less [Internet]. 2016 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://choosemyplate-prod.azureedge.net/sites/default/files/tentips/DGTipsheet18EnjoyYourFood.pdf.
  109. 109. Le Bel JL. Investigation into the experience of pleasure: Intensity, its relationship to consumption behavior, and moderators thereof.Ph.D. Ann Arbor: McGill University (Canada); 2000.
  110. 110. Absolom S, Roberts A. Connecting with others: The meaning of social eating as an everyday occupation for young people. J Occup Sci. 2011; 18: 339–346.
  111. 111. Prior A-L, Limbert C. Adolescents' perceptions and experiences of family meals. J Child Health Care. 2013; 17: 354–365. pmid:23455875
  112. 112. Remick AK, Pliner P, McLean KC. The relationship between restrained eating, pleasure associated with eating, and well-being re-visited. Eat Behav. 2009; 10: 42–44. pmid:19171316
  113. 113. Government of South Australia. Nutrition information fact sheet: Healthy food, happy kids [Internet]. 2009 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://www.wch.sa.gov.au/services/az/other/nutrition/documents/health_food_happy_kids.pdf.
  114. 114. Government of Canada. Canada's Food Guide: Canada's Dietary Guidelines [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://food-guide.canada.ca/static/assets/pdf/CDG-EN-2018.pdf.
  115. 115. Government of Canada. Canada's Food Guide: Eat meals with others [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://food-guide.canada.ca/en/healthy-eating-recommendations/eat-meals-with-others/.
  116. 116. Gouvernement du Canada. Bien manger avec le Guide Alimentaire Canadien: Ressource à l'intention des éducateurs et communicateurs [Internet]. 2007 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://publications.gc.ca/collections/Collection/H164-38-2-2007F.pdf.
  117. 117. Government of Canada. Canada's Food Guide: Take time to eat [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://food-guide.canada.ca/en/healthy-eating-recommendations/be-mindful-of-your-eating-habits/take-time-to-eat/.
  118. 118. République Française. Programme de formation des enseignants sur les classes du goût [Internet]. 2011 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://agriculture.gouv.fr/sites/minagri/files/documents/alimentation/pdf/Classesdugout-290911_cle07ff18.pdf.
  119. 119. République Française. Consommateur stratège [Internet]. 2016 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://agriculture.gouv.fr/telecharger/84025?token=d36aee989e92958bd5f2560c81b90f97.
  120. 120. République Française. Faire soi-même [Internet]. 2016 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://agriculture.gouv.fr/telecharger/84027?token=594887e0b67e5c918c16c0c35c460071.
  121. 121. République Française. Programme national nutrition-santé. La santé vient en mangeant: Le guide alimentaire pour tous [Internet]. 2002 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://solidarites-sante.gouv.fr/IMG/pdf/guide_alimentairetous.pdf.
  122. 122. Harvard School of Public Health Prevention Research Center (HPRC). Food & Fun after school: Change is good! [Internet]. 2011 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://www.foodandfun.org/.
  123. 123. NIH: National Institute on Anging. Healthy eating: Choosing Healthy Meals As You Get Older [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.nia.nih.gov/health/choosing-healthy-meals-you-get-older.
  124. 124. United States Department of Agriculture. 10 Tips: Cut Back on Your Kid’s Sweet Treats [Internet]. 2016 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.choosemyplate.gov/ten-tips-cut-back-on-sweet-treats.
  125. 125. Huang Y, Wu J. Food pleasure orientation diminishes the "healthy = less tasty" intuition. Food Qual Prefer. 2016; 54: 75–78.
  126. 126. Smith T, Hawks SR. Intuitive Eating, Diet Composition, and the Meaning of Food in Healthy Weight Promotion. Am J Health Educ. 2006; 37: 130–136.
  127. 127. Hansen T. The effect of physical surroundings in usage situations on consumer perception of food quality and on consumer emotions. J Int Consum Mark. 2002; 15: 31–51.
  128. 128. Monnery-Patris S, Marty L, Bayer F, Nicklaus S, Chambaron S. Explicit and implicit tasks for assessing hedonic-versus nutrition-based attitudes towards food in French children. Appetite. 2016; 96: 580–587. pmid:26522508
  129. 129. Niva M. 'All foods affect health': Understandings of functional foods and healthy eating among health-oriented Finns. Appetite. 2007; 48: 384–393. pmid:17166625
  130. 130. Wahl DR, Villinger K, Konig LM, Ziesemer K, Schupp HT, Renner B. Healthy food choices are happy food choices: Evidence from a real life sample using smartphone based assessments. Sci Rep. 2017; 7. pmid:29213109
  131. 131. Baek U. The impact of customer involvement in the cooking process on dining satisfaction. M.Sc. Thesis. Ann Arbor: Purdue University; 2009.
  132. 132. Australian Government. Discretionary food and drink choices [Internet]. 2017 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.eatforhealth.gov.au/food-essentials/discretionary-food-and-drink-choices.
  133. 133. Bissell P, Peacock M, Blackburn J, Smith C. The discordant pleasures of everyday eating: Reflections on the social gradient in obesity under neo-liberalism. Soc Sci Med. 2016; 159: 14–21. pmid:27155225
  134. 134. République Française. L’évolution de l’alimentation en France [Internet]. 2012 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: http://agreste.agriculture.gouv.fr/IMG/pdf/doctravail50112.pdf.
  135. 135. Gouvernement du Canada. Amélioration des compétences culinaires: Synthèse des données probantes et des leçons pouvant orienter l'élaboration de programmes et de politiques [Internet]. 2010 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.canada.ca/fr/sante-canada/services/publications/aliments-et-nutrition/amelioration-competences-culinaires-synthese-donnees-probantes-leacons-pouvant-orienter-elaboration-programmes-politiques.html.
  136. 136. Ensaff H, Crawford R, Russell JM, Barker ME. Preparing and sharing food: a quantitative analysis of a primary school-based food intervention. J Public Health. 2017; 39: 567–573. pmid:27591297
  137. 137. Yang CL, Khoo-Lattimore C, Lai MY. Eat to Live or Live to Eat? Mapping Food and Eating Perception of Malaysian Chinese. J Hospit Market Manag. 2014; 23: 579.
  138. 138. Goutons un monde meilleur. Mieux manger en 6 gestes [Internet]. 2011 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.alimenterre.org/system/files/ressources/pdf/292_mieux-manger-en-6-gestes.pdf.
  139. 139. République Française. Recommandations sur l'alimentation, l'activité physique & la sédentarité pour les adultes [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://pole-sante.creps-vichy.sports.gouv.fr/wp-content/uploads/2019/02/DP-Reco-Nutritionnelles-220119.pdf.
  140. 140. Arch JJ, Brown KW, Goodman RJ, Della Porta MD, Kiken LG, Tillman S. Enjoying food without caloric cost: The impact of brief mindfulness on laboratory eating outcomes. Behav Res Ther. 2016; 79: 23–34. pmid:26945478
  141. 141. Hong PY, Lishner DA, Han KH. Mindfulness and eating: An experiment examining the effect of mindful raisin eating on the enjoyment of sampled food. Mindfulness. 2014; 5: 80–87.
  142. 142. Vogel E, Mol A. Enjoy your food: on losing weight and taking pleasure. Sociol Health Illn. 2014; 36: 305–317. pmid:24528307
  143. 143. Australian Government. Choosing nutritious foods [Internet]. 2015 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.eatforhealth.gov.au/eating-well/tips-eating-well/choosing-nutritious-foods.
  144. 144. Government of Canada. Canada's Food Guide: Healthy eating for holidays and events [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://food-guide.canada.ca/en/tips-for-healthy-eating/holidays-and-events/.
  145. 145. Jackson B, Cooper M, Mintz L, Albino A. Motivations to eat: Scale development and validation. J Res Pers. 2003; 37: 297–318.
  146. 146. Thogersen-Ntoumani C, Ntoumanis N, Barkoukis V, Spray CM. The role of motivation to eat in the prediction of weight control behaviors in female and male adolescents. Eat Behav. 2009; 10: 107–114. pmid:19447352
  147. 147. Robinson E, Blissett J, Higgs S. Recall of vegetable eating affects future predicted enjoyment and choice of vegetables in British University undergraduate students. J Am Diet Assoc. 2011; 111: 1543–1548. pmid:21963021
  148. 148. Robinson E, Blissett J, Higgs S. Changing memory of food enjoyment to increase food liking, choice and intake. Br J Nutr. 2012; 108: 1505–1510. pmid:22214529
  149. 149. Cabanac M. Physiological role of pleasure. Science. 1971; 173: 1103–1107. pmid:5098954
  150. 150. McClain AD, Pentz MA, Nguyen-Rodriguez ST, Shin H-S, Riggs NR, Spruijt-Metz D. Measuring the meanings of eating in minority youth. Eat Behav. 2011; 12: 277–283. pmid:22051360
  151. 151. Hur J, Jang S. Anticipated guilt and pleasure in a healthy food consumption context. Int J Hosp Manag. 2015; 48: 113–123.
  152. 152. Vinai P, Vinai L, Vinai P, Bruno C, Studt S, Cardetti S, et al. "The next mouthful will be the best": Influence of prevision of the pleasure on the decision of having a second helping of a just eaten food. Eat Weight Disord. 2016; 21: 453–457. pmid:26604201
  153. 153. République Française. Ministère de l'éducation nationale et de la jeunesse. Éducation à l'alimentation et au goût [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://www.education.gouv.fr/cid115024/education-nutritionnelle.html.
  154. 154. Otake K, Kato K. Subjective happiness and emotional responsiveness to food stimuli. J Happiness Stud. 2017; 18: 691–708.
  155. 155. Government of Canada. Canada's Food Guide: Cultures, food traditions and healthy eating [Internet]. 2019 [cited 23 June 2020]. Available from: https://food-guide.canada.ca/en/healthy-eating-recommendations/enjoy-your-food/cultures-food-traditions-and-healthy-eating/.
  156. 156. Appleton K, McGowan L. The relationship between restrained eating and poor psychological health is moderated by pleasure normally associated with eating. Eat Behav. 2006; 7: 342–347. pmid:17056410
  157. 157. Hunot C, Fildes A, Croker H, Llewellyn CH, Wardle J, Beeken RJ. Appetitive traits and relationships with BMI in adults: Development of the Adult Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. Appetite. 2016; 105: 356–363. pmid:27215837
  158. 158. Loh DA, Moy FM, Zaharan NL, Mohamed Z. Eating Behaviour among Multi-Ethnic Adolescents in a Middle-Income Country as Measured by the Self-Reported Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (Eating Behaviour among Malaysian Adolescents). PloS One. 2013; 8: e82885. pmid:24349385
  159. 159. Hege MA, Veit R, Krumsiek J, Kullmann S, Heni M, Rogers PJ, et al. Eating less or more—Mindset induced changes in neural correlates of pre-meal planning. Appetite. 2018; 125: 492–501. pmid:29524474
  160. 160. Adam M, Young-Wolff KC, Konar E, Winkleby M. Massive open online nutrition and cooking course for improved eating behaviors and meal composition. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015; 12. pmid:26630879
  161. 161. Gravel K, St-Hilaire GO, Deslauriers A, Watiez M, Dumont M, Bouchard AAD, et al. Effect of sensory-based intervention on the increased use of food-related descriptive terms among restrained eaters. Food Qual Prefer. 2014; 32: 271–276.
  162. 162. Block LG, Grier SA. From Nutrients to Nurturance: A Conceptual Introduction to Food Well-Being. J Public Policy Mark. 2011; 30: 5–13.
  163. 163. Zurawicki L. Pleasures of the palate from the consumer marketing perspective. Food Res Int. 2015; 76: 200–206.
  164. 164. Lovell-Smith D, Kenealy T, Buetow S. Eating when empty is good for your health. Med Hypotheses. 2010; 75: 172–178. pmid:20227835
  165. 165. Phull S, Wills W, Dickinson A. Is It a Pleasure to Eat Together? Theoretical Reflections on Conviviality and the Mediterranean Diet. Sociol Compass. 2015; 9: 977–986.
  166. 166. Zheng H, Berthoud H-R. Eating for pleasure or calories. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2007; 7: 607–612. pmid:18037344
  167. 167. Mela DJ. Eating for pleasure or just wanting to eat? Reconsidering sensory hedonic responses as a driver of obesity. Appetite. 2006; 47: 10–17. pmid:16647788
  168. 168. Marty L, Chambaron S, Nicklaus S, Monnery-Patris S. Learned pleasure from eating: An opportunity to promote healthy eating in children? Appetite. 2018; 120: 265–274. pmid:28890391
  169. 169. Muzaffar H, Metcalfe JJ, Fiese B. Narrative Review of Culinary Interventions with Children in Schools to Promote Healthy Eating: Directions for Future Research and Practice. Curr Dev Nutr. 2018; 2. pmid:29955728
  170. 170. Warren JM, Smith N, Ashwell M. A structured literature review on the role of mindfulness, mindful eating and intuitive eating in changing eating behaviours: effectiveness and associated potential mechanisms. Cambridge University Press. 2017; 30: 272–283.
  171. 171. Higgs S. Cognitive influences on food intake: The effects of manipulating memory for recent eating. Physiol Behav. 2008; 94: 734–739. pmid:18486159
  172. 172. Menneteau U, Kureta-Vanoli K. Le goût, un outil dans la prise en charge des patients obèses ou en surpoids, Taste, a useful tool in the management of obese or overweight patients. Obésité. 2009; 4: 120–125.
  173. 173. Sackner-Bernstein J, Kanter D, Kaul S. Dietary Intervention for Overweight and Obese Adults: Comparison of Low-Carbohydrate and Low-Fat Diets. A Meta-Analysis. PloS one. 2015; 10: e0139817. pmid:26485706
  174. 174. Johnston BC, Kanters S, Bandayrel K, Wu P, Naji F, Siemieniuk RA, et al. Comparison of weight loss among named diet programs in overweight and obese adults: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2014; 312: 923–933. pmid:25182101
  175. 175. Clifford D, Ozier A, Bundros J, Moore J, Kreiser A, Morris MN. Impact of Non-Diet Approaches on Attitudes, Behaviors, and Health Outcomes: A Systematic Review. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2015; 47: 143–155. pmid:25754299
  176. 176. Li SS, Blanco Mejia S, Lytvyn L, Stewart SE, Viguiliouk E, Ha V, et al. Effect of Plant Protein on Blood Lipids: A Systematic Review and Meta‐Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials. J Am Heart Assoc. 2017; 6. pmid:29263032
  177. 177. Kiefer I, Rathmanner T, Kunze M. Eating and dieting differences in men and women. J Mens Health Gend. 2005; 2: 194–201.
  178. 178. Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O'Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and Explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018; 169: 467. pmid:30178033