Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Prevalence and related factors of Active and Healthy Ageing in Europe according to two models: Results from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE)

  • Cristina Bosch-Farré ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Cristina Bosch-Farré, Josep Garre-Olmo, Maria Carme Malagón-Aguilera, Dolors Juvinyà-Canal

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Health and Healthcare Research Group, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain, Nursing Department, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain

  • Josep Garre-Olmo ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Cristina Bosch-Farré, Josep Garre-Olmo, Maria Carme Malagón-Aguilera, Dolors Juvinyà-Canal

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    josep.garre@ias.cat

    Affiliations Girona Biomedical Research Institute (IDIBGI), Salt, Catalonia, Spain, Healthcare Institute (IAS), Salt, Catalonia, Spain, Department of Medical Sciences, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain

  • Anna Bonmatí-Tomàs ,

    Roles Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Nursing Department, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain

  • Maria Carme Malagón-Aguilera ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Cristina Bosch-Farré, Josep Garre-Olmo, Maria Carme Malagón-Aguilera, Dolors Juvinyà-Canal

    Roles Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Health and Healthcare Research Group, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain, Nursing Department, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain

  • Sandra Gelabert-Vilella ,

    Roles Resources, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Nursing Department, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain

  • Concepció Fuentes-Pumarola ,

    Roles Resources, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliations Health and Healthcare Research Group, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain, Nursing Department, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain

  • Dolors Juvinyà-Canal

    Contributed equally to this work with: Cristina Bosch-Farré, Josep Garre-Olmo, Maria Carme Malagón-Aguilera, Dolors Juvinyà-Canal

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Health and Healthcare Research Group, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain, Nursing Department, University of Girona, Girona, Catalonia, Spain

Abstract

Background

Active and Healthy Ageing (AHA) is the process of optimizing opportunities related to health, participation, and safety in order to improve quality of life. The approach most often used to measure AHA is Rowe and Kahn’s Satisfactory Ageing model. Nonetheless, this model has limitations. One of the strategic objectives of the WHO Global Strategy and Action Plan (2016) is to improve Healthy Ageing measurement. Our objectives were to compare two models of assessing AHA and further compare the results by country and sociodemographic variables.

Methods

This was a cross-sectional, observational analysis of a representative sample of the general population aged 50 years and older in Europe. The data analysed were obtained by the Study of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). The dependent variable was AHA and its dimensions, measured using the Rowe and Kahn AHA model (AHA-B) and the authors’ model based on the WHO definition (AHA-BPS). A descriptive analysis and multivariate models of binary logistical regression were developed.

Results

The sample consisted of 52,641 participants (mean age 65.24 years [SD = 10.18; Range = 50–104], 53.2% women). Healthy Ageing prevalence in the AHA-B model was 23.5% (95%CI = 23.1%-23.9%). In the AHA-BPS model, this prevalence was 38.9%. In both models, significant variations were observed between countries, and were distributed along a north-western to south-eastern gradient. The sociodemographic variables associated with the absence of AHA were advanced age, female sex, death of spouse, low educational level, lack of employment, and low financial status. Comparing the two models, the strength of association between absence of AHA and advanced age (85 years and older) was four times greater in the AHA-B model.

Conclusions

Our results showing differences between these two models provide evidence that the AHA-BPS model does not penalize older age and is more likely to characterize AHA from a health promotion perspective.

Introduction

The ageing of the world’s population has significant consequences in areas such as the labour market, pensions, health provisions, housing and social services [1,2] that can be seen as threatening the level of social welfare provided for older people. However, the older population is an important part of a society’s economic, social, and cultural capital. In this regard, the World Health Organization (WHO) and other global organizations emphasize the promotion of active and healthy ageing (AHA) among their priorities [36]. According to the WHO Strategy and action plan for healthy ageing in Europe 2012–2020, AHA can contribute to making health and well-being systems sustainable, especially by encouraging older people to stay active, independent, and fully integrated into society [6].

Nonetheless, a uniform definition of AHA is still lacking, which gives rise to ambiguity in its operationalization, causing heterogeneity of results [7,8] and multiple models of analysis that emphasize either biomedical or psychosocial aspects [710]. Cosco et al. described 105 ways of operationalizing the AHA that produced a range of prevalence from 0.4% to 91.7% [7]. The more flexible the model, the higher the prevalence of AHA, and the inverse was true: models that applied more restrictive criteria showed a lower AHA prevalence.

One of the most widely used biomedical models of AHA in the scientific literature is the Successful Ageing Model developed by Rowe and Kahn [11]. It includes 3 domains: 1) low probability of disease and disability; 2) maintenance of high physical and cognitive functioning; 3) engagement in social and productive activities. Despite the incorporation of the social dimension, this model is considered predominantly biomedical because its definition of AHA requires a physical criterion of no disease or disability [1214]. According to this model, the prevalence of AHA in European studies ranges from 1.6% to 21.1% [15]. The Rowe and Kahn model has been applied and adapted by numerous authors [10,13,1518].

The biomedical AHA model has created several controversies [8]. Some authors believe the presence of disease and disability to be a limiting factor in categorizing AHA [12,13,1921]. The model also does not consider certain dimensions, especially mental well-being [7,8,12,22], and subjective variables that contribute to AHA [10,2226]. For this reason, WHO advocates for a multidimensional AHA construct with a positive and bio-psycho-social (BPS) perspective that defines AHA as the process of optimizing opportunities for health, participation, and security, with the aim of increasing healthy life expectancy and quality of life [6]. The proposals to measure AHA based on the WHO definition include the 6 Factor Model by Paúl et al. (personal factors, behaviour determinants, determinants of social environment; determinants of health and social services; determinants of physical environment; economic determinants), which the authors were unable to validate empirically in their study [27]; the Active Ageing Index (AAI) developed by the European Commission, which emphasizes social engagement and measures AHA at national and subnational levels based on general population variables rather than individual characteristics, and therefore provides only an overview of ageing by country [28]; and the European Innovation Partnership on Active Healthy Ageing (INAHA) within the framework of the Europe 2020 strategy, an effort to operationalize AHA with tools such as WHODAS 2.0 (the revised WHO Disability Assessment Schedule) and Health-Related Quality of Life (HRQoL) [29]. WHODAS 2.0 measures disability and does not take a positive ‘healthy ageing’ perspective.

Although many AHA models exist, few are focussed specifically on the definition proposed by WHO, respecting the intrinsic AHA concepts of that definition and adopting a holistic and positive perspective on health. According to the Global Strategy and Action Plan (2016), which urges the establishment of the required evidence, one area to be addressed is the improvement of AHA evaluation, monitoring, and research [30]. The AHA-BPS model allows a detailed analysis of ageing using a database that is representative of the European population, together with a longitudinal study design that permits the definition of baseline values from which to monitor the evolution of AHA status and the impact of interventions in favour of AHA. A strength of this approach is the possibility to validate the model and obtain an instrument to assess AHA at the individual level.

As the WHO report points out, the extent of the opportunities that arise from increasing longevity will be heavily dependent on one key factor: the health of these older populations. If people are experiencing these extra years in good health and live in a supportive environment, their ability to do the things they value will have few limits [30]. Therefore, a comprehensive response is urgently needed to promote healthy ageing. The lack of a consensus definition of AHA has been a great weakness for researchers [7]. The objective of this project was to develop an AHA model in accordance with the WHO principles, measure the prevalence of AHA in 14 European countries, and compare the findings with the results obtained using the biomedical model.

Material and methods

Design

This was an analytical observational cross-sectional study.

Population and sample

The 2013 data analysed came from the fifth wave of the Study of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE), which assessed the health, socioeconomic status, and social and family networks of community-dwelling people aged 50 and older in 15 countries (DOI: 10.6103/SHARE.w5.100) [31]. The exclusion criteria were residence in Israel or the region of Girona and missing data for any of the key variables in the AHA model (Fig 1). Despite the imputation strategies applied by SHARE researchers to minimize missing values, participants with at least one missing variable constituted 9.43% of eligible individuals (n = 5,480). Table in S1 Table shows the distribution of the final sample of 52,641 participants across 14 countries and the proportion of missing data per country.

Procedure

SHARE is based on an in-home computer-aided personal interview (CAPI) that includes 22 modules collecting information on sociodemographic variables, physical and mental health, biomarkers, psychological and economic status, and social and family networks. The methodology, fieldwork procedures, and database have been previously described [32,33].

Assessment of AHA

Initially, AHA was assessed with a biomedical model based on the Rowe and Kahn model (AHA-B) and the biopsychosocial AHA definition based on the WHO criteria (AHA-BPS). The variables to characterize the two models were extracted from 7 of the 22 SHARE modules: physical health, cognitive function, handgrip strength, mental health, social support, activities, and financial resources.

The AHA-B model was operationalized according to McLaughlin et al. [17], as modified by Hank according to the variables available to SHARE [15]. A person fulfils AHA criteria if 3 conditions are met: (a) no disease, no disability; (b) high cognitive and physical functioning; (c) active engagement in society. Each condition is operationalized with the following indicators [15]:

  1. Absence of major diseases. No doctor has ever told the respondents they have cancer, chronic lung disease, diabetes, heart disease, or stroke, or they had no scores of 4 or higher in the EURO-D depression scale [34, 35]. Absence of disability. The respondents reported no limitation in basic activities of daily living (ADL): walking indoors, dressing, bathing or showering, eating, getting into or out of bed, toileting.
  2. A high level of cognitive function was considered to be an above-average total score on all tests of mental orientation, memory, and mathematical calculations (maximum score, 29 points): correctly state the day of the week, date, month, and year (1 point for each); remember list of 10 words, immediate and delayed recall (1 point for each); complete 5 consecutive mathematical calculations (subtractions, 1 point per answer). High level of physical function was recorded if the person had difficulty with no more than one of the following six activities: climb a full flight of stairs, climb several flights of stairs, lift more than 5 kg, lean / squat / kneel, push large objects, walk 100 meters.
  3. Social engagement included paid work or volunteering in the previous month or caring for grandchildren at some time during the past 12 months. Social support was defined as living with a partner, providing economic or household help to relatives, friends, or neighbours, or engaging in a sport in the previous month.

The AHA-BPS model also assesses three dimensions, each using two indicators. The WHO definition includes physical, mental, and social well-being dimensions, operationalized as follows:

  1. Physical well-being. The two indicators were non-frail or pre-frail according to the SHARE-FI scale [36] and good cognitive function, defined as meeting the orientation criterion (AHA-B, above) and being above the 10th percentile (p10), age-adjusted (age groups: 50–64, 65–74, 75–84, and 85 years and older) in at least 3 of 4 cognitive tests (immediate memory, delayed memory, mathematical calculation, verbal flow).
  2. Mental well-being. The two indicators were self-reported satisfaction with life (defined as 7 or higher on a 0–10 scale) and no depressive symptoms (defined as having 3 or fewer symptoms on the Euro-D scale) [34].
  3. Social well-being. The two indicators were social participation, defined as any paid work, caring for grandchildren, and social activities (sports, training, religion, politics, volunteering) and a satisfaction score of 7 or higher on a 0–10 scale in the social activity performed and social support, including social network (i.e., having given or received financial or household help) and family support. For people with children or a partner, family support was defined as living with a partner or child or living within 5km of a son or daughter and a score <5 (never or seldom feeling alone) on the Revised University of California-Los Angeles (R-UCLA) Loneliness Scale [37]. For those without a partner or children, only the subjective criterion (R-UCLA <5) was measured.

The variables drawn from the SHARE database are shown in Supplementary S2 Table (S2 Table. Coding of dependent and independent variables selected from the SHARE database).

Independent variables

The study analysed seven sociodemographic variables: 1) age and age group (50–64, 65–74, 75–84, 85 years and older; 55 years and older; 65 years and older); 2) sex; 3) marital status (married or stable partner, single, divorced or separated, widowed); 4) educational level according to International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED-97), categorized as low (less than secondary education), moderate (completed secondary education) and high (some postsecondary education) [38]; 5) employment status (retired, actively employed or self-employed, and other, such as unemployment, homemaker, or disabled); 6) economic status related to perception of income adequacy (reaches the end of the month without difficulty [easily or more than easily], with some difficulty, or with great difficulty); and 7) country of residence (Austria, Belgium, Denmark, Estonia, France, Germany, The Netherlands, Italy, Luxembourg, Slovenia, Spain, Sweden, Switzerland, Czech Republic).

Statistical analysis

The prevalence of AHA in both models was determined by relative frequencies with 95% confidence intervals (95%CI), stratified by age group and country. The prevalence of AHA in the two models was compared and, according to the different dimensions, stratified by sex, age group, and socioeconomic status. Differences in the prevalence of AHA dimensions were compared with Chi-square tests for age groups and geographical locations. To determine if the differences between the two models were statistically significant, the relative frequencies (Chi-square test) and the odds ratio were calculated. Weighted statistical samples were used to minimize the potential for selection bias in different countries [33]. Multivariate logistic regression was used to analyse the strength of the association in both models between AHA and the sociodemographic variables. Results are expressed as absolute numbers and percentages, means, standard deviations (SD), odds ratios (OR), and 95% confidence intervals (CI). Statistical tests were considered to be significant with a two-tailed p value < 0.05. The data were analysed using SPSS.19.

Ethics statement

Use of the SHARE data (5th wave) was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Max Planck Society for the Progress of Science. SHARE-ERIC's activities related to human subjects research are guided by international research ethics principles such as the Respect Code of Practice for Socio-Economic Research and the Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

Mean age was 65.24 years (SD = 10.18; Range = 50–104); 52.6% of participants were aged 50–64 years, followed by 65–74 years (26.8%), 75–84 years (16.1%), and 85 years and older (4.5%). Women were 53.2% of the sample. Most participants (72.1%) were married, and 21.8% had a high educational level, 38.3% a moderate level, and 39.9% a low level. While 48.7% of participants were retired, 33.3% were actively employed. When asked about making ends meet, or “getting to the end of the month”, 33.0% reported having economic difficulty (23.3% some difficulty and 9.7% great difficulty). In supplementary information, Table in S3 Table shows a summary of participant characteristics, by country.

The overall prevalence of AHA was 23.5% (95%CI = 23.1%-23.9%) in the AHA-B model and 38.9% (95%CI = 38.5%-39.3%) in the AHA-BPS model. Fig 2 shows the percentages of participants that fulfilled each dimension and, for the AHA-BPS model, the results for the combination of each pair of dimensions. The criteria that were least often met were physical and cognitive functioning in the AHA-B model and mental well-being in the AHA-BPS model.

When the models were compared, the degree of correlation was low (Kappa Index = 0.475). In the distribution of relative frequencies, 19.6% of participants reported AHA according to both the AHA-B model and the AHA-BPS model; in contrast, 57.2% of people who did not meet AHA criteria according to the AHA-B model also did not meet them using the AHA-BPS model. According to the AHA-BPS model, 21,689 individuals reported AHA, 8,428 (63.55%) more than the 13,261 identified by the AHA-B model (Table 1).

Tables 2 and 3 show the prevalence of each component of the operational definition of the AHA models, stratified by criteria and by age groups. In both models, the older the age group, the less likely were they to report AHA. This decline was more pronounced after 75 years of age; in the AHA-B model, 34.3% of people aged 50–64 years had AHA, compared to 1.3% of people aged 85 and older. Overall, the criterion that most inhibited AHA was disease or disability (45.0%). In those aged 75 and older, two criteria were most often not met: social participation and high cognition. In the AHA-BPS model, which does not include the disease criterion, the dimension that decreased most with age was social welfare, while the dimension that remained more stable was mental well-being.

thumbnail
Table 2. Prevalence of AHA in AHA-B model, by dimensions and age groups *.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206353.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Prevalence of AHA in AHA-BPS model, by dimensions and age groups *.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206353.t003

Stratified by country, AHA prevalence ranged from 14.1% in Spain to 39.5% in Switzerland in the AHA-B model. In contrast, using the AHA-BPS model, the prevalence ranged from 23.7% in Estonia to 61.2% in Denmark. In both models, the countries with the highest AHA prevalence were located in northern and western Europe. In the AHA-BPS model, more than half of the northern population reported AHA, compared to approximately a quarter of the participants in Estonia and Spain (23.7% and 27.9%, respectively). (Fig 3)

Analysed by country, less than half of the participants (46.6%) in Estonia met the criteria of dimension 1 (low probability of disease and disability) in the AHA-B model, compared to 67.9% in Switzerland. On dimension 2 (high cognitive and physical function), the lowest percentage (31.2%) was found in Spain, in contrast to Switzerland, where 70.9% of participants met the criteria. Likewise, less than half of the Spanish participants (44.7%) met the criteria for dimension 3 (participation in society), compared to 75% in Denmark. It should be noted that Spain scored low (55.5%) on the physical well-being dimension in the AHA-BPS model because it includes cognitive function and the Spanish participants again scored lower on that criterion in the AHA-B model (57.9%). Table 4 summarizes the AHA results for each model in each country, grouped by European geographic region according to Eurovoc and defined as follows: ‘northern’ (Denmark, Sweden, and Estonia), ‘eastern’ (Czech Republic and Slovenia), ‘southern’ (Italy and Spain); and ‘western’ (Austria, Belgium, France, Germany, Luxembourg, Holland / The Netherlands, and Switzerland) [39]. In the northern region, 8 out of 10 people had high cognitive function. Regarding mental well-being, the differences between countries were mainly based on life satisfaction. The countries of southern and eastern Europe scored lowest in the social welfare dimension, mainly due to the social participation criterion. In S4 Table shows a summary of AHA prevalence in both models, by country.

thumbnail
Table 4. Prevalence of AHA in both models, by the geographical location of Europe *.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206353.t004

Table 5 presents the results for participants who met AHA criteria in each model, stratified by sociodemographic variables. In both models, the prevalence of AHA was greater among younger participants, men, and those with more education, some employment, and without economic difficulty getting to the end of the month. In both models, those who were widowed had the lowest AHA prevalence.

thumbnail
Table 5. Percentage of participants with AHA, stratified by model and sociodemographic variables *.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206353.t005

Table 6 includes odds ratios (OR) to show the strength of the association between a lack of AHA in each model and the main demographic subgroups. Using the AHA-B model, people aged 85 years and older were less likely to report AHA (OR = 15.271, 95%CI: 15.151–15.393) than those aged 75–84 years (OR = 3.843, 95%CI: 3.834–3.352), and those who were not working (OR = 3.459, 95%CI: 3.454–3.463) or had difficulty getting to the end of the month economically (OR = 2.149, 95%CI: 2.146–2.151). Using the AHA-BPS model, the corresponding subgroups were those aged 85 years and older (OR = 3.783, 95%CI: 3.771–3.796), not working (OR = 3.205, 95%CI: 3.202–3.209), and having economic difficulty (OR = 2.633, 95%CI: 2.630–2.635). Comparing the absence of AHA between the two models, the strength of its association with age (85 years and older) was four times greater in the AHA-B model, while economic difficulty was more strongly associated in the AHA-BPS model.

thumbnail
Table 6. Multivariate logistical regression of sociodemographic variables, by models *.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206353.t006

Discussion

This study compared two models of assessing AHA and further compared the results by country and by sociodemographic variables. The prevalence of AHA varied (15.4%) according to the operational definition used by each model [40,41], being 23.5% according to AHA-B, the more traditional biomedical model, and 38.9% according to the AHA-BPS model, based on the WHO criteria. The AHA-B model considers the presence of disease and disability a limiting factor in the achievement of AHA, and indeed 45% of the sample was excluded from the possibility of AHA in that analysis due to the presence of at least one of the six chronic diseases incorporated into the AHA-B construct. Other authors who use biomedical models have excluded nearly half of the sample due to illness [10,16,21], confirming disease and disability as the main factors limiting the categorization of AHA from a biomedical perspective [17, 20,42].

Authors who measure AHA with psychosocial or subjective dimensions have obtained a higher AHA prevalence, compared to the biomedical approach [13,21,43,44]. Similarly, people who self-report AHA may have chronic diseases [10,25,45,46]. Specifically, Parslow et al. found that those with the highest AHA scores also reported a mean of two chronic diseases [47]. Public health policies must be designed to maximize AHA among older people so that they are able to do what they want to do regardless of having a chronic disease; therefore, models of ageing should focus on function rather than disease or disability [48]. The AHA-BPS model does not include freedom from disease in its construct; rather, it measures frailty, using the SHARE-FI scale, which gives greater flexibility to AHA measurement and allows an exploration of the individual’s level of vulnerability, independent of any disease. In terms of physical health, frailty could be considered the opposite of AHA [8,49,50].

A number of studies confirm that the more flexible the AHA definition used, the more extensive is the AHA prevalence observed [12,13,19,41]. The more flexible models are more useful for public health purposes [12] because it is easier to identify areas of deficiency in multidimensional models, allowing more targeted actions in response [7]. Inui highlights the importance of applying an AHA construct with a holistic, non-reductionist perspective [51]. Nonetheless, some authors have concluded that when the definition incorporates more domains it becomes more restrictive and AHA prevalence is lower [42]. Based on our results, we concluded that the level of restriction in each domain has more impact than the number of domains considered and that the AHA-BPS model is less restrictive than the AHA-B model. Therefore, the AHA-BPS model permits the identification of vulnerable older populations requiring interventions designed to improve their health status, while the AHA-B model is less useful in public health planning because it identifies groups with better physical health, a smaller population (8,428 fewer individuals than were identified by AHA-BPS). Moreover, the AHA-BPS model contains all three dimensions included in the WHO definition of AHA (physical, mental, and social well-being), which have also been present in most other AHA models [7,9,52] and AHA qualitative studies [44,53].

Although mental well-being has been identified as a key factor in the positive perception of ageing [54], the AHA-B model considers it only as a criterion (specifically, a diagnosis of depression) in the calculation of the physical dimension that can exclude individuals from achieving AHA. Phelan and Anderson criticized this lack of a mental health dimension in the Rowe and Kahn construct [46]. In the AHA-BPS model, it is measured as a separate dimension. Psychological and social dimensions of ageing may not be positively associated with physical changes throughout life [45]. In addition, individuals may have mechanisms of compensation, resilience, and/or adaptation that help them adjust to the physical decline that is inherent in ageing and allow for AHA despite physical limitations and disease [13,45]. The person who is able to adapt to limitations and compensate for them by maximizing benefits and minimizing losses is better able to achieve “healthy ageing”: an optimal level of well-being and quality of life [45,55,56]. Therefore, psychosocial changes largely explain how old age can be a period of highly subjective well-being [57]. The findings in these earlier studies are in line with our results for the AHA-BPS model, where the most stable dimension over time was mental well-being, compared to other dimensions, which decreased with age. Perales et al. also found that the age gradient occurs in various AHA models, except in psychosocial AHA models that value subjective aspects of well-being [58]. This phenomenon is related to the so-called "subjective well-being paradox", according to which older people are not the least happy population despite having more health problems [59,60].

The AHA-BPS model showed 9.5% greater likelihood of meeting the criteria for social well-being, compared to the biomedical AHA-B model. Our AHA-B data also showed a progressive decrease in performance as age increased, a contrast with the findings by Weir et al. in a Canadian population, where the social domain varied little or not at all with age in their analysis (using the Rowe and Kahn model) [20]. In our study, social well-being was the dimension that decreased most with age in the AHA-BPS model. As other authors have observed, old age leads to considerable changes beyond biological decline, such as changes in social function and status and the need to face the loss of close relationships [3]. Loneliness is one of the social network variables most strongly associated with health [61]. The AHA-BPS model includes subjective measures. Other studies that measured psychosocial or subjective dimensions show high AHA prevalence compared to biomedical models [12,1921].

Comparing AHA prevalence by country and model, according to the AHA-B model the population aged 65 years and older scored 4.6% higher in our study than was reported by Hank in 2010 [15]. In the Hank study, Denmark had the highest AHA prevalence, followed by The Netherlands, Sweden, and Switzerland, with the lowest prevalence in Mediterranean and eastern European countries. The comparison must take into consideration Hank's analysis of survey data from Waves 1 and 2 (our study used Wave 5 data) and differences in the countries studied (Poland, Greece, and Ireland did not participate in Wave 5, but it did include Luxembourg, Estonia, and Slovenia). In addition, Hank includes data from Israel. Other studies that have applied the AHA-B model in people aged 65 and older in the United States also obtained similar prevalence data, such as 18.8% in 2002 [10] or a lower prevalence in 2010 [17]. Comparing AHA prevalence by country, the results are consistent with Hank's north-western to south-eastern (Mediterranean and eastern Europe) gradient: Denmark, The Netherlands, Sweden, and Switzerland had the highest AHA prevalence and Spain had the lowest prevalence, with Italy in the penultimate place [15]. The analysis by age groups followed the same gradient, as in other studies that emphasized physical well-being measures such as disability [62]. The overall AHA prevalence according to the AHA-BPS model was 36.9% in people 55 and older, comparable to AAI data for the population aged 55 and older in 27 European countries (not including Switzerland). In 2012 data, the best AHA scores were obtained in Sweden and Denmark, while Estonia was ranked 16th of 27 countries, higher than Spain although both scores were very similar (33.1 and 32.5, respectively) [63]. In the AHA-BPS model, Denmark had the highest AHA prevalence, well ahead of fourth-place Sweden, and Estonia was in last place while Spain occupied the penultimate place. In 2014 AAI data analysed for 28 countries, Estonia was 10th while Spain remained in 17th place; Sweden and Denmark continued to lead with the best AHA scores and the lowest scores went to Poland and last-place Greece [64].

In general, both of these models showed a geographic pattern similar to that of the AHA-B model in earlier studies, in which the highest scores are observed in northern countries and western Europe, but with great variability in the order of countries listed [64]. Results reported by Perales et al. and Sowa et al. follow a similar geographic gradient [58,65]. Measuring mental well-being with a subjective variable, "satisfaction with life", affected the distribution of AHA by country. The countries of southern and eastern Europe that scored lower in this variable were below the global mean in the AHA-BPS model, while some of them were above the mean according to the AHA-B model. Other researchers corroborate these levels of satisfaction with life observed in northern and south-eastern Europe [66]. In addition, both models (AHA-B and AHA-BPS) found less social participation in the countries of southern and eastern Europe. Similarly, Litwin notes that non-Mediterranean men participate in more social activities, and do so more frequently, than do Mediterranean men [67]. Within the social network criterion, if economic or household help received from outside the home is calculated using the AHA-BPS model, the countries with the lowest scores on social networks are those of southern and western Europe, and Spain scored last. These data coincide with a study showing a great difference between northern and southern Europe. In the northern countries, older people who live alone receive more social assistance and institutional support, whereas in the Mediterranean countries, the support is provided by the family [68].

In relation to the sociodemographic variables associated with AHA, they were the same in both models: being male, younger, better educated, actively employed, and not having economic difficulty reaching the end of the month. Regarding AHA prevalence by age group, older age was associated with less AHA. This difference was greater in the AHA-B model, especially with respect to those aged 50–64 compared to the group aged 65–74, where the prevalence decreased almost 50% with older age. In the AHA-BPS model, AHA decreased more progressively with age. This age gradient has also been shown in previous studies [1517,69,70]. A lower percentage of women reported AHA in both models, although the difference was greater in the AHA-BPS model, with men scoring 14.1 percentage points higher. This was mainly due to the AHA-BPS model’s inclusion of mental well-being variables, where women scored 13.2 percentage points lower. These results coincide with other authors who confirmed a higher percentage of AHA in men [1517,71] and Bowling reported this difference both in the biomedical model and when the participants were asked for their self-perceptions about ageing [72]. Similarly, in two AAI datasets separated by sex, women generally had lower scores, regardless of differences between countries [63]. However, other authors have found little difference in AHA by sex [9,12,25,58] or greater AHA among women [8]. In most studies, a better socio-economic level was associated with a higher prevalence of AHA [1517,19,69]. In the AHA-BPS model, more married people had AHA, compared to those who were widowed, divorced, or single, in concordance with other authors [23,58,70]. In both models we studied, the most significant difference related to marital status was to be widowed, although some authors have reported no differences according to marital status [23,69]. With respect to educational level, more education was associated with greater AHA in both models. The difference was greatest between the groups with the lowest and highest educational levels, coinciding with previous reports [16,58,70,73]. Having economic difficulty reaching the end of the month was associated with less AHA, more markedly in the AHA-B model. The AAI was also positively correlated with a country’s GDP per capita [64]. The OR for AHA was similar in both models for all sociodemographic variables except age: in the AHA-B model, people aged 85 and older were 15 times more likely to have AHA than people 50–64 years old; the difference was less than four times as likely in the AHA-BPS model. This finding provides greater evidence that the AHA-BPS model does not penalize older age (and the increased morbidity inherent to old age) and is more likely to characterize AHA from a health promotion perspective.

The main limitation of our work was that cross-sectional design precludes establishing causality relationships between the constructs or validating the predictive capacity of the AHA-BPS model. However, the potential exists for longitudinal analysis using Wave 6 SHARE data. The strength of this research is the comparison of two models of AHA assessment in a representative sample of 52,641 people from 14 European countries.

We concluded that the AHA-BPS model is more inclusive, allows measurement of AHA from a positive health perspective (without penalising for disease), and better identifies the population for which AHA promotion policies should be established. The contribution of this model is very important, in view of the global challenge that population ageing presents, because it applies the WHO criteria. Moreover, according to the Global Strategy and Action Plan (2016), our results reflect improved AHA measurement, monitoring, and research systems using this approach. The Global Strategy and Action Plan (2016) that was approved at the 69th World Assembly urges the establishment of the required evidence and partnerships necessary to support a Decade of Healthy Ageing from 2020 to 2030. One of the 10 mid-term progress indicators related to the action plan objectives is “Number of countries with cross-sectional, nationally representative, anonymous, individual-level data collected since 2010 on older adults and their health status and needs in the public domain” [30]. Analysis of the SHARE data on ageing and health offers a superb opportunity to contribute to the evidence needed to generate actions that will promote healthy ageing and health equity. The AHA-BPS model allows a detailed analysis of ageing in Europe and defines a baseline from which to monitor the evolution of the situation and the impact of the various interventions in favour of AHA [29]. Rowe recently affirmed that successful population ageing depends on how world societies adapt to the ageing phenomenon and invited others to learn from the European experience as an older continent [74].

As SHARE is a longitudinal study, future lines of research can be designed to validate the predictive value of the AHA-BPS model and to monitor and characterize the ageing of individuals, beginning at 50 years of age in some cases. Future research is needed to identify key public health actions carried out in the countries with the best AHA prevalence and to study the details of systems supporting health and well-being in greater depth, in order to find the strong points that have a direct impact on AHA and therefore contribute to improving the health and quality of life of older people.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Number of participants and missing values, by country.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206353.s001

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Coding of dependent and independent variables selected from the SHARE Database.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206353.s002

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Participant characteristics by country.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206353.s003

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Prevalence of AHA in both models, by country.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206353.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

Elaine Lilly, Ph.D., provided guidance in the translation and assistance with final English language revision of the article.

References

  1. 1. European Commission. Directorate-General for Economic and Financial Affairs. The 2018 Ageing Report. Underlying Assumptions and Projection Methodologies [Internet]. Luxembourg: European Commission; 2017 [cited 2018 Aug 24]. Available from: file:///C:/Users/admin/Downloads/KCBC17065ENN.en.pdf
  2. 2. Prince MJ, Wu F, Guo Y, Gutierrez Robledo LM, O’Donnell M, Sullivan R, et al. The burden of disease in older people and implications for health policy and practice. Lancet. 2014 Nov;6736(14):1–14. Available from: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0140673614613477
  3. 3. World Health Organization. World report on ageing and health [Internet]. Geneva: WHO; 2015 [cited 2017 Nov 25]. Available from: http://www.who.int/ageing/publications/world-report-2015/en/
  4. 4. World Health Organization. Noncommunicable Disease Prevention and Health Promotion Aging and Life Course. Active Ageing: A Policy Framework [Internet]. Geneva: WHO; 2002 [cited 2017 Oct 8]. Available from: http://whqlibdoc.who.int/hq/2002/WHO_NMH_NPH_02.8.pdf
  5. 5. A Europe 2020 Initiative. Strategic Implementation Plan of the Pilot European Innovation Partnership on Active and Healthy Ageing—Innovation Union—European Commission [Internet]. Brussels: European Commission; 2011 [cited 2017 Sep 10]. Available from: https://ec.europa.eu/research/innovation-union/pdf/active-healthy-ageing/steering-group/implementation_plan.pdf
  6. 6. World Health Organization. Regional Office for Europe. Strategy and action plan for healthy ageing in Europe, 2012–2020 [Internet]. Malta: WHO; 2012 [cited 2017 Nov 20]. Available from: http://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0008/175544/RC62wd10Rev1-Eng.pdf
  7. 7. Cosco TD, Prina AM, Perales J, Stephan BCM, Brayne C. Operational definitions of successful aging: a systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr. 2014 Mar;26(3):373–81. Available from: https://www.cambridge.org/core/services/aop-cambridge-core/content/view/FDA38D217059EC8F9F61B23A16A32155/S1041610213002287a.pdf/operational_definitions_of_successful_aging_a_systematic_review.pdf pmid:24308764
  8. 8. Depp CA, Jeste D V. Definitions and Predictors of Successful Aging: A Comprehensive Review of Larger Quantitative Studie. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(1):6–20. Available from: http://search.proquest.com/docview/195987370?accountid=15295 pmid:16407577
  9. 9. Peel N, Bartlett H, McClure R. Healthy ageing: how is it defined and measured? Aust J Ageing. 2004;23(3):115–9. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/j.1741-6612.2004.00035.x
  10. 10. Strawbridge WJ, Wallhagen MI, Cohen RD. Successful Aging and Well-Being: Self-Rated Compared with Rowe and Kahn. Gerontologist. 2002;42(6):727–33. Available from: https://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/content/42/6/727.full pmid:12451153
  11. 11. Rowe JW, Kahn RL. Successful Aging. Gerontologist. 1997;37(4):433–40. pmid:9279031
  12. 12. McLaughlin SJ, Jette AM, Connell CM. An examination of healthy aging across a conceptual continuum: prevalence estimates, demographic patterns, and validity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2012 Jun;67(7):783–9. Available from: http://biomedgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/content/67/7/783.full pmid:22367432
  13. 13. Cho J, Martin P, Poon LW. The older they are, the less successful they become? Findings from the Georgia centenarian study. J Aging Res. 2012 Jan;2012. Available from: http://www.hindawi.com/journals/jar/2012/695854/
  14. 14. Katz S, Calasanti T. Critical perspectives on successful aging: Does it “appeal more than it illuminates”? Gerontologist. 2015;55(1):26–33 pmid:24747713
  15. 15. Hank K. How “Successful” Do Older Europeans Age? Findings From SHARE. J Gerontol Soc Sci. 2011;66B(2):230–6. Available from: https://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2010/12/06/geronb.gbq089.full
  16. 16. Britton A, Shipley M, Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG. Successful aging: the contribution of early-life and midlife risk factors. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008 Jun;56(6):1098–105. Available from: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01740.x/full pmid:18482302
  17. 17. McLaughlin SJ, Connell CM, Heeringa SG, Li LW, Roberts JS. Successful aging in the United States: prevalence estimates from a national sample of older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010;65B(2):216–26. pmid:20008481
  18. 18. Willcox B. Successful aging: is there hope? CMAJ. 2012 Dec 11;184(18):1973–4. Available from: http://www.cmaj.ca/content/184/18/1973.full.pdf+html
  19. 19. Jeste D V, Savla GN, Thompson WK, Vahia I V, Glorioso DK, Martin AS, et al. Association Between Older Age and More Successful Aging: Critical Role of Resilience and Depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(2):188–96. Available from: https://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/doi/full/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12030386 pmid:23223917
  20. 20. Weir PL, Meisner BA, Baker J. Successful aging across the years: does one model fit everyone? J Health Psychol. 2010 Jul;15(5):680–7. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1359105309353648 pmid:20603291
  21. 21. Cosco TD, Blossom S, Brayne C. (Unsuccessful) Binary modeling of successful aging in the oldest-old adults: A call for continuum-based measures. JAGS. 2014;62(8):1597–8. Available from: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jgs.12958/pdf
  22. 22. Fernández-Ballesteros R, Molina M, Schettini R, Santacreu M. The Semantic Network of Aging Well. Annu Rev Gerontol Geriatr. 2013;33:79–107. Available from: http://www.springerpub.com/annual-review-of-gerontology-and-geriatrics-volume-33-2013-9780826109941.html
  23. 23. Pruchno RA, Wilson-Genderson M, Rose M, Cartwright F. Successful aging: early influences and contemporary characteristics. Gerontologist. 2010 Dec;50(6):821–33. pmid:20513694
  24. 24. Martin P, Kelly N, Kahana B, Kahana E, J Willcox B, Willcox DC, et al. Defining Successful Aging: A Tangible or Elusive Concept? Gerontologist. 2015 Feb;55(1):14–25. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24840916 pmid:24840916
  25. 25. Montross L, Depp C, Daly J, Reichstadt J, Golshan S, Moore D, et al. Correlates of Self-Rated Successful Aging Among Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(1):43–51. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1064748112615917 pmid:16407581
  26. 26. Vahia I V, Thompson WK, Depp CA, Allison M, Jeste D V. Developing a dimensional model for successful cognitive and emotional aging. Int Psychogeriatr. 2012 Apr;24(4):515–23. pmid:22050770
  27. 27. Paúl C, Ribeiro O, Teixeira L. Active Ageing: An Empirical Approach to the WHO Model. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. 2012 Jan;2012. Available from: http://www.hindawi.com/journals/cggr/2012/382972/
  28. 28. Zaidi A, Gasior K, Hofmarcher MM, Lelkes O, Marin B, Rodrigues R, et al. Active Ageing Index 2012. Concept, Methodology and Final Results. Vienna: European Centre Vienna; 2013. p. 68.
  29. 29. Bousquet J, Kuh D, Bewick M, Standberg T, Farrell J, Pengelly R, et al. Operational definition of Active and Healthy Ageing (AHA): A conceptual framework. J Nutr Heal Aging. 2015.
  30. 30. World Health Organization. Global strategy and action plan on ageing and health [Internet]. Geneva: WHO; 2017 [cited 2018 Aug 8]. Available from: http://www.who.int/ageing/WHO-GSAP-2017.pdf?ua=1
  31. 31. Börsch-Supan A. Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) Wave 5. Release version: 1.0.0. SHARE-ERIC. Dataset. 2015.
  32. 32. Börsch-Supan A, Brandt M, Hunkler C, Kneip T, Korbmacher J, Malter F, et al. Data Resource Profile: The Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). Int J Epidemiol. 2013 Aug;42(4):992–1001. Available from: http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2013/06/18/ije.dyt088.full pmid:23778574
  33. 33. Malter F, Börsch-Supan A, editors. SHARE Wave 5: Innovations & Methodology [Internet]. München: Munich Center for the Economics of Ageing (MEA), Max Planck Institute for Social Law and Social Policy; 2015. 177 p. Available from: http://www.share-project.org/fileadmin/pdf_documentation/Method_vol5_31March2015.pdf https://doi.org/10.1002/wdev.204
  34. 34. Castro-Costa E, Dewey M, Stewart R, Banerjee S, Huppert F, Mendonca-Lima C, et al. Ascertaining late-life depressive symptoms in Europe: an evaluation of the survey version of the EURO-D scale in 10 nations. The SHARE project. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2008 Jan;17(1):12–29. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18286461 pmid:18286461
  35. 35. Prince MJ, Reischies F, Beekman AT, Fuhrer R, Jonker C, Kivela SL, et al. Development of the EURO-D scale—a European, Union initiative to compare symptoms of depression in 14 European centres. Br J Psychiatry. 1999 Apr 1;174(4):330–8. Available from: http://bjp.rcpsych.org/content/174/4/330
  36. 36. Romero-Ortuno R, Walsh CD, Lawlor BA, Kenny RA. A Frailty Instrument for primary care: findings from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). BMC Geriatr. 2010;10:57. Available from: https://bmcgeriatr.biomedcentral.com/track/pdf/10.1186/1471-2318-10-57 pmid:20731877
  37. 37. Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A Short Scale for Measuring Loneliness in Large Surveys: Results From Two Population-Based Studies. Res Aging. 2004 Jan 1;26(6):655–72. Available from: http://roa.sagepub.com/content/26/6/655.full.pdf+html pmid:18504506
  38. 38. International Standard Classification of Education ISCED 1997 [Internet]. Geneva: UNESCO; 1997 [cited 2017 Jun 3]. Available from: http://www.unesco.org/education/information/nfsunesco/doc/isced_1997.htm
  39. 39. EuroVoc Multilingual Thesaurus of the European Union. Europe [Internet]. Brussels: European Union; 2018 [cited 2018 Aug 8]. Available from:http://eurovoc.europa.eu/drupal/?q=request&mturi=http://eurovoc.europa.eu/100277&language=en&view=mt&ifacelang=en
  40. 40. Phelan EA, Larson EB. “Successful Aging”-Where Next? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002 Jul;50(7):1306–8. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50324.x pmid:12133032
  41. 41. Fernández-Ballesteros R, Zamarrón MD, López MD, Molina MÁ, Díez J, Montero P, et al. Envejecimiento con éxito: criterios y predictores. Psicothema. 2010;22(4):641–7. Available from: http://www.unioviedo.es/reunido/index.php/PST/article/view/8930
  42. 42. Jeste D V, Depp CA, Vahia I V. Successful cognitive and emotional aging. World Psychiatry. 2010 Jun;9(2):78–84. Available from: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/j.2051-5545.2010.tb00277.x/full pmid:20671889
  43. 43. Depp CA, Glatt SJ, Jeste D V. Recent advances in research on successful or healthy aging. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2007;9(1):7–13. Available from: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11920-007-0003-0 pmid:17257507
  44. 44. Bowling A. Lay perceptions of successful ageing: findings from a national survey of middle aged and older adults in Britain. Eur J Ageing. 2006 Sep 5;3(3):123–36. Available from: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10433-006-0032-2 pmid:28794758
  45. 45. Young Y, Frick KD, Phelan EA. Can successful aging and chronic illness coexist in the same individual? A multidimensional concept of successful aging. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2009 Feb;10(2):87–92. pmid:19187875
  46. 46. Phelan EA, Anderson LA, Lacroix AZ, Larson EB. Older Adults’ Views of “‘Successful Aging’” How do they compare with researchers’ definitions? Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52(3):211–6. Available from: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52056.x/full
  47. 47. Parslow RA, Lewis VJ, Nay R. Successful aging: development and testing of a multidimensional model using data from a large sample of older australians. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011 Nov;59(11):2077–83. Available from: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03665.x/pdf pmid:22091484
  48. 48. Beard JR, Bloom DE. Towards a comprehensive public health response to population ageing. Lancet. 2014 Nov;385:658–61. Available from: http://www.thelancet.com/journals/lancet/article/PIIS0140-6736(14)61461-6/fulltext pmid:25468151
  49. 49. Swindell WR, Ensrud KE, Cawthon PM, Cauley JA, Cummings SR, Miller RA, et al. Indicators of “Healthy Aging” in older women (65–69 years of age). A data-mining approach based on prediction of long-term survival. BMC Geriatr. 2010;10(55):1–25. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2936300/
  50. 50. Fuchs J, Scheidt-Nave C, Hinrichs T, Mergenthaler A, Stein J, Riedel-Heller SG, et al. Indicators for healthy ageing—a debate. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2013 Dec;10(12):6630–44. Available from: http://search.proquest.com/docview/1494553046?rfr_id=info:xri/sid:primo pmid:24317381
  51. 51. Inui TS. The need for an integrated biopsychosocial approach to research on successful aging. Ann Intern Med. 2003;139(5):391–4. Available from: http://annals.org/article.aspx?articleid=1136312
  52. 52. Hung L-W, Kempen GIJM, De Vries NK. Cross-cultural comparison between academic and lay views of healthy ageing: a literature review. Ageing Soc. 2010 Aug 9;30(8):1373–91. Available from: http://www.journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0144686X10000589
  53. 53. Fernández-Ballesteros R, García LF, Abarca D, Blanc L, Efklides A, Kornfeld R, et al. Lay concept of aging well: cross-cultural comparisons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008 May;56(5):950–2. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01654.x pmid:18454758
  54. 54. Tumminello M, Miccichè S, Dominguez LJ, Lamura G, Melchiorre MG, Barbagallo M, et al. Happy Aged People Are All Alike, While Every Unhappy Aged Person Is Unhappy in Its Own Way. Perc M, editor. PLoS One. 2011 Sep 8;6(9):e23377. Available from: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023377 pmid:21931596
  55. 55. VonFaber M, Bootsma–van der Wiel A, van Exel E, Gussekloo J, Lagaay AM, Dongen E van, et al. Successful Aging in the Oldest Old. Arch Intern Med. 2001;161:2694–700. Available from: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/article.aspx?articleid=752299 pmid:11732934
  56. 56. Baltes P, Baltes M. Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In: Baltes P, Baltes M, editors. Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1990. p. 1–34.
  57. 57. Steptoe A, Deaton A, Stone AA. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet. 2014 Nov;6736(13):1–9. Available from: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0140673613614890
  58. 58. Perales J, Martin S, Ayuso-Mateos JL, Chatterji S, Garin N, Koskinen S, et al. Factors associated with active aging in Finland, Poland, and Spain. Int Psychogeriatr. 2014 Aug;26(8):1363–75. Available from: https://www.cambridge.org/core/services/aop-cambridge-core/content/view/5257F8ABE4DCDED47A635A43A384443B/S1041610214000520a.pdf/factors_associated_with_active_aging_in_finland_poland_and_spain.pdf pmid:24735743
  59. 59. Myers DG, Diener E. Who is happy? Psychol Sci. 1995 Jan 1;6(1):10–9. Available from: http://pss.sagepub.com/content/6/1/10.full.pdf
  60. 60. Borchelt M, Gilberg R, Horgas AL, Geiselmann B. On the significance of morbidity and disability in old age. In: Baltes PB, Mayer KU, editors. The Berlin Aging Study: Aging from 70 to 100. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2001. p. 403–429.
  61. 61. Rico-Uribe LA, Caballero FF, Olaya B, Tobiasz-Adamczyk B, Koskinen S, Leonardi M, et al. Loneliness, Social Networks, and Health: A Cross-Sectional Study in Three Countries. Gilman SE, editor. PLoS One. 2016 Jan 13;11(1):e0145264. Available from: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0145264 pmid:26761205
  62. 62. Wahrendorf M, Reinhardt JD, Siegrist J. Relationships of Disability with Age Among Adults Aged 50 to 85: Evidence from the United States, England and Continental Europe. Zhang H, editor. PLoS One. 2013 Aug 14;8(8):e71893. Available from: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071893 pmid:23977172
  63. 63. Employment Social Affairs and Inclusion. Introducing the Active Ageing Index. Policy brief. Brussels: European Commission; 2013. p. 14.
  64. 64. United Nations Economic Commission for Europe. Active Ageing Index 2014. Analytical Report. Geneva: UNECE; 2015. p. 45.
  65. 65. Sowa A, Tobiasz-Adamczyk B, Topór-Mądry R, Poscia A, Ignazio D, Milia L. Predictors of healthy ageing: public health policy targets. BMC Health Serv Res. 2016;16(Suppl 5):289. Available from: https://bmchealthservres.biomedcentral.com/track/pdf/10.1186/s12913-016-1520-5?site=bmchealthservres.biomedcentral.com
  66. 66. Rodrigues R, Huber M, Lamura G, editors. Facts and Figures on Healthy Ageing and Long-term Care: Europe and North America. Vienna: European Centre for Social Welfare Policy and Research; 2012. Available from: file:///C:/Users/admin/Downloads/LTC_Final.pdf
  67. 67. Litwin H. Social networks and well-being: a comparison of older people in Mediterranean and non-Mediterranean countries. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010 Sep 14;65(5):599–608. Available from: https://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/content/65B/5/599.full pmid:20008485
  68. 68. Börsch-Supan A, Brugiavini A, Jürges H, Mackenbach JP, Siegrist J, Weber G. Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe: First Results from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe. Mannheim: Mannheim Research Institute for the Economics of Aging; 2005. 372 p.
  69. 69. Chaves ML, Camozzato AL, Eizirik CL, Kaye J. Predictors of Normal and Successful Aging Among Urban-Dwelling Elderly Brazilians. J Gerontol Psychol Sci. 2009 Jul 15;64B(5):597–602. Available from: http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/content/64B/5/597.full
  70. 70. Sabia S, Singh-Manoux A, Hagger-Johnson G, Cambois E, Brunner EJ, Kivimaki M. Influence of individual and combined healthy behaviours on successful aging. CMAJ. 2012 Dec 11;184(18):1985–92. Available from: http://www.cmaj.ca/content/184/18/1985.full.pdf+html pmid:23091184
  71. 71. Tovel H, Carmel S. Maintaining Successful Aging: The Role of Coping Patterns and Resources. J Happiness Stud. 2013 Feb 24;15(2):255–70. Available from: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10902-013-9420-4
  72. 72. Bowling A, Iliffe S. Which model of successful ageing should be used? Baseline findings from a British longitudinal survey of ageing. Age Ageing. 2006 Nov;35(6):607–14. Available from: http://ageing.oxfordjournals.org/content/35/6/607 pmid:16951427
  73. 73. Paulo AC, Sampaio A, Santos NC, Costa PS, Cunha P, Zihl J, et al. Patterns of Cognitive Performance in Healthy Ageing in Northern Portugal: A Cross-Sectional Analysis. Laks J, editor. PLoS One. 2011 Sep 8;6(9):e24553. Available from: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024553 pmid:21931752
  74. 74. Rowe JW. Successful Aging of Societies. Dædalus. 2015;144(2):5–12. Available from: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/DAED_a_00325