Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The Desire for Amputation or Paralyzation: Evidence for Structural Brain Anomalies in Body Integrity Identity Disorder (BIID)

  • Rianne M. Blom ,

    riannemb@gmail.com

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Guido A. van Wingen,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Brain Imaging Center, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Sija J. van der Wal,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Judy Luigjes,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Brain Imaging Center, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Milenna T. van Dijk,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Sackler Institute of Graduate Biomedical Sciences, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States of America

  • H. Steven Scholte,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Damiaan Denys

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Brain Imaging Center, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Abstract

Background

Body Integrity Identity Disorder (BIID) is a condition in which individuals perceive a mismatch between their internal body scheme and physical body shape, resulting in an absolute desire to be either amputated or paralyzed. The condition is hypothesized to be of congenital nature, but evidence for a neuro-anatomical basis is sparse.

Methods

We collected T1-weighted structural magnetic resonance imaging scans on a 3T scanner in eight individuals with BIID and 24 matched healthy controls, and analyzed the data using voxel-based morphometry.

Results

The results showed reduced grey matter volume in the left dorsal and ventral premotor cortices and larger grey matter volume in the cerebellum (lobule VIIa) in individuals with BIID compared to controls.

Conclusion

The premotor cortex and cerebellum are thought to be crucial for the experience of body-ownership and the integration of multisensory information. Our results suggest that BIID is associated with structural brain anomalies and might result from a dysfunction in the integration of multisensory information, leading to the feeling of disunity between the mental and physical body shape.

Introduction

We are supposed to have an internal body scheme that can distinguish our physical body from other objects [1,2]. However, there are cases in which persons feel that a specific body part is superfluous or alien. Such is the case in stroke patients who have suffered a hemorrhage in the parietal cortex and request the nurse to remove ‘that strange leg’ from their bed [35]. Individuals with Body Integrity Identity Disorder (BIID) experience a difference between their internal body scheme and physical body shape from early youth on. As a result, they desire to be either amputated or paralyzed, in order to neutralize this conflicting internal and external body (scheme) [68]. Despite clear observed disturbance in brain structures in body-image stroke patients [35,9], there are few studies that can provide evidence for brain abnormalities in BIID [10,11].

Originally BIID was seen as a mere psychological problem [1214]. The obsession to amputate one’s limbs was called apotemnophilia and seen as an erotic manifestation of hatred towards maternal figures [12,13]. Later on, the incongruence between one’s experienced and assigned body was observed [15,16]. Parallels -the ‘not feeling well in one’s body’- were made with Gender Dysphoria, and the condition was classified as an identity disorder (BIID) [7]. Nowadays, the disorder is hypothesized to be a congenital cerebral disorder [1721]. The early onset, the therapeutically unchangeable desire, the exact boundaries of the modification, and parallels in the symptomatology of several neurological conditions with an abnormal bodily experience (e.g. somatoparaphrenia or misoplegia) strengthen this hypothesis [14].

Next to the wish for limb amputation a milder variant of BIID is reported as well: the desire for non-functioning limbs [6,8,16]. The so-called paralyzation variant is characterized by the same typical BIID features [6,8,16,22], with the exception of a higher percentage of females [6,8]. Important to note, individuals with paralyzation BIID, only aim for non-functioning of the limbs and they prefer to keep the functions of the urogenital tract. In general, individuals with BIID do not aim for disability in order to obtain any sort of benefit other than feeling at ease with their own body. There is no conversion, imagined ugliness, pain or psychosis that could explain the desirability of body-modification [6]. Due to embarrassment for their bizarre wish, subjects with BIID rarely present themselves to medical professionals. This complicates investigating the disorder properly and results in the disorder not being officially recognized by the WHO [6,7].

Models of body-ownership are mainly based on studies with healthy individuals and patients with brain lesions. Studies in healthy individuals have often used an experimental paradigm called the rubber hand illusion (RHI). In RHI a fake hand will be perceived as one’s own when the actual hand is touched simultaneously with the fake one and only the latter is seen [23]. It is hypothesized that this illusion comes about by integration of multisensory (visual, proprioceptive and tactile) stimuli and a pre-existing body scheme that contains a reference description of the body [1,2]. In general, important brain areas involved in this illusion are the visual and primary sensory (SI) cortices that are involved in recognizing multisensory stimuli, the parietal cortex that is involved in multimodal integration [1,2,24]. Furthermore, most RHI studies report that the ventral premotor cortex is crucial to the emergence of the ownership experience [1,2428], although there is some evidence that the insular cortex is also involved in the maintenance of the sense of body ownership [2,29].

Lesions in brain areas involved in this body ownership network lead to a variety of neurological body-ownership pathologies [4,30]. Somatoparaphrenia patients misidentify the ownership of a left-sided body part [3,30]; in misoplegia the loss of recognition goes along with hatred of a body part [31,32]; in alien hand syndrome involuntary movements and estrangement from a particular limb are prominent [33,34] and asomatognosonia patients lack awareness of a limb [30], although sometimes this misconception can (temporarily) be corrected. The right posterior insula seems to be a crucial structure in patients with disturbed sensation of limb ownership [4,9], but some case-studies also report patients with alien hand syndrome and asomatognosia with single lesions in the corpus callosum [33,35] or premotor cortex [36] respectively.

One study investigated structural anomalies in subjects with BIID. In 2013 Hilti et al preformed structural imaging in 13 male patients with a strong desire for amputation and an equal number matched controls [10]. Surface-based morphometry revealed group differences in the somatosensory and parietal cortices, although the results did not remain significant after correcting for multiple comparisons. Hilti et al. assumed that BIID is not a disorder that can be localized in any circumscribed region of the human brain, but assumed that it reflected a breakdown of key areas coding for different facets of the experience of body-ownership.

We recently reported the results of a functional MRI study in individuals with BIID [11]. We included 5 BIID patients with a single leg amputation desire and matched them to healthy controls. Our study showed that individuals with BIID have overall heightened responsivity of a large somatosensory network during tactile stimulation of both legs. Importantly, we found reduced activity in the left premotor cortex during stimulation of the disowned leg compared to the owned leg, suggesting that altered somatosensory processing in the premotor cortex is associated with the feeling of disownership in BIID.

To investigate whether altered processing in the premotor cortex is associated with differences in neuroanatomy, we now also analyzed the structural MRI data from our previous study in a slightly larger sample of participants. In line with our previous findings, we hypothesized that BIID could be associated with differences in cortical gray or white matter volume of the premotor cortices.

Methods

Participants

BIID participants were recruited through advertisements on online BIID forums and via other participants and were matched to healthy controls (HC). In order to increase power we collected thrice as many HC as individuals with BIID. BIID was diagnosed with a psychiatric assessment, using the definition of BIID as the existence of a lifelong desire to have either an amputation or paralyzation with the primary objective to restore one's true identity. Furthermore, patients were screened for the presence of DSM-IV axis I disorders. The study was approved by the Medical Ethical committee of the Academic Medical Centre (AMC) of the University of Amsterdam and written informed consent was obtained after complete description of the study to the subjects.

Image acquisition

We acquired a T1-weighted whole brain high-resolution MRI on a 3 Tesla Intera full-body scanner (Philips Healthcare, Best, The Netherlands), equipped with a SENSE eight-channel receiver head coil, with the following parameters: matrix size: 256 × 256, field of view (FOV): 226 × 226 × 218 mm3, voxel size: 0.88 × 0.88 × 1.2 mm3, repetition time (TR): 9.6 msec., echo time (TE): 4.6 msec.; Flip angle: 8°; no. slices: 182. Head movement was restricted using foam padding.

Image processing

We examined images for scanning and motion artefacts and large anatomical abnormalities before processing. Subsequently, we centered all images manually at the anterior commissure and pre-processed them with SPM8 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/sofware/) implemented in Matlab 8.1.0 (The Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA). Pre-processing for VBM included segmenting structural T1 images into grey matter (GM), white matter (WM), cerebrospinal fluid and skull, manually quality control check, normalization to MNI space, modulation with the Jacobian determinant, and spatial smoothing (Gaussian kernel of 12-mm full-width at half maximum), using the standard tools of SPM8. The obtained images were used for statistical analysis.

Statistical analyses

We compared demographics using non-parametric tests (the Mann-Whitney U test) for linear variables and chi-square test for dichotomous variables in SPSS (IBM SPSS for Windows, 20.0, 2011). Voxel-wise statistical tests were family-wise error (FWE) rate corrected for multiple comparisons (p<0.05) across the whole brain or the region of interest (ROI), and were corrected for total intracranial volume (TIV) by including TIV as covariate of no interest. Given our a-priori knowledge of the involvement of the left dorsal and ventral premotor cortex related to BIID in our previous study, we applied a small volume correction (SVC) to these regions by centring a sphere of 15mm radius around the meta-analytic center of the premotor cortices based on a study by Mayka et. al. (2006) (i.e. PMd x = -20; y = -4; z = 58 and PMv x = -50; y = 5; z = 22) [37]. Moreover, for exploratory purposes we applied SVC to regions found in the only other BIID MRI paper of Hilti et al. by centring spheres of 15mm radius around their peak-coordinates found (i.e. superior parietal lobule x = 17 y = -50 z = 61; inferior parietal lobule x = 57 y = -27 z = 38; central sulcus x = 33 y = -16 z = 40; anterior insular cortex, upper cluster y = 32 x = 25 z = 9; anterior insular cortex, lower cluster x = 32 y = 20 z = -4; primary somatosensory cortex x = 5 y = -38 z = 62; secondary somatosensory cortex x = 54 y -3 z = 9; inferior parietal lobule x = 35 y = -31 z = 42) and combined those into one region of interest. In addition we reported clusters of more than 10 contiguous voxels, tresholded at p<0.005 uncorrected for multiple comparisons.

Results

Subjects

We recruited eight individuals with BIID and 24 matched healthy controls without a neurological or psychiatric history (Table 1). Characteristics of subjects with BIID are described in Table 2. In summary, two of the BIID participants had a psychiatric co-morbid mood disorder, resulting from having to deal with BIID, a strange, untreatable condition. Three individuals with BIID desired amputation of the right leg, two the left leg, one desired both legs to be amputated and two desired both legs to be paralyzed.

thumbnail
Table 1. Subject characteristics and global brain measures of BIID patients (BIID) and healthy controls (HC).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165789.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Subject characteristics of Body Integrity Identity Disorder (BIID) patients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165789.t002

Voxel-based morphometry data

Global brain measures did not differ significantly between groups (Table 1). Gray matter volumes in both the left PMd and left PMv were significantly smaller in BIID patients than in healthy controls (Figs 1 and 2). Gray matter volume of the left cerebellum (lobule VII) was significantly larger in BIID patients than in healthy controls (Table 3, Fig 3). To explore whether there the BIID patients in our study showed similar gray matter abnormalities as reported in the only other study on BIID, we created an ROI that included all the regions that were reported in Hilti et al. The brain region with the maximum difference was located in at the right inferior parietal lobule, but did not survive the SVC (p = 0.191 (SVC), T = 3.54, maximum difference at MNI x = 24 Y; y = -36; z = 48). There were no other significant differences in gray or white matter after correction for multiple comparisons. For exploratory purpose, results at p<0.005 uncorrected were reported in Tables 3 & 4.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Significant smaller grey matter volume in the left ventral premotor cortex in BIID subjects (PMv: p = 0.006 (SVC), T = 4.27,maximum difference at MNI x = -36, y = 6, z = 22).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165789.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Significant smaller grey matter volume in the left dorsal premotor cortex in BIID subjects (PMd: p = 0.007 (SVC), T = 4.33, maximum difference at MNI x = -18; y = -6; z = 52).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165789.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Significant larger grey matter volume in the left cerebellum in BIID subjects (p = 0.038 FWE corrected), T = 5.47, maximum difference at MNI x = -2, y = -90, z = -34).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165789.g003

thumbnail
Table 3. Results from the whole brain analysis corrected for total intracranial volume for grey matter volume differences between BIID patients (BIID) and healthy controls (HC).

Values are displayed for clusters with >10 voxels at an initial threshold of p <0.005 uncorrected; L: left; R: right.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165789.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Results from the whole brain analysis corrected for total intracranial volume for white matter volume differences between BIID patients (BIID) and healthy controls (HC).

Values are displayed for clusters with >10 voxels at an initial threshold of p <0.005 uncorrected; L: left; R: right.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165789.t004

Discussion

In this study we investigated structural brain differences in 8 BIID subjects with either a paralyzation or amputation variant using VBM. As expected we found significantly reduced grey matter volume in the left dorsal (PMd) and ventral (PMv) premotor cortices in BIID subjects compared to controls. Moreover, we additionally observed larger grey matter volume in the cerebellum (lobule VIIa) of BIID subjects compared to HC participants.

In line with the altered PM processing that we reported earlier in our functional MRI study in BIID individuals, we found structural PM gray matter differences [38]. It is accepted that the integration of visual, tactile and proprioceptor information across different body parts contributes to the perception of body ownership [1,2,39,40]. Activity in the premotor cortex (PM) has been shown to reflect the feeling of ownership of a limb by cortical integration of this multisensory information [27,38,39]. Especially the PMv is hypothesized to be crucial in the self-attribution of body-parts. The PMv is anatomically connected to somatosensory and visual areas in the posterior parietal cortex and to frontal motor areas [39,41].

In perceptual illusions and our BIID study the PM seems crucial in experiencing body-ownership, while in stroke patients with disturbance in body-image (n = 70), no difference in PM connectivity was found [25]. Body-ownership problems in neurological patients are mainly seen as impairment of the parietal cortex and insula [9,42]. From a clinical point of view, the loss of recognition of a particular body part is prominent in those neurological patients [3,30], whereas subjects with BIID admit that their limbs are theirs but experience an internal struggle of feeling overcomplete with four functioning limbs [6,14,18]. Therefore, PMv could be more related to the higher order inner body-experiences, whereas the parietal and insular cortex deficits result in a more primary disturbance when it comes to processing visual and sensory information leading to recognition of body-parts. This is underlined by a recent study of Lenggenhager et al. in which 9 BIID subjects were able to experience a rubber foot illusion, although the vividness of the illusion was stronger than in controls [43]. Following this hypothesis, the paralyzation-variant can be seen as a ‘milder form of BIID’, disintegrating the multisensory information to the extent that limbs are not functioning. Whereas a stronger dysfunction might lead to stronger desire, i.e. amputation of the limbs.

One can only speculate about the possible mechanisms underlying the gray matter differences in the PM in BIID, as differences in gray matter could lead to or may be the consequence of abnormal body experience and PM processing. BIID subjects report symptoms of estrangement from early youth on [6,7]. This strengthens the idea of a congenital cause of the condition [17]. The gray matter reductions might therefore be attributed to an early developmental problem. On the other hand, reduced neuronal activity in the PM could lead to structural changes of this area.

The finding of enlarged gray volume in the cerebellum (lobule VIIa, crus II) was unexpected. From an anatomical point of view prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices are reciprocally interconnected with cerebellar lobule VIIa [44]. Functionally this lobule is involved in emotional and cognitive processing [45].

In another psychiatric body-image disorder structural cerebellar differences have been found. In anorexia nervosa (AN) patients, in which a distorted body image is a major component of their condition, atrophy of crus I and II of the cerebellum was found and related to mood alterations and cognitive rigidity [46,47]. However, a meta-analysis of structural MRI data in AN patients did not confirm this cerebellar volumetric differences [48]. Little is known about emotional and cognitive processing in BIID. Clinical studies in BIID report mood symptoms accompanying an unsolved desire [6,22]. Remarkably those depressive symptoms disappear after (self) amputation of their limb [20,49,50]. Through limbic projections on the cerebellum, this structure is an important region in neuroanatomical models of mood-disorders [51]. One study formally assessed emotional processing in BIID [52]. Seven individuals with BIID were tested in facial emotion recognition and emotional responses to disgusting images and, in general, showed no emotional impairment [52]. Functional imaging data are needed to test whether over-activity of the emotional brain circuitry, including the cerebellum, is present in BIID as a result of life-long depressive symptoms.

In addition to emotional processing, the cerebellum is also suggested to be involved in multisensory integration leading to the feeling of body-ownership, next to the vPM. Cerebellar activity, as well as premotor activity, was found to correlate with the strength of the ownership illusion in healthy controls [24,39]. Whereas in patients with neurodegenerative cerebellar ataxia, a reduced sense of the subjective illusory experience was observed [53]. Strikingly enough, the PM was intact in those patients, suggesting that an intact ventral premotor cortex is not sufficient to generate an intact illusion [53]. Moreover, our fMRI study showed that the cerebellum displays heightened activity in BIID during tactile stimulation [38]. Overall, one could hypothesize that, in BIID subjects, impaired PM functioning (reduced GM) leads to over-activity of the lobule VIIa of the cerebellum, leading, in turn, to increased volumetric differences of this structure.

One other neuro-imaging study investigated structural brain abnormalities in BIID. It reported subtle differences in cortical thickness and surfaces area across the insula and parietal cortex, although those results did not survive correction for multiple comparisons [19]. Differences in the PM and cerebellum were not reported but were also not included as regions of interest. Furthermore, our results show also differences in the parietal cortex at an exploratory statistical level and partly replicate the results of Hilti et al. In both studies structural gray matter abnormalities in the superior and inferior parietal lobe and frontal lobe were found. These might be the result of altered neuronal activity in the frontoparietal network involved in the experience of body-ownership in BIID [17,38,54]. The finding of structural frontoparietal abnormalities in two BIID studies provides evidence for the actual presence of these deviations, and therefore point to the direction that the anomalies may not reach statistical significance as a result of limited statistical power due to a limited number of these rare cases included in both studies.

This is the first VBM study reported in BIID subjects, including both the paralyzation and amputation variant of BIID, assuming both are of the same disorder since restoring the primary body identity is the main motivation for body modification, and both disorders only included disunity of the limbs (i.e. not the urogenital tract). After the main analysis, we would have preferred to stratify on both variants, although the number of subjects was not sufficient enough to do so. In addition, the small sample places limitations on generalizability. However, a sample size of eight affected subjects is still considered reasonable due to the rarity of BIID. To increase statistical power, we included three times as many healthy controls.

In conclusion, we found volumetric differences in BIID in the left premotor cortex and cerebellum (left lobule VII) using VBM. Both structures are hypothesized to be crucial for the experience of body-ownership and to integrate multisensory information. Considering these findings BIID could result from a dysfunction in the integration of multisensory information causing the feeling of disunity between the mental and physical bodyshape. Further research is warranted to replicate these findings and correlate them to fMRI data. More importantly, this evidence underlines the suggestion of a neural basis of BIID, and turns away from classifying the symptoms as a mere psychological condition. This is particularly important for the acknowledgment of the disorder as official in ICD-11, which will facilitate the quest to targets for new therapies.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all subjects for participation in this study. The authors thank Bart de Kwaasteniet for help with the data collection.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: MTvD HSS DD.
  2. Data curation: RMB SvdW GAvW.
  3. Formal analysis: RMB SvdW GAvW.
  4. Funding acquisition: DD.
  5. Methodology: MTvD HSS DD.
  6. Project administration: MTvD RMB.
  7. Resources: RMB JL MTvD.
  8. Software: MTvD HSS GAvW RMB.
  9. Supervision: GAvW DD.
  10. Validation: RMB JL MTvD.
  11. Visualization: RMB GAvW.
  12. Writing – original draft: RMB SvdW GAvW DD.
  13. Writing – review & editing: MTvD HSS JL RMB SvdW GAvW DD.

References

  1. 1. Makin TR, Holmes NP, Ehrsson HH. On the other hand: Dummy hands and peripersonal space. Behav Brain Res. 2008;191: 1–10. pmid:18423906
  2. 2. Tsakiris M. My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48: 703–712. pmid:19819247
  3. 3. Vallar G, Ronchi R. Somatoparaphrenia: a body delusion. A review of the neuropsychological literature. Exp Brain Res. 2009;192: 533–551. pmid:18813916
  4. 4. Baier B, Karnath H-OO. Tight link between our sense of limb ownership and self-awareness of actions. Stroke. 2008;39: 486–488. pmid:18162622
  5. 5. Cereda C, Ghika J, Maeder P, Bogousslavsky J. Strokes restricted to the insular cortex. Neurology. 2002;59: 1950–1955. pmid:12499489
  6. 6. Blom RM, Hennekam RC, Denys D. Body integrity identity disorder. PLoS.One. 2012. p. e34702–. pmid:22514657
  7. 7. First MB. Desire for amputation of a limb: paraphilia, psychosis, or a new type of identity disorder. PsycholMed. 2005;35: 919–928.
  8. 8. Giummarra MJ, Bradshaw JL, Hilti LM, Nicholls MER, Brugger P. Paralyzed by Desire : A New Type of Body Integrity Identity Disorder. 2012;00: 1–8.
  9. 9. Karnath H-OO, Baier B. Right insula for our sense of limb ownership and self-awareness of actions. Brain StructFunct. 2010;214: 411–417. pmid:20512380
  10. 10. Hilti LM, Hänggi J, Vitacco DA, Kraemer B, Palla A, Luechinger R, et al. The desire for healthy limb amputation: structural brain correlates and clinical features of xenomelia. Brain. 2013;136: 318–329. pmid:23263196
  11. 11. van Dijk MT, van Wingen G a, van Lammeren A, Blom RM, de Kwaasteniet BP, Scholte HS, et al. Neural basis of limb ownership in individuals with body integrity identity disorder. PLoS One. 2013;8: e72212. pmid:23991064
  12. 12. Money J, Simcoe Kent W, Acrotomophilia, sex and disability: new concepts and case report. Sexuality and Disability. 1984. pp. 43–50.
  13. 13. Money J, Jobaris R, Furth G. Apotemnophilia: two cases of self-demand amputations as paraphilia. J Sex Res. 1977. pp. 115–125.
  14. 14. Sedda A, Bottini G. Apotemnophilia, body integrity identity disorder or xenomelia? Psychiatric and neurologic etiologies face each other. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2014;10: 1255–65. pmid:25045269
  15. 15. Everaerd W. A case of apotemnophilia: a handicap as sexual preference. Am.J.Psychother. pp. 285–293.
  16. 16. Bruno RL, Ph D. Devotees, Pretenders and Wannabes: Two Cases of Factitious Disability Disorder. Sex Disabil. Springer Netherlands; 1997;15: 243–260. Available: http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024769330761
  17. 17. Brang D, McGeoch PD, Ramachandran VS. Apotemnophilia: a neurological disorder. Neuroreport. 2008;19: 1305–1306. pmid:18695512
  18. 18. Brugger P, Lenggenhager B, Giummarra MJ. Xenomelia: a social neuroscience view of altered bodily self-consciousness. Front Psychol. 2013;4: 204. pmid:23630513
  19. 19. Giummarra MJ, Bradshaw JL, Nicholls MER, Hilti LM, Brugger P. Body integrity identity disorder: deranged body processing, right fronto-parietal dysfunction, and phenomenological experience of body incongruity. NeuropsycholRev. 2011;21: 320–333. pmid:22086292
  20. 20. Blom RM, Hennekam RCM. [Desire for amputation in body integrity identity disorder]. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd. 2014;158: A7146. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24713336 pmid:24713336
  21. 21. Lenggenhager B, Hilti L, Palla A, Macauda G, Brugger P. Vestibular stimulation does not diminish the desire for amputation. Cortex. 2014;54: 210–2. pmid:24630361
  22. 22. First MB, Fisher CE. Body integrity identity disorder: the persistent desire to acquire a physical disability. Psychopathology. 2012. pp. 3–14. pmid:22123511
  23. 23. Botvinick M, Cohen J. Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature. 1998. p. 756–. pmid:9486643
  24. 24. Ehrsson HH, Spence C, Passingham RE. That’s my hand! Activity in premotor cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb. Sci (New York, NY). 2004;305: 875–877. pmid:15232072
  25. 25. Zeller D, Gross C, Bartsch A, Johansen-Berg H, Classen J. Ventral Premotor Cortex May Be Required for Dynamic Changes in the Feeling of Limb Ownership: A Lesion Study. J Neurosci. 2011;31: 4852–4857. pmid:21451023
  26. 26. Bekrater-Bodmann R, Foell J, Diers M, Flor H. The perceptual and neuronal stability of the rubber hand illusion across contexts and over time. Brain Res. Elsevier B.V.; 2012;1452: 130–139. pmid:22459041
  27. 27. Gentile G, Björnsdotter M, Petkova VI, Abdulkarim Z, Ehrsson HH. Patterns of neural activity in the human ventral premotor cortex reflect a whole-body multisensory percept. Neuroimage. Elsevier B.V.; 2015;109: 328–40. pmid:25583608
  28. 28. Limanowski J, Blankenburg F. That’s not quite me: limb ownership encoding in the brain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015; nsv079. pmid:26089343
  29. 29. Tsakiris M, Hesse MD, Boy C, Haggard P, Fink GR. Neural signatures of body ownership: a sensory network for bodily self-consciousness. Cereb.Cortex. 2006. pp. 2235–2244. pmid:17138596
  30. 30. Feinberg TE, Venneri A, Simone AM, Fan Y, Northoff G. The neuroanatomy of asomatognosia and somatoparaphrenia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2010;81: 276–281. pmid:19778911
  31. 31. Loetscher T, Regard M, Brugger P. Misoplegia: a review of the literature and a case without hemiplegia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;77: 1099–1100. pmid:16914766
  32. 32. Pearce JMS. Misoplegia. Eur Neurol. 2007;57: 62–4. pmid:17119338
  33. 33. Scepkowski LA, Cronin-Golomb A. The Alien Hand: Cases, Categorizations, and Anatomical Correlates. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2003;2: 261–277. pmid:15006289
  34. 34. Graff-Radford J, Rubin MN, Jones DT, Aksamit AJ, Ahlskog JE, Knopman DS, et al. The alien limb phenomenon. J Neurol. 2013;260: 1880–1888. pmid:23572346
  35. 35. Geschwind DH, Iacoboni M, Mega MS, Zaidel DW, Cloughesy T, Zaidel E. Alien hand syndrome: interhemispheric motor disconnection due to a lesion in the midbody of the corpus callosum. Neurology. 1995;45: 802–808. pmid:7723974
  36. 36. Arzy S, Overney LS, Landis T, Blanke O. Neural mechanisms of embodiment: asomatognosia due to premotor cortex damage. Arch Neurol. 2006;63: 1022–1025. pmid:16831974
  37. 37. Mayka MA, Corcos DM, Leurgans SE, Vaillancourt DE. Three-dimensional locations and boundaries of motor and premotor cortices as defined by functional brain imaging: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage. 2006;31: 1453–74. pmid:16571375
  38. 38. van Dijk MT, van Wingen GA, Van LA, Blom RM, de Kwaasteniet BP, Scholte HS, et al. Neural basis of limb ownership in individuals with body integrity identity disorder. PLoS.One. 2013. p. e72212–. pmid:23991064
  39. 39. Ehrsson HH. Touching a Rubber Hand: Feeling of Body Ownership Is Associated with Activity in Multisensory Brain Areas. J Neurosci. 2005;25: 10564–10573. pmid:16280594
  40. 40. Romano D, Sedda A, Brugger P, Bottini G. Body ownership: When feeling and knowing diverge. Conscious Cogn. 2015;34: 140–8. pmid:25955181
  41. 41. Rizzolatti G, Luppino G, Matelli M. The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1998;106: 283–96. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9741757 pmid:9741757
  42. 42. Karnath H-OO, Baier B, Nagele T, Nägele T. Awareness of the functioning of one’s own limbs mediated by the insular cortex? JNeurosci. 2005;25: 7134–7138. pmid:16079395
  43. 43. Lenggenhager B, Hilti L, Brugger P. Disturbed body integrity and the “rubber foot illusion”. Neuropsychology. 2015;29: 205–11. pmid:25265068
  44. 44. Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. Cortex. Elsevier Srl; 2010;46: 831–844. pmid:20152963
  45. 45. Stoodley C, Schmahmann J. Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage. Elsevier Inc.; 2009;44: 489–501. pmid:18835452
  46. 46. Boghi A, Sterpone S, Sales S, D’Agata F, Bradac GB, Zullo G, et al. In vivo evidence of global and focal brain alterations in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. Elsevier Ireland Ltd; 2011;192: 154–159. pmid:21546219
  47. 47. Amianto F, Caroppo P, D’Agata F, Spalatro A, Lavagnino L, Caglio M, et al. Brain volumetric abnormalities in patients with anorexia and bulimia nervosa: A Voxel-based morphometry study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. Elsevier; 2013;213: 210–216. pmid:23856299
  48. 48. Titova OE, Hjorth OC, Schiöth HB, Brooks SJ. Anorexia nervosa is linked to reduced brain structure in reward and somatosensory regions: a meta-analysis of VBM studies. BMC Psychiatry. 2013;13: 110. pmid:23570420
  49. 49. Noll S, Kasten E. Body Integrity Identity Disorder (BIID): How Satisfied are Successful Wannabes. Psychol Behav Sci. 2014;3: 222–232.
  50. 50. Blom RM, Braam AW, de Boer-Kreeft N, Sonnen MPAM. [Body integrity identity disorder, relief after amputation]. Tijdschr Psychiatr. 2014;56: 54–7. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24446228 pmid:24446228
  51. 51. Soares JC, Mann JJ. The anatomy of mood disorders—review of structural neuroimaging studies. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41: 86–106. pmid:8988799
  52. 52. Bottini G, Brugger P, Sedda A. Is the desire for amputation related to disturbed emotion processing? A multiple case study analysis in BIID. Neurocase. 2015;21: 394–402. pmid:24679146
  53. 53. Fiorio M, Mariotti C, Panzeri M, Antonello E, Classen J, Tinazzi M. The role of the cerebellum in dynamic changes of the sens of body ownership: a study in patients with cerebellar degeneration. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013;26: 712–721. pmid:24236765
  54. 54. McGeoch PD, Brang D, Song T, Lee RR, Huang M, Ramachandran VS. Xenomelia: a new right parietal lobe syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg psychiatry. 2011;82: 1314–9. pmid:21693632