Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Gait in Very Preterm School-Aged Children in Dual-Task Paradigms

Abstract

Objective

The control of gait requires executive and attentional functions. As preterm children show executive and attentional deficits compared to full-term children, performing concurrent tasks that impose additional cognitive load may lead to poorer walking performance in preterm compared to full-term children. Knowledge regarding gait in preterm children after early childhood is scarce. We examined straight walking and if it is more affected in very preterm than in full-term children in dual-task paradigms.

Study design

Twenty preterm children with very low birth-weight (≤ 1500 g), 24 preterm children with birth-weight > 1500 g, and 44 full-term children, born between 2001 and 2006, were investigated. Gait was assessed using an electronic walkway system (GAITRite) while walking without a concurrent task (single-task) and while performing one concurrent (dual-task) or two concurrent (triple-task) tasks. Spatio-temporal gait parameters (gait velocity, cadence, stride length, single support time, double support time), normalized gait parameters (normalized velocity, normalized cadence, normalized stride length) and gait variability parameters (stride velocity variability, stride length variability) were analyzed.

Results

In dual- and triple-task conditions children showed decreased gait velocity, cadence, stride length, as well as increased single support time, double support time and gait variability compared to single-task walking. Further, results showed systematic decreases in stride velocity variability from preterm children with very low birth weight (≤ 1500 g) to preterm children with birth weight > 1500 g to full-term children. There were no significant interactions between walking conditions and prematurity status.

Conclusions

Dual and triple tasking affects gait of preterm and full-term children, confirming previous results that walking requires executive and attentional functions. Birth-weight dependent systematic changes in stride velocity variability indicate poorer walking performance in preterm children who were less mature at birth.

Introduction

The incidence of very preterm birth (i.e., birth before the 32nd gestational week) has been rising and survival rates of the very preterm have increased due to improved neonatal care [1]. This development is leading to an increased number of very prematurely born children who are entering public school today. However, even generally well-developing very preterm children who are enrolled in elementary school are at increased risk for several long-term sequels [2], including impairments in cognitive processes such as executive and attentional functions [35], and motor development [6], [7].

Motor skills affect important aspects of children’s development [8]. Children with poor motor skills may voluntarily withdraw from situations in which they might show their lower motor abilities [912] what may put further motor development at risk [13]. In addition, children with poor motor skills may be perceived by peers as being different or awkward, which may lead to peer rejection [1416]. Withdrawal from or exclusion by the peer group may both lead to decreased self-esteem, which in turn may increase emotional and behavioral problems [17].

However, although motor impairments have been studied in very preterm children [6], [7], there is scarce of knowledge regarding their development of walking—the most important mode of human locomotion [18].

Gait is a remarkably complex task involving neural control systems to produce coordinated limb movements [19]. Learning to walk freely is a milestone in motor development and requires a learning period of approximately 12 to 14.5 months in full-term infants [20]. Thereafter, gait is continuously stabilized until a mature walking pattern is reached at about 7 years [21]. However, there is evidence that gait continues to develop across childhood and adolescence [2224]. For example, Hausdorff, Zemany, Peng, and Goldberger [22] showed that gait variability, reflecting the automaticity, rhythmicity, and regularity of gait [25], decreased from childhood to adolescence. In a related vein, Belmonti, Cioni, and Berthoz [24] showed that curvilinear walking, i.e. walking along curved trajectories, is not fully developed before children reach 11 years of age.

Studies with preterm infants indicate that the onset of independent walking is delayed [2628]. During childhood and adolescence, preterm children show an increased risk for gross motor deficits in dynamic and static balance skills, such as heel walking and one-leg standing [5], [29], and report lower physical activity [3032] and participation in organized sports [30], [32] than full-term peers. These patterns persist into adulthood [3335].

Generally, it is acknowledged that executive and attentional functions are involved in the control of gait [36]. Executive functions refer to higher cognitive processes that include the control and allocation of attentional resources necessary for adaptive planning of behaviors [37]. Studies using a dual-task paradigm showed that gait is affected when participants are asked to walk and perform a concurrent task [38]. Studies also revealed that dual-task effects on gait variability are particularly profound in elderly individuals [39] and neurological patients [4042], who show reduced executive and attentional functions [25], [36], [4143]. Dual tasking also resulted in gait alterations in children, indicating that gait requires executive and attentional functions also during childhood [4446]. The effects of dual tasking on gait were more profound in children with deficits in executive and attentional functions, such as children with severe post-traumatic brain injury [47].

In sum, research on gait in generally well-developing very preterm children is scarce. Moreover, the role that limitations in executive and attentional functions play in gait in very preterm children is unknown.

The main goal of the present study was therefore to assess gait and concurrent task performance in very preterm and full-term children in single-, dual-, and triple-task conditions. To achieve ecological validity the dual and triple tasks included tasks that are often required in children's everyday lives while walking, such as listening to someone talking. First, we expected to find impaired gait performance in dual- and triple-task conditions compared to a single-task walking condition in preterm and full-term children. Second, we expected to find more strongly compromised gait performance in very preterm compared to full-term children in dual- and triple-task conditions due to their deficits in executive and attentional functions. Particularly, we hypothesized that very preterm children show higher gait variability than full-term children in dual- and triple-task conditions. Finally, we expected to find decreased performance on the concurrent tasks in preterm compared to full-term children, assuming that concurrent walking would distract their attentional resources more strongly [48].

Materials and Methods

Participants

Forty-nine very preterm children were recruited from an initial cohort of 260 very preterm children, born between 2001 and 2006 and postnatally treated at the University Children’s Hospital Basel (Switzerland). One hundred and two preterm children were excluded from the initial cohort due to no or incomplete information on the neurobehavioral development before age 2 or severe developmental delay, insufficient German language skills of parents to give informed consent, or residence outside of Switzerland or more than 100 km away from the study center. Additionally, 7 children could not be traced. Of the 151 parents who were contacted by phone, 49 agreed to allow their children to participate. Compared to nonparticipants, participating preterm children had a higher birth weight (1422 g vs. 1251 g, F(1,150) = 5.46, p = .02), higher gestational age (30.2 weeks vs. 29.3 weeks, F(1,150) = 6.67, p = .01), and a shorter hospital stay (46.5 days vs. 56.3 days, F(1,148) = 5.89, p = .02). Age- and sex-matched full-term children were recruited from birth announcements in newspapers and local schools. As parents of some full-term children refused participation, we were unable to recruit controls for two of the 49 preterm children. Therefore, these two preterm children were excluded from the present study. All children were screened for intellectual disability (IQ ≤ 70) using the German version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th edition [49] and for developmental coordination disorder using the German version of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd edition with a cut-off below the 16th percentile [50]. None of the children were excluded because of their IQ, whereas 3 preterm children and 1 full-term child were excluded because of significant motor impairment.

The final sample for this study consisted of 44 very preterm children (mean age = 9.48 years, 25 boys, < 32 weeks of gestation, mean birth weight = 1423 g) and 44 full-term children (mean age = 9.47 years, 24 boys, > 37 weeks of gestation, mean birth weight = 3353 g). The sample of preterm children included 20 children with very low birth weight (≤ 1500 g; mean birth weight: 1026 g) and 24 children with birth weight > 1500 g (mean birth weight: 1754 g). None of the children suffered from periventricular leukomalacia, while one of the 44 preterm children was diagnosed with mild intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH) grade 1. All children attended primary school in Switzerland. Preterm and full-term children were comparable regarding age, sex, height, weight, and leg length (Table 1).

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of preterm and full-term children.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144363.t001

To screen for executive and attentional functions, we measured selective attention using the selective attention subtest of the Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS) [51], [52]. Additionally, we measured hyperactivity-inattention with the German version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [53]. Results revealed a tendency towards lower IDS (p = .091) and higher SDQ (p = .093) scores in preterm compared to full-term children, indicating that executive and attentional functions were marginally lower in the preterm children of our study compared to full-term children.

The Ethics Committee of Basel approved the study, and it was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki. Parents gave written informed consent for the children to participate and assent was obtained from the children.

Procedure and Measures

A portable GAITRite electronic walkway system (GAITRite Platinum; CIR Systems, Sparta, New Jersey) was used for gait assessment. This system consists of a 7.01-m-long electronic mat with 23 040 integrated pressure sensors. Electronically inactive sections with a length of 1.25 m were added on each end of the system to minimize the effects of acceleration and deceleration. The reliability and validity of children’s gait assessment using GAITRite is well established [54], [55]. Analyses were performed according to the European guidelines for spatiotemporal gait analysis [56]. The following spatio-temporal gait parameters were derived: gait velocity (cm/s) which is considered a marker of general functional performance [40] and is the most common reported gait parameter; cadence, measured as steps per minute; stride length (cm) which is the distance between the heel points of two consecutive footfalls of the same foot; single support time (s) which is the time elapsed between the last contact of the current footfall and the initial contact of the next footfall of the same foot; double support time (s) which is the time when both feet are on the floor. In order to account for differences in height of the children we additionally normalized gait velocity, cadence, and stride length to dimensionless quantities using the formulas: where g is the gravitational constant (9.81 m/s2) and l is leg length [57], [58]. Finally, we measured gait variability, assessed as variability in stride velocity and stride length using the percentage coefficient of variation (standard deviation/mean × 100). Gait variability is sensitive to more subtle physiological changes and is considered to reflect the regularity, rhythmicity, and automaticity of gait [25]. Correlations between gait parameters in single-task walking are shown in Table 2 and comparable to the correlations in dual- and triple-task conditions (data not shown).

thumbnail
Table 2. Correlations between gait parameters in single-task walking (N = 88).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144363.t002

Prior to gait assessment children were asked to complete all concurrent tasks for 10 s while standing. Concurrent tasks were selected according to related dual-task research and were (1) naming animals at self-selected speed and rhythm (animals) [59], [60]; (2) listening to and memorizing digits (digits) [61], [62], where children heard a word list comprising five digits and five objects as distractors presented in randomized order from a computer over loudspeakers, installed at the front left and right corner of the laboratory. Afterward, the children were asked to recall the digits; (3) carrying a tray (45 × 30 cm) loaded with 7 table tennis balls (tray) [45], [63], [64]. The performance on this task was assessed as number of fallen balls while walking; and (4) unfastening and fastening a button [65], [66] having a diameter of 1 cm (button).

To familiarize the children with the walkway system, each child was given one demonstration and one practice trial before the recordings. A walk was approximately 10 m long and comprised 4 to 5 strides. Then, children were instructed to walk at their normal pace (single-task condition) in four trials. For each child gait parameters were averaged over the trials for further data analyses. Afterward, the children were instructed to walk at their normal pace and simultaneously perform a concurrent task (4 dual-task conditions) or 2 concurrent tasks (3 triple-task conditions) with 1 trial each. While triple-task walking, the children were asked to (1) name animals and carry a tray with balls (animals/tray); (2) listen to and memorize digits and carry a tray with balls (digits/tray) and; (3) listen to and memorize digits and unfasten and fasten a button (digits/button).

We defined “single tasks” as performing only one task at a time (i.e. walking without concurrent tasks or performing a concurrent task without walking). “Dual tasks” were defined as performing two tasks at a time (i.e. walking and one concurrent task such as naming animals), while “triple tasks” were defined as performing three tasks at a time (i.e. walking and two concurrent tasks such as naming animals and carrying a tray). In the dual- and triple-task conditions children were not instructed to prioritize any of the tasks. Task order was not randomized across the study. No randomization was conducted to avoid that some of the children had to perform the same concurrent task in an immediate sequence. Consecutive performance of the same concurrent task (e.g. naming animals) could have led to enhanced learning and memorizing effects in these children.

Statistical Analysis

First, group differences in gait parameters in the single-task condition were assessed for spatio-temporal gait parameters, normalized gait parameters, and gait variability measures using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). Second, effects of dual- and triple-task conditions on gait were examined using repeated-measures MANOVAs with 3 between-subjects factors (prematurity status: preterm ≤ 1500 g, preterm > 1500 g, full-term) and 8 within-subject factors (walking condition: one single-task, four dual-tasks, three triple-tasks) for each gait parameter. Significant effects were followed up with Bonferroni corrected post-hoc pairwise comparisons in which we focused on single-task versus dual- and triple-task comparisons. Additionally, tests for linear trend by polynomial linear contrast analysis were conducted to test for gait alterations across preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g, preterm children with birth weight > 1500 g, and full-term children. Extreme values in gait parameters defined as a z score exceeding 3 SDs from the mean were truncated to ± 3 SD.

Third, a MANOVA was performed to assess group differences in concurrent task performance during single-task condition and, finally, repeated-measures MANOVAs were conducted to examine the effects of dual- and triple-task conditions on the concurrent tasks. Significant effects were followed up with Bonferroni corrected post-hoc pairwise comparisons in which we focused on single-task versus dual- and triple-task comparisons. Tests for linear trend by polynomial linear contrast analysis were employed to test for alterations in concurrent task performance across preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g, preterm children with birth weight > 1500 g, and full-term children.

The normality of distributions of the data was assessed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. As parameters of double support time, gait variability and concurrent task performance were not normally distributed, they were log-transformed. The level of significance was set to .05, with p < .10 considered a tendency. The F statistic, p values (two-tailed), and effect sizes (η2) are reported.

Results

For spatio-temporal gait parameters, means and standard deviations in single-, dual-, and triple-task conditions are shown in Table 3. In the single-task condition the MANOVA showed no significant group differences in these gait parameters (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(10,162) = 0.870, p = .563, η2 = 0.051). Repeated-measures MANOVAs revealed a significant within-subject effect of walking condition on each spatio-temporal gait parameter (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(7,73) = 22.983 to 103.825, p < .001, η2 = 0.691 to 0.909). Pairwise comparisons revealed lower gait velocity, lower cadence, lower stride length as well as higher single support time and higher double support time in dual- and triple-task conditions compared to single-task walking (p < .01) with exception of the dual-task condition button in which children showed comparable single support time compared to single-task walking. There were no significant main effects of prematurity status or Walking Condition × Prematurity Status interactions.

thumbnail
Table 3. Means (and standard deviations) of spatio-temporal gait parameters for preterm and full-term children in single-, dual-, and triple-task conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144363.t003

For normalized gait parameters, means and standard deviations in single-, dual-, and triple-task conditions are shown in Table 4. In the single-task condition the MANOVA showed no significant group differences in these gait parameters (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(6,166) = 0.692, p = .656, η2 = 0.024). Repeated-measures MANOVAs revealed a significant within-subject effect of walking condition on each normalized gait parameter (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(7,73) = 37.336 to 95.504, p < .001, η2 = 0.784 to 0.902). Pairwise comparisons revealed lower normalized velocity, lower normalized cadence, and lower normalized stride length in dual- and triple-task conditions compared to single-task walking (p < .001). There were no significant main effects of prematurity status or Walking Condition × Prematurity Status interactions.

thumbnail
Table 4. Means (and standard deviations) of normalized gait parameters for preterm and full-term children in single-, dual-, and triple-task conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144363.t004

Table 5 shows means and standard deviations of gait variability measures in single-, dual-, and triple-task conditions. In the single-task condition the MANOVA showed no significant group differences in the gait variability parameters (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(4,168) = 0.599, p = .664, η2 = 0.014). Statistical results from the repeated-measures MANOVAs for each gait parameter are presented in Table 6. For gait variability, results revealed a significant within-subject effect of walking condition on stride velocity variability (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(7,72) = 30.200, p < .001, η2 = 0.746) and stride length variability (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(7,72) = 19.003, p < .001, η2 = 0.649). Pairwise comparisons revealed higher stride velocity variability and higher stride length variability in dual- and triple-task conditions compared to single-task walking (p < .05) with exception of the dual-task condition digits in which children showed comparable stride length variability compared to single-task walking.

thumbnail
Table 5. Means (and standard deviations) of gait variability parameters for preterm and full-term children in single-, dual-, and triple-task conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144363.t005

thumbnail
Table 6. Statistical results from the repeated-measures MANOVAs comparing the single- to the dual- and triple-task conditions for each gait parameter.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144363.t006

For stride velocity variability there was a tendency for a between-subjects effect of prematurity status, F(2,78) = 3.021, p = .088, η2 = 0.060, with post-hoc comparisons showing that preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g walked with marginally significantly higher gait variability compared to controls (p = .084). Additionally, polynomial contrasts revealed a linear trend across all walking conditions (p = .028), indicating systematically decreasing gait variability from preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g to preterm children with birth weight > 1500 g to full-term children. The significance of this multivariate linear trend was driven by univariate linear trends in the walking conditions digits (p = .069), digits/tray (p = .035), and digits/button (p = .044), as shown in Fig 1. There were no significant Walking Condition × Prematurity Status interactions.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Means and standard errors for gait variability including stride velocity variability (A) and stride length variability (B) for preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g and > 1500 g, and full-term children in single-, dual-, and triple-task conditions.

Concurrent tasks were naming animals (animals), listening to and memorizing digits (digits), carrying a tray with table tennis balls (tray), and unfastening and fastening a button (button). P values are presented for significant main effects of walking conditions (comparing single task vs. dual and triple tasks) and for linear trends showing increasing gait performance from preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g to preterm children with birth weight > 1500 g to full-term children. For statistical analyses log-transformed parameters of gait variability were used.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144363.g001

Means and standard errors for task performance in single-, dual- and triple-task conditions are shown in Fig 2. As both groups of children showed no variance in the tray concurrent task, it was excluded from further analyses. In single-task conditions the MANOVA revealed a significant effect of prematurity status (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(6,148) = 2.473, p = .026, η2 = 0.091). Follow-up univariate tests showed a significant group difference for animals (F(2,76) = 6.277, p = .003, η2 = 0.142). Post-hoc pairwise tests showed that preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g named fewer animals than controls (p = .004). Additionally, the test for linear trend was significant, indicating systematically increasing task performance from preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g to preterm children with birth weight > 1500 g to full-term children (p = .001).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Means and standard errors for task performance for preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g and > 1500 g, and full-term children in single-, dual-, and triple-task conditions.

Measures were number of named animals (animals), recalled digits (digits), and how many times a button could be unfastened and fastened (button). Performance in the tray concurrent task was excluded from the figure as both groups showed no variance in this task. P values are presented for significant main effects of walking conditions (comparing single task vs. dual- and triple tasks) and for linear trends showing increasing gait performance from preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g to preterm children with birth weight > 1500 g to full-term children. For statistical analyses log-transformed parameters of task performance were used.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144363.g002

Repeated-measures MANOVAs revealed a significant within-subject effect of walking condition on naming animals (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(2,83) = 46.299, p < .001, η2 = 0.527). Pairwise comparisons showed that children named fewer animals in the triple-task condition compared to single-task walking (p < .001). There was no main effect of prematurity status nor a significant Walking Condition × Prematurity Status interaction.

Further, there was a significant within-subject effect of walking condition on listening to and memorizing digits (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(3,64) = 16.033, p < .001, η2 = 0.429). Pairwise comparisons showed that children recalled fewer digits in the triple-task conditions compared to single-task walking (p < .01). There was no main effect of prematurity status nor a significant Walking Condition × Prematurity Status interaction.

Finally, there was a significant within-subject effect of walking condition on unfastening and fastening a button (Wilks’ multivariate test, F(2,80) = 7.341, p = .001, η2 = 0.155). Pairwise comparisons showed that in the triple-task condition children unfastened and fastened a button less often than in single-task walking (p < .05). There was no main effect of prematurity status nor a significant Walking Condition × Prematurity Status interaction.

Discussion

In everyday life children often do things concomitantly with walking, such as fastening jacket buttons or listening to someone talking, which results in less attention that can be directed to the control of gait. As very preterm children show deficits in executive and attentional functions, the aim of the study was to investigate for the first time gait alterations in dual- and triple-task conditions in very preterm and full-term children during middle childhood.

Our results are consistent with the notion that gait requires executive and attentional functions in children [4446]: Concurrent information processing in dual- and triple-task conditions interfered with gait, leading to lower gait velocity, lower stride length and higher gait variability compared to single-task walking in both preterm and full-term children.

Regarding prematurity status the results showed systematic decreases in stride velocity variability from preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g to preterm children with birth weight > 1500 g to full-term children, which is in line with research showing that motor impairments occur more frequently in preterm children who were less mature at birth [7], [67]. No significant group differences, however, were revealed in spatio-temporal gait parameters, normalized gait parameters or stride length variability. The result that there were significant group differences in gait variability but not in gait velocity is in line with the notion that gait variability may provide a more discriminant and sensitive measure of gait than other gait variables [25].

Higher gait variability is also present in individuals who exhibit impairments in executive and attentional functions [25], [36], [4143]. Therefore, deficits in executive and attentional functions in preterm children [35] may contribute to the alterations in stride velocity variability in preterm children found in this study. However, the underlying mechanisms for dual task interference in preterm children are not clear. In accordance with the capacity-sharing theory [68] it may be possible that more limited attentional resources in preterm compared to full-term children [69] lead to impaired gait or concurrent task performance as soon as the demands on attention exceed a certain threshold. On the other hand it might be possible that preterm children have more difficulties in switching from one task to the other compared to full-term children [69], which may lead to diminished performance in one or both of the tasks. This notion is derived from the bottleneck theory [70] proposing that two simultaneously performed tasks are cognitively processed sequentially which poses high demands on the capacity to switch between tasks.

In the present study stride velocity variability increased with decreasing maturity of the children at birth, which was most apparent in conditions in which the children had to listen to and memorize digits. This finding appears consistent with results from Huang et al. [46] who found that a concurrent auditory task showed the largest interference effect on gait in five- to seven-year-old children. These authors’ interpretation of their findings was that it is particularly difficult to walk and concurrently perform a task requiring continuous processing of new auditory information. Regarding concurrent task performance, the concurrent performance in naming animals was also related to birth weight such that there was a systematic increase in the number of named animals from preterm children with birth weight ≤ 1500 g to preterm children with birth weight > 1500 g to full-term children, what is in line with previous research [1]. Further, preterm children named fewer animals than full-term children while single tasking. This is in accordance with previous research showing that preterm children scored lower than full-term children in word fluency tests, which are an established measure of executive function [6]. Moreover, it is in line with a recent study showing that the gap in cognitive performance of preterm compared to full-term children systematically increased with increasing cognitive workload of the tasks [71].

However, it remains to be determined why effects of prematurity status were particularly apparent in auditory and verbal fluency conditions. One possible explanation may lie in preterm children’s alterations of interhemispheric integration. Recently, Belmonti, Berthoz, Cioni, Fiori and Guzzetta [72] studied 22 children with cerebral palsy of which 10 children were born premature. In these children locomotor navigation was affected by lesions involving the right frontal lobes, indicating that spatial memory in navigation might depend on right-lateralized networks. Further, there is evidence that cerebral connectivity is altered in auditory language functions in preterm children [73]. Therefore, it is of interest for future studies if such alterations in connectivity in preterm children are also evident in locomotor navigation tasks as they might underlie preterm children's difficulties in walking and concurrently performing an auditory or a verbal fluency task.

From studies with elderly individuals it is known that particularly increased gait variability is associated with a higher risk of falling [34]. However, we are not aware of studies that have examined associations between gait variability and accidents or physical activity in childhood and adolescence. On the other hand, there is evidence that preterm children are less physically active and participate less often in organized sports from childhood through young adulthood [3035], which may increase their risk for poor cardiovascular outcomes in later life [33]. It is possible that organized sports provide less reinforcing experiences for preterm children due to their more limited walking performance, which may eventually direct them toward less physically active leisure activities. However, the error variance within the assessed groups was high and effect sizes for group differences between preterm and full-term children were small and not consistent across all gait parameters. Therefore, it remains to be shown in future research whether the small differences in gait patterns between preterm and full-term children are meaningful for everyday life and associated with other aspects of children’s development.

Our study has strengths and limitations. We consider it a strength of this study that gait was assessed using an objective electronic gait assessment system with proven reliability and validity [54], [55] for gait assessment when children are wearing their normal clothes and shoes, making it possible to assess children’s gait as it is exhibited in their everyday lives. However, the number of walks per condition was limited and increasing the number of walks might have increased reliability of gait measures. Therefore, replication in other samples of preterm children is important to exclude the possibility that the significant group differences found in the present study were due to the limited number of walks. Further, we analyzed spatio-temporal, normalized, and variability measures of straight walking as the walkway system used in our study did not allow the capture of gait kinematics [74] or the assessment of curvilinear walking [24]. Preterm children participating in our study had higher birth weight and gestational age than nonparticipants, which may lead to underestimation of the effects of dual and triple tasking on gait as cognitive and motor deficits are more profound in less maturely born preterm children [7], [67]. Finally, the cross-sectional design precludes the identification of developmental changes. A longitudinal research approach may be taken to examine whether the gait alterations in very preterm children have to be seen as maturational delay, i.e. that gait matures later in preterm compared to full-term children, or rather as a persistent deviance from the gait pattern of full-term children, i.e. that preterm children do not achieve the maturation of full-term children. In this line, it has been suggested that poorer performance in executive functions of preterm compared to full-term children might reflect a developmental delay rather than a deviance [75].

Conclusion

Results of this study support the role of executive and attentional functions in the control of gait. Further, the results of our study indicate that preterm children who were less mature at birth walk with higher stride velocity variability. The relevance of these results for everyday motor activity of preterm children as well as whether early developmental intervention training programs focusing on executive function and/or motor behavior [76] could improve gait of preterm children should be the topic of further studies.

Supporting Information

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: PH OM NP AG SL. Performed the experiments: PH OM NP. Analyzed the data: PH OM NP PW AG SL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: PH OM NP PW AG SL. Wrote the paper: PH OM NP PW AG SL.

References

  1. 1. Saigal S, Doyle LW. An overview of mortality and sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood. Lancet. 2008;371: 261–269. pmid:18207020
  2. 2. Lemola S. Long-term outcomes of very preterm birth. Mechanisms and interventions. Eur Psychol. 2015;20: 128–137.
  3. 3. Aarnoudse-Moens CSH, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2009;124: 717–728. pmid:19651588
  4. 4. Mulder H, Pitchford NJ, Hagger MS, Marlow N. Development of executive function and attention in preterm children: a systematic review. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34: 393–421. pmid:20183707
  5. 5. Hagmann-von Arx P, Perkinson-Gloor N, Brand S, Albert D, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Grob A, Weber P, Lemola S. In school-age children who were born very preterm sleep efficiency is associated with cognitive function. Neuropsychobiology. 2014;70: 244–252. pmid:25720547
  6. 6. Goyen TA, Lui K. Developmental coordination disorder in “apparently normal” school children born extremely preterm. Arch Dis Child. 2009;94: 298–302. pmid:18838419
  7. 7. De Kievit JF, Piek JP, Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Oosterlaan J. Motor development in very preterm and very low-birth-weight children from birth to adolescence. JAMA. 2009;302: 2235–2242. pmid:19934425
  8. 8. Piek JP, Baynam GB, Barett NC. The relationship between fine and gross motor ability, self-perception and self-worth in children and adolescents. Hum Movement Sci. 2006:25; 65–75.
  9. 9. Cantell M, Smyth M, Ahonen T. Clumsiness in adolescence: educational, motor, and social outcomes of motor delay detected at 5 years. Adapt Phys Activ Q. 1994;11: 115–129.
  10. 10. Piek JP, Dworcan M, Barret N, Coleman R. Determinants of self-worth in children with developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neur. 2000;37: 976–984.
  11. 11. Schoemaker MM, Kalverboer AF. Social and affective problems of children who are clumsy: how early do they begin. Adapt Phys Activ Q. 2004;11: 130–140.
  12. 12. Skinner RA, Piek JP. Psychosocial implications of poor motor coordination in children and adolescents. Hum Mov Sci. 2001;20: 73–94. pmid:11471399
  13. 13. Bouffard M, Watkinson EJ, Thompson LP, Causgrove Dunn JL, Romanow SKE. (1996). A test of the activity deficit hypothesis with children with movement difficulties. Adapt Phys Activ Q. 1996;13: 61–73.
  14. 14. Bejerot S, Humble MB. Childhood clumsiness and peer victimization: A case-control study of psychiatric patients. BMC Psychiatry. 2013;13: 68. pmid:23442984
  15. 15. Bejerot S, Plenty S, Humble A, Humble MB. Poor motor skills: A risk marker for bully victimization. Aggressive Behav. 2013;39: 453–461.
  16. 16. Piek JP, Barrett NC, Allen LSR, Jones A, Louise M. The relationship between bullying and self-worth in children with movement coordination problems. Br J Educ Psychol. 2005;75: 453–463. pmid:16238876
  17. 17. Zwicker JG, Missiuna C, Harris SR, Boyd LA. Developmental coordination disorder: A review and update. Europ J Paediatr Neuro. 2012;16: 573–581.
  18. 18. Adolph KE, Vereijken B, Shrout PE. What changes in infant walking and why. Child Dev. 2003;74: 475–497. pmid:12705568
  19. 19. Hausdorff JM. Gait dynamics, fractals and falls: finding meaning in the stride-to-stride fluctuations of human walking. Hum Mov Sci. 2007;26: 555–589. pmid:17618701
  20. 20. Storvold GV, Aarethun K, Bratberg GH. Age for onset of walking and prewalking strategies. Early Human Dev. 2013;89: 655–659.
  21. 21. Jeng SF, Hua-Fang L, Jin-Shin L, Jia-Woei H. Optimization of walking in children. Med Sci Sports. 1997;29: 370–376.
  22. 22. Hausdorff JM, Zemany L, Peng CK, Goldberger AL. Maturation of gait dynamics: stride-to-stride variability and its temporal organization in children. J Appl Physiol. 1999;86: 1040–1047. pmid:10066721
  23. 23. Lythgo N, Wilson C, Gaela M. Basic gait and symmetry measures for primary school-aged children and young adults whilst walking barefoot and with shoes. Gait Posture. 2009;30: 502–506. pmid:19692245
  24. 24. Belmonti V, Cioni G, Berthoz A. Development of anticipatory orienting strategies and trajectory formation in goal-oriented locomotion. Exp Brain Res. 2013;227: 131–147. pmid:23588420
  25. 25. Hausdorff JM. Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2005;2: 19. pmid:16033650
  26. 26. Jeng SF, Lau TW, Hsieh WS, Luo HJ, Chen PS, Lin KW et al. Development of walking in preterm and term infants: age of onset, qualitative features and sensitivity to resonance. Gait Posture. 2008;27: 340–346. pmid:17532638
  27. 27. Nuysink J, van Haastert IC, Eijsermans MJC, Koopman-Esseboom C, Helders PJM, de Vries LS et al. Prediction of gross motor development and independent walking in infants born very preterm using the Test of Infant Motor Performance and the Alberta Infant Motor Scale. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89: 693–697. pmid:23712056
  28. 28. Marin Gabriel MA, Pallas Alonso CR, De La Cruz Bertolo J, Caserio Carbonero S, Lopez Maestro M, Moral Pumarega M et al. Age of sitting unsupported and independent walking in very low birth weight preterm infants with normal motor development at 2 years. Acta Paediatr. 2009;98: 1815–1821. pmid:19807707
  29. 29. Marlow N, Hennessy EM, Bracewell MA, Wolke D. Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics. 2007;120: 793–803. pmid:17908767
  30. 30. Rogers M, Fay TB, Whitfield MF, Tomlinson J, Grunau RE. Aerobic capacity, strength, flexibility, and activity level in unimpaired extremely low birth weight (≤800 g) survivors at 17 years of age compared with term-born control subjects. Pediatrics. 2005;116: e58. pmid:15997047
  31. 31. Clemm H, Roksund O, Thorsen E, Eide GE, Markestad T, Halvorsen T. Aerobic capacity and exercise performance in young people born extremely preterm. Pediatrics. 2012;129: e97–e105. pmid:22201154
  32. 32. Finn K, Johannsen N, Specker B. Factors associated with physical activity in preschool children. J Pediatr. 2002;140: 81–85. pmid:11815768
  33. 33. Kajantie E, Strang-Karlsson S, Hovi P, Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K et al. Adults born at very low birth weight exercise less than their peers born at term. J Pediatr. 2010;157: 610–616. pmid:20493499
  34. 34. Saigal S, Stoskopf B, Boyle M, Paneth N, Pinelli J, Streiner D et al. Comparison of current health, functional limitations, and health care use of young adults who were born with extremely low birth weight and normal birth weight. Pediatrics. 2007;119: e562–e573. pmid:17332176
  35. 35. Odberg MD. Low birth weight young adults: quality of life, academic achievements and social functioning. Acta Paediatr. 2001;100: 284–288.
  36. 36. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord. 2008;23: 329–342. pmid:18058946
  37. 37. Anderson V. Executive function in children: Introduction. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;8: 69–70. pmid:12638060
  38. 38. Huang HJ, Mercer VS. Dual-task methodology: applications in studies of cognitive and motor performance in adults and children. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2001;13: 133–140. 0898-5669/01/1303-0133 pmid:17053670
  39. 39. Beurskens R, Bock O. Age-related deficits of dual-task walking: a review. Neural Plast. 2012;2012: 131608. pmid:22848845
  40. 40. Al-Yahya E, Dawes H, Smith L, Dennis A, Howells D, Cockburn J. Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35: 715–728. pmid:20833198
  41. 41. Sheridan PL, Solomont J. Kowall N, Hausdorff JM. Influence of executive function on locomotor function: divided attention increases gait variability in Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51: 1633–1637. pmid:14687395
  42. 42. Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22: 1248–1256. pmid:16176368
  43. 43. Hausdorff JM, Yogev G, Springer S, Simon ES, Giladi N. Walking is more like catching than tapping: gait in the elderly as a complex cognitive task. Exp Brain Res. 2005;164: 541–548. pmid:15864565
  44. 44. Abbruzzese LD, Rao AK, Bellows R, Figueroa K, Levy J, Lim E, et al. Effects of manual task complexity on gait parameters in school-aged children and adults. Gait Posture. 2014;40: 658–663. pmid:25168911
  45. 45. Cherng RJ, Liang LY, Hwang IS, Chen JY. The effect of a concurrent task on walking performance of preschool children. Gait Posture. 2007;26: 231–237. pmid:17045803
  46. 46. Huang HJ, Mercer VS, Thorpe DE. Effects of different concurrent cognitive tasks on temporal-distance gait variables in children. Pediatr Phys Ther 2003;15: 105–113. pmid:17057440
  47. 47. Katz-Leurer M, Rotem H, Keren O, Meyer S. Effect of concurrent cognitive tasks on gait features among children post-severe traumatic brain injury and typically-developed controls. Brain Injury. 2011;25: 581–586. pmid:21534735
  48. 48. Yogev-Seligman G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. Do we always prioritize balance when walking? Towards an integrated model of task prioritization. Mov Disord. 2012;27: 765–770. pmid:22419512
  49. 49. Petermann F, Petermann U. (Eds.) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV). Frankfurt, Germany: Pearson Assessment; 2011.
  50. 50. Petermann F. (Ed.) Movement Assessment Battery for Children. 2nd ed. Frankfurt, Germany: Pearson Assessment; 2008.
  51. 51. Grob A, Meyer CS, Hagmann-von Arx P. Intelligence and Development Scales. Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber; 2009.
  52. 52. Hagmann-von Arx P, Grob A, Petermann F, Daseking M. Concurrent validity of the HAWIK-IV and the Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS). Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2012;40: 41–50. pmid:22161941
  53. 53. Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38: 581e6.
  54. 54. Thorpe DE, Dusing SC, Moore CG. Repeatability of temporospatial gait measures in children using the GAITRite electronic walkway. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2005;86: 2342–2346. pmid:16344033
  55. 55. Rinehart NJ, Tonge BJ, Iansek R, McGinley J, Brereton AV, Enticott PG, et al. Gait function in newly diagnosed children with autism: cerebellar and basal ganglia related motor disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2007;48: 819–820.
  56. 56. Kressig RW, Beauchet O. European GAITRite Network Group. Guidelines for clinical applications of spatio-temporal gait analysis in older adults. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2006;18: 174–176. pmid:16702791
  57. 57. Hof AL. Scaling gait data to body size. Gait Posture. 1996;4: 222–223.
  58. 58. Stansfield BW, Hillman SJ, Hazlewood ME, Lawson AM, Mann AM, Loudon IR, Robb JE. Normalisation of gait data in children. Gait Posture. 2003;17: 81–87. pmid:12535730
  59. 59. Dubost V, Kressig RW, Gonthier R, Herrmann FR, Aminian K, Najafi B, et al. Relationships between dual-task related changes in stride velocity and stride time variability in healthy older adults. Hum Mov Sci. 2006;25: 372–382. pmid:16714067
  60. 60. Muhaidat J, Kerr A, Evans JJ, Skelton DA. Exploring gait-related dual-task tests in community-dwelling fallers and non-faller: a pilot study. Physiother Theory Pract. 2013;29: 351–370. pmid:23289962
  61. 61. Leitner Y, Barak R, Giladi N, Peretz C, Eshel R, Gruendlinger L, et al. Gait in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of methylphenidate and dual tasking. J Neurol. 2007;254: 1330–1138. pmid:17401735
  62. 62. Lindenberger U, Marsiske M, Baltes P. Memorizing while walking: increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. Psychol Aging. 2000;15: 417–436. pmid:11014706
  63. 63. Bloem BR, Valkenburg VV, Slabbekoorn M, van Dijk JG. The multiple tasks test. Strategies in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2001;137: 478–486. pmid:11355392
  64. 64. Cherng RJ, Liang LY, Chen YJ, Chen YJ. The effects of a motor and a cognitive concurrent task on walking in children with developmental coordination disorder. Gait Posture. 2009;29: 204–207. pmid:18809330
  65. 65. Ebersbach G, Dimitrijevic MR, Poewe W. Influence of concurrent tasks on gait: a dual-task approach. Percept Mot Skills. 1995;81: 107–113. pmid:8532444
  66. 66. Yang YR, Chen YC, Lee CS, Cheng SJ, Wang RY. Dual-task-related gait changes in individuals with stroke. Gait Posture. 2007;25: 185–190. pmid:16650766
  67. 67. Erikson C, Allert C, Carlberg EB, Katz-Salomon M. Stability of longitudinal motor development in very low birthweight infants from 5 months to 5.5 years. Acta Paediatr. 2003;92: 197–203. pmid:12710646
  68. 68. Tombu M, Jolicœur P. A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2003;29: 3–18. pmid:12669744
  69. 69. Anderson PJ, De Luca CR, Hutchinson E, Spencer-Smith MM, Roberts G, Doyle LW, Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. Attention problems in a representative sample of extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2011;36: 57–73. pmid:21253991
  70. 70. Pashler H. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychol Bull. 1994;116: 220–244. pmid:7972591
  71. 71. Jaekel J, Baumann N, Wolke D. Effects of gestational age at birth on cognitive performance: A function of cognitive workload demands. Plos One. 2013;8: e65219. pmid:23717694
  72. 72. Belmonti V, Berthoz A, Cioni G, Fiori S, Guzzetta A. Navigation strategies as revealed by error patterns on the Magic Carpet test in children with cerebral palsy. Front Psychol. 2015;6: 880. pmid:26217250
  73. 73. Gozzo Y, Vohr B, Lacadie C, Hampson M, Katz KH, Maller-Kesselman J, Schneider KC, Peterson BS, Rajeevan N, Makuch RW, Constable RT, Ment LR. Alterations in neural connectivity in preterm children at school age. NeuroImage. 2009;48: 458–463. pmid:19560547
  74. 74. Chester VL, Tingley M, Biden EN. A comparison of kinetic gait parameters for 3–13 year olds. Clin Biomech. 2006;21: 726–732.
  75. 75. Ritter BC, Nelle M, Perrig W, Steinlin M, Everts R. Executive functions of children born very preterm–deficit or delay? Eur J Pediatr. 2013;172: 473–483. pmid:23247616
  76. 76. Spittle A, Orton J, Anderson P, Boyd R, Doyle LW. Early developmental intervention programes post-hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairments in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012;12: CD005495. pmid:23235624