Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Relative Deprivation, Poverty, and Subjective Health: JAGES Cross-Sectional Study

  • Masashige Saito ,

    masa-s@n-fukushi.ac.jp

    Affiliation Department of Social Welfare, Nihon Fukushi University, Aichi, Japan

  • Katsunori Kondo,

    Affiliation Center for Preventive Medical Science, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan

  • Naoki Kondo,

    Affiliation Department of Health Economics and Epidemiology Research, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan

  • Aya Abe,

    Affiliation Department of Empirical Social Security Research, National Institute of Population and Social Security Research, Tokyo, Japan

  • Toshiyuki Ojima,

    Affiliation Department of Community Health and Preventive Medicine, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Shizuoka, Japan

  • Kayo Suzuki,

    Affiliation Department of Social Studies, Aichi Gakuin University, Aichi, Japan

  • the JAGES group

    Membership of the JAGES project is provided in the Acknowledgments.

Abstract

To evaluate the association between relative deprivation (lacking daily necessities) and subjective health in older Japanese adults, we performed a cross-sectional analysis using data from the Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study (JAGES). The data were obtained from functionally independent residents aged ≥65 years from 24 municipalities in Japan (n = 24,742). Thirteen items in three dimensions were used to evaluate relative deprivation of material conditions. Approximately 28% of older Japanese people indicated that they lacked some daily necessities (non-monetary poverty). A two-level Poisson regression analysis revealed that relative deprivation was associated with poor self-rated health (PR = 1.3–1.5) and depressive symptoms (PR = 1.5–1.8) in both men and women, and these relationships were stronger than those observed in people living in relative poverty (monetary poverty). The interaction effect between relative deprivation and relative poverty was not associated with poor health. As a dimension of the social determinants of health, poverty should be evaluated from a multidimensional approach, capturing not only monetary conditions but also material-based, non-monetary conditions.

Introduction

The association between poverty and health has been established. A number of studies have revealed that relative income poverty is significantly related to poor health [1][3]. However, this approach has limitations when attempting to capture the diverse and complex aspects of poverty. In reality, older people tend to have comparatively high-quality living conditions due to savings and property ownership, even if their income is low [4]. To overcome this limitation, poverty research has proposed the concept of relative deprivation in material conditions to reflect the multidimensional non-monetary aspects of poverty [5][15]. Townsend [5] developed 60 relative deprivation indices within 12 dimensions composed of items such as “Household does not have a refrigerator” and “Has not had a week’s holiday away from home in the last 12 months,” and found that poverty in the United Kingdom was more extensive than generally believed or officially reported. The Europe 2020 strategy has adopted the concept of relative deprivation as a material dimension of social exclusion and has set the elimination of severe material deprivation as a goal for the next decade [16].

In investigating relationships between poverty and health, some studies have applied a social indicator approach, such as the Carstairs deprivation score or Townsend deprivation index, to include unemployment rate or proportion of non-car ownership. These previous studies found that relatively deprived areas were associated with standardized mortality rates [17][20], cancer mortality rates [21], suicide rates [22], coronary heart disease [23], dental caries [24], number of sound teeth remaining [25], and depression symptoms [26]. However, there have been few studies analyzing the relationship between an individual’s relative deprivation and his or her health. Some studies have shown an association between deprivation in living conditions and poor health [14], low levels of satisfaction with life [27], and poor social support [28]. Furthermore, no studies investigating the different associations between relative deprivation, relative poverty, and health have been conducted, although it has been suggested that people who live in relative deprivation have different characteristics than those living in relative poverty [9], [13], [15]. In order to clarify the relationships between poverty and health, it is important to identify the aspects of poverty that are the strongest predictors of health.

In addition, there have been few studies focusing on relative deprivation among older adults [8], [15]. Previous studies comparing younger and older people found that both the prevalence and depth of deprivation were more severe in younger people or single parents of working age [4], [13], [29]. However, from a life course perspective, the impact of relative deprivation on health should be evidenced in older people because the influence of poverty on health may accumulate over time. Finally, with the exception of a study that analyzed 131,335 people in 11 countries [10], most of the previous related research has been conducted on a relatively small scale. Analysis of relative deprivation should be performed with a large sample in order to derive robust findings, as few cases fall into each of the deprivation indices.

The present study posed three research questions: (1) is relative deprivation associated with poor health, even after monetary poverty is controlled for; (2) is the interaction effect between relative deprivation and relative poverty significantly associated with poor health, and (3) do older people with social support have good health, even if they are relatively deprived? In social psychology and sociology, the concept of relative deprivation has also been used to measure psychosomatic stress related to complaints or dissatisfaction based on comparisons with their reference groups [30][38]. However, our focus on relative deprivation is based on Townsend’s definition used in the poverty and social policy research described above.

Methods

Study samples

We used data from the Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study (JAGES), which was cross-sectional in design. JAGES was a postal survey of 112,123 people aged ≥65 who were randomly selected from the older residents of 31 municipalities in Japan. Data were collected from August 2010 to January 2012, with a response rate of 66.3%. For our study, we included 24,742 respondents from 24 municipalities who answered a relative deprivation questionnaire. The average age of the respondents was 74.6 years [standard deviation (SD) = 6.4] and 54.1% were women. Our study protocol and questionnaire procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee in Research of Human Subjects at Nihon Fukushi University. Written informed consent was assumed with voluntary return of the questionnaire.

Dependent variables

We used self-rated health and depressive symptoms as indicators of subjective health. Self-rated health and depressive symptoms have been found to be highly valid predictors of mortality, regardless of other medical, behavioral, or psychosocial factors [39], [40]. Self-rated health was measured using the question “How do you feel about your current health status: excellent, good, fair, or poor?” Responses were recoded into dichotomous response variables (excellent/good or fair/poor). Depressive symptoms were assessed using the short version of the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15), which was developed for self-administration in the community using a simple yes/no format [41], [42]. The validity and reliability of this scale have also been confirmed for Japanese older people, and it is often used in Japanese surveys [43], [44]. According to Sheikh et al. [42], scores of ≥5 on the GDS-15 indicated the presence of depressive symptoms of mild to severe depression. Our data showed approximately 30% of respondents had depressive symptoms. It is consistent with preceding Japanese studies [43], [44].

Independent variables: relative deprivation and relative poverty

The indicators of relative deprivation used in preceding studies differ, because the standard living condition or decent life varies by culture and times. In reference to preceding research including Japanese studies [5][8], [10][12], [14], we evaluated thirteen indices that equated “lack of daily necessities,” “lack of living environment,” and “lack of social life due to economic reasons” with a low standard of living. Although lack of access to medical and health care services is another important element of standard of living, this was excluded from our deprivation indices because we assumed it to be directly reflected in poor health. On the other hand, we included experience of cutoff of essential services as a typical condition of lacking decent life, which is also used as a measure of social exclusion in a Japanese study [45].

Indicators of “lack of daily necessities” included having no television, refrigerator, air conditioner, microwave oven, or water heater. “Lack of living environment” indicators included having no private toilet, kitchen, or bathroom in the house and having a dining room that was not separate from the bedroom. “Lack of social life due to economic reasons” indicators included having no telephone or formal dress, being absent from family celebrations and events during the previous year due to economic reasons, and having essential services such as water, electricity or gas, cut off in the previous year (except in cases of forgetting to make a payment).

Relative poverty was defined as an income of less than half of the median annual equivalent income in the National Survey of Family Income and Expenditure in Japan [46]; the threshold was 1.49 million Japanese yen. This is the definition of relative poverty accepted by the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development and is conceptually based on the relative approach of the Luxembourg Income Study [47]. We used annual pre-tax household income. For each response, we calculated the equivalent household income by dividing income by the square root of the number of household members. Responses were categorized into three groups: poverty (28.2%), non-poverty (51.7%), and unknown (20.1%).

Covariates

We used several control variables at the individual level: sex, age, educational attainment (to represent ascribed and achieved statuses), medical treatment (to represent recent physical condition), self-recognition of forgetfulness (to represent prodrome of dementia), and social support (to represent a buffer between poverty and health). Medical treatment was determined by asking “Are you currently receiving any medical treatment?” Self-recognition of forgetfulness was measured by asking “Do people around you notice your forgetfulness, for example, by telling you that you often ask the same thing?” Social support was measured using two questions representing emotional and institutional support: “Do you have someone who listens to your concerns and complaints?” and “Do you have someone who looks after you when you are sick and have to stay in bed for a few days?” Responses of “no” or “nobody” to both questions indicated an absence of social support.

In our data set, individuals were nested within each municipality. Previous studies reported significant associations between individual health and regional characteristics such as social capital and income inequality [48], [49]. We used the proportion of older people residing in the area (%), population density in inhabitable areas (1,000 person/km2), and the proportion of persons receiving public assistance (‰) for characteristics at the municipal level. These variables were based on 2010 census and government data for Japan. The distribution of these variables is shown is Table 1.

Statistical analysis

First, we confirmed the distribution of the relative deprivation index and calculated crude odds ratios for subjective health. Second, we applied two-level Poisson regression analysis of random-intercept and fixed-slope models to assess the relationship between relative deprivation and self-rated health and depressive symptoms, adjusting for individual factors and municipal-level covariates (level 1: 24,742 individuals, level 2: 24 municipalities). We adopted multilevel modeling in order to control for intraclass (municipal level) cluster correlation. We also examined the interaction effect of relative deprivation and relative poverty. Individual and municipality fixed parameters were converted to prevalence ratios (PR) with a 95% confidence interval (95%CI). Finally, we calculated the proportion of poor health among deprived people with social support. We used the computer software, STATA 12.1 for all analyses.

Results

Table 2 showed that 1.6% to 8.4% of respondents lived in deprived conditions, as defined by the study parameters. A higher percentage of respondents did not have a dining room separate from the bedroom (14.9%). Univariate analysis showed that ORs for respondents living in deprived conditions, according to each relative deprivation index, were approximately 1.3–2.5 times higher for fair/poor self-rated health and 1.7–4.1 times higher for depressive symptoms than respondents who did not live in relative deprivation. In particular, the crude ORs for having essential services cut off and absence from family celebrations and events were relatively high. Relative deprivation scores also showed that respondents who were deprived of one item (14.3%) and two or more items (13.6%) were more likely to report fair/poor self-rated health [OR = 1.61 (95%CI: 1.48–1.75) and OR = 1.86 (95%CI: 1.70–2.02), respectively] and depressive symptoms [OR = 1.93 (95%CI: 1.77–2.10) and OR = 2.57 (95%CI: 2.35–2.81), respectively].

Table 3 shows the associations between subjective health and a combination of relative deprivation and relative poverty. The proportion of respondents with poor health was high with respect to “poverty and deprivation,” “deprivation only,” “poverty only,” and “no deprivation or poverty.” Odds ratios for respondents living in poverty and deprivation (two or more deprivation items) were 2.51 (95%CI: 2.21–2.86) and 3.53 (95%CI: 3.10–4.02) times higher for fair/poor self-rated health and depressive symptoms, respectively, than ORs for respondents with no deprivation or poverty.

thumbnail
Table 3. Combination of relative deprivation and poverty.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0111169.t003

Table 4 shows the results of a two-level Poisson regression analysis. Random effects showed that municipal-level variance in each model was smaller than that in the null model. This means that part of the municipal-level variance was explained by the individual- and municipal-level variables in the model. Fixed effects showed similar associations for both genders. Respondents with low educational attainment, no social support, under medical treatment, and prodrome of dementia tended to have poor subjective health, although relationships between age and municipal level variables were not consistent.

thumbnail
Table 4. Association of subjective health and relative deprivation by two-level Poisson regression analysisa).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0111169.t004

Relative deprivation was significantly associated with poor health, regardless of the status of relative poverty and other individual- and municipal-level characteristics. In male respondents with two or more deprivation items, rates of fair/poor self-rated health were 1.34 times (95%CI: 1.16–1.54) higher and rates of depressive symptoms were 1.62 times (95%CI: 1.42–1.85) higher than those observed in non-deprived individuals. Similarly, relative deprivation was associated with fair/poor self-rated health [PR = 1.27 (95%CI: 1.10–1.46)] and depressive symptoms [PR = 1.43 (95%CI: 1.25–1.64)] in women. Relative poverty was also related to self-rated health [men: PR = 1.25 (95%CI: 1.11–1.40); women: PR = 1.13 (95%CI: 1.01–1.26)] and depressive symptoms [men: PR = 1.34 (95%CI: 1.20–1.50); women: PR = 1.24 (95%CI: 1.11–1.37)]. The PRs for relative deprivation were comparatively higher than those for relative poverty. The interaction effect between relative poverty and relative deprivation for subjective health was not statistically significant.

Figure 1 shows different associations between subjective health and relative deprivation according to social support. The proportion of respondents with poor health was remarkably lower in those with social support relative to those without. However, relative to non-deprived respondents with any level of social support, the proportions of respondents with poor/fair subjective health were remarkably higher in the relative deprivation groups (1 and ≤2, respectively).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Proportion (95% Confidence Interval) of poor health in relation to relative deprivation and social support.

Both figures show a low proportion of poor health in the presence of social support. Meanwhile, the proportion of those with poor health increases as the relative deprivation index score increases even with social support, indicating that social support does not fully cancel out the negative impacts of relative deprivation on health.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0111169.g001

Discussion

It was previously thought that all Japanese people were middle class. However, recent surveys have shown that intergenerational inequality exists in Japan [50], and the number of older people on public assistance is increasing [51]. The World Health Organization stated that poverty and relative deprivation have a major impact on health and premature death [52]. On the other hand, most poverty research has been based solely on the concept of relative poverty (monetary poverty) since data on material and environmental poverty was severely limited in Japan.

The present study addressed the concepts of both relative poverty and relative deprivation through a large survey of older Japanese and analyzed the association between health and relative poverty and deprivation. Our results showed that relative deprivation and relative poverty were related to poor health, even after other variables were controlled for. Our results were consistent with preceding findings; Abe [14] found that relative deprivation is closely associated with poor self-rated health and the presence of depressive symptoms using national Japanese representative cross-sectional data of participants aged ≥20 years. In particular, our results suggest that the concept of relative deprivation could address a different aspect of poverty that is related to health but is not addressed by the concept of relative poverty. People who have overlapping multidimensional disadvantages are more likely to be socially excluded [9] and to experience premature death [53]. Our results also showed that the negative effects of relative poverty and deprivation on health are additive; people with both relative deprivation and poverty were more disadvantaged with respect to health, but the relationship was not multiplicative.

Our study adds new evidence regarding which elements of poverty have strong impacts on the health of older adults. An important finding is that relative deprivation has a stronger association with subjective health than relative poverty for both sexes. Some studies have revealed that people living in relatively deprived conditions experienced long-term, severe poverty throughout their life course [10], [11]. For example, Whelan et al. [10] reported that approximately 40% of persistent income poverty overlapped with lifestyle deprivation in a broadly uniform manner. Consequently, the concept of relative deprivation could capture severe and absolute poverty better than relative poverty, which is based on the distribution of income in society. This could mean that relatively deprived older people might tend to be more disadvantaged, even in good health.

Finally, similar to a preceding study using cross-sectional data from 5,624 women aged 2059 [28], our data confirmed that relative deprivation was associated with the absence of social support [15]. Moreover, our results showed that having social support of any form could mitigate some of the negative impacts of relative deprivation on health. However, it is important to note that even with social support, relatively deprived people have more disadvantages with respect to health than non-deprived people. Therefore, the effects of material and environmental deprivation on poor health cannot be explained only by the absence of social support. As shown in preceding study [30][38], relative deprivation might increase social stresses and anxieties while lowering self-efficacy by depriving a living standard most people in the society enjoy. Furthermore, unlike monetary poverty, poor standard of living such as relative deprivation might closely be related to unhealthy lifestyles including poor eating habit and nutrition and lack of access to healthcare and welfare services.

Compared to relative poverty, which is based on a simple indicator and is often used in international comparative studies, relative deprivation is composed of complex indicators and has limitations for use in comparative studies. In fact, most preceding studies have applied a consensual approach based on public opinion in creating and selecting daily necessities and basic needs indices [4], [6], [7], [10], [12][14]. As a result, each relative deprivation indicator was different among preceding studies, although they often reflected the characteristics of that nation and culture. However, relative deprivation could more accurately represent the phenomenon of poverty due to multidimensional living conditions than it does relative poverty. Although measurements of relative deprivation have been made in order to establish the poverty line, our results suggest that relative deprivation is also important for public health policy as it represents a dimension of the social determinants of health.

Study limitations

The present study has some limitations. First, our relative deprivation indicators did not cover the full range of daily resources among older people in Japan. Although we included indicators used in preceding studies, the indicators should be more sophisticated. Second, while the overall response rate for our data was relatively high, the response rates among the lower income categories were comparatively lower [54]. Therefore, our findings may be underestimated because people living in serious poverty and deprivation may have been less likely to participate in our survey. Third, there is a possibility of selection bias at the municipal level since our data are not representative of the whole country. On the other hand, our subjects were randomly selected in each municipality, and it is important to note that we did perform a large-scale survey concerning non-monetary poverty among older people in more than one municipality. Further research should include longitudinal surveys to reveal whether a causal relationship between relative deprivation and health exits.

Conclusion

Relative deprivation (non-monetary poverty) is an important element in poverty. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the association between health conditions and relative deprivation and poverty among older Asian adults. The results showed that relative deprivation has stronger associations with self-rated health and depressive symptoms than with relative poverty. There was an independent and additive association between relative deprivation and poverty with respect to subjective health, and the presence of social support may not fully mitigate the negative association between relative deprivation and health. Our results suggest that relative deprivation is one social determinant of health that the concept of relative poverty cannot address.

Acknowledgments

The JAGES project (2014) comprises a coordinating group and collaborating investigators in each of the following field centers: Prof. K. Kondo (lead investigatorBZH12275@nifty.ne.jp), Dr H. Hikichi, Mr Y. Miyaguni, Dr Y. Sasaki, Dr Y Nagamine, Dr M. Hanazato, Chiba University; Dr N. Kondo, Dr T. Ashida, Dr D. Takagi, Dr Y. Tani, Tokyo University; Prof. T. Ojima, Dr E. Okada, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine; Prof. K. Osaka, Dr J. Aida, Dr T. Tuboya, Tohoku University; Dr M. Saito, Nihon Fukushi University; Dr H. Hirai, Iwate University; Dr Y. Shobugawa, Niigata university; Dr K. Suzuki, Aichi Gakuin University; Dr Y. Ichida, Doctoral Institute for Evidence Based Policy; Dr T. Yamamoto, Kanagawa Dental University; Prof. C. Murata, Dr T. Saito, Dr S. Jeong, National Institute for Longevity Sciences; Dr M. Nakade, Tokai Gakuen University; Prof. T Takeda, Seijo University; Prof. N Cable, University College London; Prof. H. Todoroki, Dr K Shirai, University of the Ryukyuus; Mr T Hayashi, Tokai College of Medical Science. Dr A. Tamakoshi, Hokkaido University; Dr J Misawa, Rikkyo University; Dr Y Fujino, University of Occupational and Environmental Health.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MS NK KK. Performed the experiments: KK the JAGES group. Analyzed the data: MS. Contributed to the writing of the manuscript: MS KK NK AA TO KS.

References

  1. 1. Lynch J, Kaplan G (2000) Socioeconomic position. In: Berkman LF, Kawachi I, editors. Social epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press, 13–35.
  2. 2. Claussen B, Davey Smith G, Thelle D (2003) Impact of childhood and adulthood socio-economic position on cause specific mortality: the Oslo Mortality Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 2003 57: 40–45.
  3. 3. Shaw M, Dorling D, Davey Smith G (2006) Poverty, social exclusion, and minorities. In: Marmot M, Wilkinson R, editors. Social determinants of health; second edition. New York: Oxford university press, 196–223.
  4. 4. Abe A (2006) Empirical analysis of relative deprivation in Japan using Japanese micro-data. The Journal of Social Policy and Labor Studies (Shakai-seisaku Gakkai Shi). 16: 251–275 (in Japanese with English abstract)..
  5. 5. Townsend P (1979) Poverty in the United Kingdom; a survey of household resources and standards of living. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books.
  6. 6. Mack J, Lansley S (1985) Poor Britain. London, George Allen and Unwin.
  7. 7. Gordon D, Adelman L, Ashworth K, Bradshaw J, Levitas R, et al.. (2000) Poverty and social exclusion in Britain. Rowntree Foundation.
  8. 8. Hiraoka K (2002) Social inequalities among elderly people in Japan. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press. (in Japanese).
  9. 9. Bradshaw J, Finch N (2003) Overlaps in dimensions of poverty. Journal of Social Policy 32(4): 513–525.
  10. 10. Whelan CT, Layte R, Maitre B (2003) Persistent income poverty and deprivation in the European Union; an analysis of first three waves of the European Community Household Panel. International Social Policy 32(1): 1–18.
  11. 11. Iwata M, Hamamoto C (2004) Experience of poverty in deflationary depression. In: Higuchi Y, Ota K, the institute for research on household economics, editors. Women in Heisei depression (Jyosei tachi no Heisei fukyo). Nikkei INC, 203–233. (in Japanese).
  12. 12. Saunders P (2008) Measuring well-being using non-monetary indicators; deprivation and social exclusion. Family Matters 78: 8–17.
  13. 13. Saunders P, Abe A (2010) Poverty and deprivation in young and old: a comparative study of Australia and Japan. Poverty & Public Policy 2(1): 67–97.
  14. 14. Abe A (2014) Constructing a deprivation scale for Japan; an index to supplement the relative poverty rate. the Quarterly of Social Security Research. 49(4): 360–371 (in Japanese)..
  15. 15. Saito M, Kondo K, Kondo N, Ojima T, Suzuki K, et al.. (2014) Prevalence and characteristics of relative deprivation among older people; from cross-sectional data in JAGES project. the Quarterly of Social Security Research. 50(3): in press. (in Japanese).
  16. 16. European Commission (2011) The social dimension of the Europe 2020 strategy; A report of the social protection committee. Directorate-General for Employment, Social Affairs and Inclusion.
  17. 17. Eames M, Ben-Shlomo Y, Marmot MG (1993) Social deprivation and premature mortality: regional comparison across England. British Medical Journal, 307(6912), 1097–1102.
  18. 18. Mac Loon P, Boddy FA (1994) Deprivation and mortality in Scotland, 1981 and 1991. British Medical Journal 309(6967): 1465–1470.
  19. 19. Langford IH, Bentham G (1996) Regional variations in mortality rates in England and Wales: an analysis using multilevel modeling. Social Science & Medicine 42(6): 897–908.
  20. 20. O'Reilly D (2002) Standard indicators of deprivation; Do they disadvantage older people? Age and Ageing 31(3): 197–202.
  21. 21. Nakaya T (2011) Evaluation socio-economic inequalities in cancer mortality by using areal statistics in Japan: a note on the relation between municipal cancer mortality and areal deprivation index. Proceedings of Institute of Statistical Mathematics. 59(2): 239–265 (in Japanese with English abstract)..
  22. 22. Mac Loon P (1996) Suicide and deprivation in Scotland. British Medical Journal. 312(7030): 543–544.
  23. 23. Lawlor DA, Davey Smith G, Patel R, Ebrahim S (2005) Life-course socioeconomic position, area deprivation, and coronary heart disease; findings from the British Women's Heart and Health Study. American Journal of Public Health. 95(1): 91–97.
  24. 24. Ellwood RP, Davies GM, Worthington HV, Blinkhorn AS, Taylor GO, et al. (2004) Relationship between area deprivation and the anticaries benefit of an oral health programme providing free fluoride toothpaste to young children. Community Dentistry and Oral Epidemiology 32(3): 159–165.
  25. 25. Bower E, Gulliford M, Steele J, Newton T (2007) Area deprivation and oral health in Scottish adults: a multilevel study. Community Dentistry and Oral Epidemiology 35(2): 118–129.
  26. 26. Walters K, Breeze E, Wilkinson P, Price GM, Bulpitt CJ, et al. (2004) Local area deprivation and urban-rural differences in anxiety and depression among people older than 75 years in Britain. American Journal of Public Health. 94(10): 1768–1774.
  27. 27. Carp FM, Carp A (1981) Age, deprivation, and personal competence; effects on satisfaction. Research on Aging. 3(3): 279–298.
  28. 28. Sacker A, Bartley M, Firth D, Fitzpatrick R (2001) Dimensions of social inequality in the health of women in England: occupational, material and behavioral pathways. Social Science and Medicine 52(5): 763–781.
  29. 29. Golant S, La Greca A (1995) The relative deprivation of U.S. elderly households as judged by their housing problems. Journal of Gerontology 50B(1): S13–S23.
  30. 30. Stouffer SA, Suchman EA, Devinney LC, Star SA, Williams RM (1949) The American Soldier. Volume I.: Adjustment During Army Life. Princeton University Press.
  31. 31. Runciman WG (1966) Relative deprivation and social justice: a study of attitudes to social inequality in twentieth century England. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
  32. 32. Scase R (1974) Relative deprivation: a comparison of English and Swedish manual workers. Wedderburn.D (eds) Poverty, inequality, and class structure. London: Syndics of cambridge university press. 197–216.
  33. 33. Crosby FJ (1982) Relative deprivation and working women. New York: Oxford University Press.
  34. 34. Kosaka K (1986) A model of relative deprivation. Journal of Mathematical Sociology 12(1): 35–48.
  35. 35. Turley RNL (2002) Is relative deprivation beneficial? the effects of richer and poorer neighbors on children's outcomes. Journal of Community Psychology 30(6): 671–686.
  36. 36. Walker I, Smith HJ (2002) Relative deprivation: specification, development, and integration. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  37. 37. Tougas F, Lagace M, Sablonniere R, Kocum L (2004) A new approach to the link between identity and relative deprivation in the perspective of ageism and retirement. International Journal of Aging and Human Development 59(1): 1–23.
  38. 38. Kondo N, Kawachi I, Hirai H, Kondo K, Subramanian SV, et al. (2009b) Relative deprivation and incident functional disability among older Japanese women and men: prospective cohort study. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 63(6): 461–467.
  39. 39. Idler EL, Benyamini Y (1997) Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health Social Behaviour 38(1): 21–37.
  40. 40. Royall DR, Schillerstrom JE, Piper PK, Chiodo LK (2007) Depression and mortality in elders referred for geriatric psychiatry consultation. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 8(5): 318–321.
  41. 41. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, et al. (1983) Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research 17: 37–49.
  42. 42. Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA (1986) Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clinical Gerontologist 5: 165–173.
  43. 43. Murata C, Saito Y, Kondo K, Hirai H (2011) Social support and depression among community living older people. Japanese Journal of Gerontology 33(1): 15–21 (in Japanese with English abstract)..
  44. 44. Watanabe M, Imagawa T (2013) Factor structure of short form of the geriatric depression scale (GDS): reliability, validity and cutoff points. the Japanese Journal of Personality 22(2): 193–197 (in Japanese with English abstract)..
  45. 45. Abe A (2007) Measuring Social Exclusion in Japan. the Quarterly of Social Security Research. 43(1): 27–40 (in Japanese)..
  46. 46. Ministry of internal affairs and communications (2009) National Survey of Family Income and Expenditure. (in Japanese). (http://www.stat.go.jp/data/zensho/2009/index.htm. Accessed 2014 Oct 3.).
  47. 47. Förster MF (1994) Measurement of low incomes and poverty. OECD labour market and social policy occasional papers, no. 14. doi:10.1787/112854878327.
  48. 48. Ichida Y, Kondo K, Hirai H, Hanibuchi T, Yoshikawa G, et al. (2009) Social capital, income inequality and self-rated health in Chita peninsula, Japan: a multilevel analysis of older people in 25 communities. Social Science & Medicine 69(4): 489–499.
  49. 49. Aida J, Hanibuchi T, Nakade M, Hirai H, Osaka K, et al. (2009) The different effects of vertical social capital and horizontal social capital on dental status; A multilevel analysis. Social Science & Medicine 69(4): 512–518.
  50. 50. Krueger AB, Abraham KG, Shapiro C (2012) Economic report of the president, transmitted to the congress February 2012; together with the annual report of the council of economic advisors. United States government printing office.
  51. 51. National Institute of Population and Social Security Research (2013) Japanese public statistics data base concerning public assistance (in Japanese). (http://www.ipss.go.jp/s-info/j/seiho/seiho.asp. Accessed 2014 Oct 3.).
  52. 52. Wilkinson R, Marmot M (2003) Social determinants of health: The solid facts (2nd ed.). WHO Regional Office for Europe.
  53. 53. Saito M, Kondo N, Kondo K, Ojima T, Hirai H (2012) Gender differences on the impacts of social exclusion on mortality among older Japanese: AGES cohort study. Social Science and Medicine 75(5): 940–945.
  54. 54. Kondo K (2010) Health inequalities in Japan: An empirical study of the older people. Melbourne: Trans Pacific Press.