Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The Impact of Social Deprivation on Paranoia, Hallucinations, Mania and Depression: The Role of Discrimination Social Support, Stress and Trust

  • Sophie Wickham ,

    slw@liverpool.ac.uk

    Affiliation Institute of Psychology, Health and Society, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, Merseyside, United Kingdom

  • Peter Taylor,

    Affiliation Institute of Psychology, Health and Society, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, Merseyside, United Kingdom

  • Mark Shevlin,

    Affiliation School of Psychology, University of Ulster, Magee Campus, Londonderry, Northern Ireland, United Kingdom

  • Richard P. Bentall

    Affiliation Institute of Psychology, Health and Society, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, Merseyside, United Kingdom

Abstract

The negative implications of living in a socially unequal society are now well documented. However, there is poor understanding of the pathways from specific environmental risk to symptoms. Here we examine the associations between social deprivation, depression, and psychotic symptoms using the 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey, a cross-sectional dataset including 7,353 individuals. In addition we looked at the mediating role of stress, discrimination, trust and lack of social support. We found that the participants' neighbourhood index of multiple deprivation (IMD) significantly predicted psychosis and depression. On inspection of specific psychotic symptoms, IMD predicted paranoia, but not hallucinations or hypomania. Stress and trust partially mediated the relationship between IMD and paranoid ideation. Stress, trust and a lack of social support fully mediated the relationship between IMD and depression. Future research should focus on the role deprivation and social inequalities plays in specific manifestations of psychopathology and investigate mechanisms to explain those associations that occur. Targeting the mediating mechanisms through appropriate psychological intervention may go some way to dampen the negative consequences of living in an unjust society; ameliorating economic injustice may improve population mental health.

Introduction

Social deprivation, inequality and psychosis

Since the 1970s, in much of the developed world, the wealthiest have become increasingly wealthy but the wealth of the collective poor has remained unchanged [1]. The negative implications for health and wellbeing of this increasing inequality are now well documented [2][5]. Research has identified a wide range of poor physical and psychological outcomes associated with inequality and social deprivation, including obesity [6], cardiovascular disease [7], anxiety [8], depression [9], suicide [10] and also severe mental health diagnoses, including psychosis [11]. The economic consequences of these inequalities are vast. Marmot [2] has estimated that, in the United Kingdom (UK) alone, the healthcare costs associated with social and economic deprivation exceed £5.5 billion per year. However, the mechanisms responsible for these associations are poorly understood. It seems likely that social stressors impacting on emotional regulation play an important role [12][15]. Here we examine associations between indices of deprivation, and serious mental illness, investigating associations with specific symptoms in the non-affective psychosis spectrum, and also with depression and mania. In addition we consider the mediating role of stress, discrimination, trust and lack of social support.

Links between economic deprivation and poor psychological functioning are now well established, with associations found for multiple psychopathologies [8], [10], [16], [17]. Economically deprived areas have excessive numbers of diagnosed individuals with depression [18] and also psychosis [19][21]. In the case of depression it is estimated that living in deprivation, poverty and feeling financial strain are all significant risk factors [9]. In the case of psychosis, a significant proportion of the variance (8%) in incidence rates is attributed to characteristics associated with neighbourhood level and environmental characteristics [5]. A recent analysis has suggested that a one standard deviation increase in deprivation in England may confer an increased relative risk of psychosis of 1.28 [17].

These associations have often been explained in terms of reverse causality on the grounds that individuals with a deteriorating mental state seek out environments that are typically urban and deprived due to downward social drift (e.g. [22]). However this explanation seems unlikely as research indicates an association before the onset of the illness and especially with deprivation during childhood [23] with one research finding associations at birth [24]. There is the possibility that drift may originate in earlier generations, accounting for these findings. However, a dose-response relationship has been detected between deprivation and psychosis, with those who spend most time in urban and deprived environments being at highest risk [25], [26]. Moreover, research that has controlled for possible genetic confounding suggests that growing up in deprivation puts individuals at a higher risk of a schizophrenia diagnosis, regardless of genetic risk, and perhaps more importantly for intervention strategies, that growing up with high socioeconomic status (SES) is a protective factor for those who are at genetic risk [27].

Recent studies have suggested that inequality (living in circumstances in which there are large variations in economic wellbeing) may confer risk that is additional to the risk of deprivation alone [28], [29]. Kirkbride et al. [17] investigated the association of social inequality with incidence rates of affective and non-affective psychosis. They used the index of multiple deprivation (IMD; a small area statistic made available by the UK Office of National Statistics) and local Gini coefficients calculated by comparing variation in deprivation within each neighbourhood at a lower, nested level of geography, and found that both were associated with non-affective psychosis but not with affective psychosis. More recently, Tsai et al. [30], in a 10-year longitudinal study found that a combination of individual and neighbourhood economic status predicted extremely poor outcomes for psychosis, so that schizophrenia patients with low SES living in deprived neighbourhoods were at 18–22% higher risk of death than those with high SES living in advantaged neighbourhoods. Schizophrenia patients with low SES living in advantaged neighbourhoods also had better outcomes than their counterparts in disadvantaged neighbourhoods.

One limitation of the above research is that it has generally employed broad diagnostic categories as outcome variables. In the case of psychosis, especially, such categories are problematic because they encompass heterogeneous phenomena, leading some to argue that psychopathology is better understood in terms of a small number of dimensions [31] or at the symptom level [32]. Indeed, existing evidence suggests that some environmental factors may confer risk of specific symptoms, for example that childhood sexual abuse confers a specific risk of auditory verbal hallucinations (AVHs) whereas attachment-disrupting events confer a specific risk of paranoia [33], [34]. However, other risk factors, for example, urbanicity [35], appear to confer risk across the broad positive symptom dimension. For example, Oher et al. [35] considered the association between deprivation and psychotic symptoms in people with first episode psychosis. They found that there were higher levels of reality distortion and depressive symptoms in the most densely populated areas, however no clear association was observed for paranoia. In addition, it was associated with hallucinations. In some cases, for example perceived discrimination, the data is inconsistent, with some studies suggesting an association with all positive symptoms [36] and some suggesting a specific association with non-clinical but not clinical paranoia [37], [38]. These kinds of specificities are important because they provide clues about the mechanisms that may give rise to symptoms. In this paper, we consider whether deprivation might be linked to specific psychotic symptoms.

It seems reasonable to hypothesize that paranoid beliefs may be particularly linked to deprivation because psychological research implicates negative social comparison and experiences of victimisation (arguably more likely in deprived and unequal neighbourhoods) in these beliefs [39], [40]. Consistent with this hypothesis, Mirowsky and Ross [41], in a population survey of residents of El Paso (USA) and Juarez (Mexico) found that paranoid beliefs were associated with urban areas in which individuals felt powerless and victimized, and that external locus of control and lack of trust mediated this association. More recently, in a study of a large student sample, Wickham et al. [42] found that recalled relative deprivation in childhood was associated with paranoid beliefs and hallucination-proneness in early adulthood. However, on investigating possible mechanisms to explain this association (beliefs about injustice, trust and social rank) this study found that these variables were significant mediators for paranoid beliefs but not for hallucination-proneness. Finally, experimental studies have demonstrated that walking through a deprived urban neighbourhood can provoke paranoid thoughts in both psychiatric patients [43] and ordinary people [44].

Underlying mechanisms between inequality and psychosis

Several underlying mechanism have been hypothesised to explain the pathways from environment risks to psychosis. Impaired nutrition [45] and infections [46] have been suggested to play a role, but with varying and unreliable findings [47][49]. Given that that stress is implicated in a range of mental illnesses and, in particular, psychosis [15], [50], [51] an alternative possibility is that experiences of deprivation, especially in highly unequal societies such as the UK or the US, are stress-inducing. Concepts such as status anxiety (SA; [52], [53]), social defeat (SD; [54]) and social exclusion (SE; [55]) have all been proposed as potential mechanisms. Although these are conceptually different, a common theme is the perception of being disadvantaged compared to others and hence excluded. The SD hypothesis, in particular, has gone to some lengths to explain the role of not only deprivation but of other risk factors for psychosis such as childhood trauma, urban upbringing, low IQ, migration and illicit drug use [54], [56]. A recent review suggests that the impact of population density, social fragmentation and deprivation on risk of psychosis may not be directly causal, but explained or modified by individual level social stressors (i.e. cannabis use, social adversity, exclusion and discrimination). However the authors of the review note that good research is lacking in this area [26].

In this study, we aimed to assess the association between deprivation as measured by the index of multiple deprivation (IMD) and specific mental health outcomes, using data from the 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (APMS2007; [57]). For reasons reviewed above, we hypothesised deprivation to be associated with paranoia. In the light of previous research (e.g. [58]), we also predicted deprivation to be associated with depression. Predictions about hallucinations had to be more tentative. On the one hand, in our previous research [42] with student participants we did find a modest association between IMD of neighbourhood during childhood and hallucination-proneness. However, research on the cognitive processes involved in AVHs has implicated impaired source monitoring as a crucial mechanism [59] and there was no reason to predict that this mechanism would be affected by current exposure to deprived urban environments. Hence we anticipated that any association between deprivation and AVHs would be small or non-existent. Given the lack of association between manic symptoms and urbanicity [60], [61], we also did not predict an association between these symptoms and deprivation.

We also tested four potential mediators of the predicted relationships which were available in APMS2007 and which we selected on the basis of previous research. Because discrimination has been implicated in psychosis in some studies [36] and paranoia in others [37], [38] this was included. Trust was included in the light of evidence that it might be specifically implicated in paranoia [41], [43]. Finally, lack of social support and isolation has been implicated in paranoia [62], [63].

Although our main research focus is the positive symptoms of psychosis, because depression has also been linked to poverty [9], [64] and social deprivation [65], [66] we included it in our models. Finally, because mania was also assessed in APMS2007, and because the research linking urbanicity to affective psychosis and bipolar disorder has been inconsistent [17], [67], we also considered it in our analyses.

Methods

Ethics statement

The current study used secondary data therefore direct ethical approval was not required. However the original data collection (APMS2007) received ethical approval from the Royal Free Hospital and Medical School Research Ethics Committee (reference number 06/Q0501/71). Written consent was obtained from all participants.

Sample

The APMS2007 was carried out between October 2006 and December 2007 by the National Centre for Social Research and the University of Leicester. The survey was commissioned by the NHS Information Centre for Health and Social Care and employed a multistage stratified probability sampling design. Individuals aged 16 years and above living in private households were identified for interview in England using postcodes. From 13,171 eligible households, 7,353 individuals completed the first phase (although a second phase involved clinical interviews with a subsample, these data are not used in this study, with the exception that they contributed to the definition of probable psychosis; see below). Researchers administered computer-assisted interviews and self-completion questionnaires using laptops to obtain data on topics including physical health, mental health, service use, religion, social capital, discrimination and sexual abuse. For more information, see McManus et al [57].

Dependent variables

Psychosis.

The APMS2007 screened for psychosis experiences during phase one using the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ; [68]), and then interviewed a subsample of participants with the Schedule for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN; [69]) in phase two. Probable psychosis and definite psychosis were binary variables derived from phase one and two data.

Probable psychosis was derived by all those who screened positive for psychosis at phase one from the PSQ and during the SCAN assessment at phase two. These were scored as probable psychosis (given a score of 1). In addition, those who did not have a SCAN assessment but met two or more positive psychosis screening criteria on the basis of their questionnaire responses were also assigned a probable psychosis outcome. A score of 0 was given to individuals who only met one positive screening criteria at phase one and who did not have a SCAN assessment, and also to all those who screened as having no psychosis experiences at phase one. The scoring of definite psychosis employed the same strategy as probable psychosis but a different restrained weighting strategy was applied to take account of non-response in phase two (see [57]). This is considered a more conservative estimate of psychotic experiences.

Paranoia, AVHs and Mania.

The PSQ [68] has 5 sections to identify psychotic-like experiences that may have occurred within the past year: mania/hypomania, thought control, paranoia, strange experiences, and AVHs. Each section has an initial question followed by 1 or 2 follow-up questions to determine severity. For present purposes, paranoia, AVHs and mania/hypomania sections were of interest, and were scored as binary variables, scoring the highest level of severity as 1, and all others as 0. Here, we are using a narrow definition of paranoia, encompassing thoughts of deliberate acts of harm and plotting against an individual as has been used in a previous publication using this dataset [33].

The highest severity of paranoia was assessed using the question, “Have there been times that you felt that a group of people was plotting to cause you serious harm or injury?” The highest severity of auditory-verbal hallucination (AVH) was identified by respondents answering yes to the question: “Did you at any time hear voices saying quite a few words or sentences when there was no one around that might account for it?” The highest severity of mania/hypomania was assessed using the question, “Did people around you think it [being happy, for no reason, for days without a break] was strange or complain about it?”

Depression.

A binary score for depression (1 = depression, 0 = absent) was derived from the phase one Clinical Interview Scale – Revised (CIS-R, see [70]). This score was derived from the APMS2007 coding script which included many variables from the CIS-R, including depressive ideas and depressive moods, but also fatigue and appetite. The final depression score encompasses mild, moderate and severe depression.

Mediating Variables

Stress.

One item from the social functioning section of the APMS2007 asked individuals whether they saw their tasks at home and at work as very stressful. Individuals answered on a 4-point Likert scale from 0 “not at all” to 3 “most of the time”.

Social Support.

Seven items were used to identify the level of social support each respondent felt that they had from family and friends. Participants were asked to respond “not true” “partly true” or “certainly true” to a series of statements, including whether family and friends did things to make them happy, made them feel loved, could be relied on no matter what, would see that they were taken care of no matter what, accepted them just the way they are, made them feel an important part of their lives, and gave them support and encouragement. Each respondent could have a score between 0 and 14, creating a 15-point scale. In the current analysis, internal consistence is good (Cronbach's α = .88).

Discrimination.

An eight point scale was derived based on cumulative experiences of discrimination in the last 12 months. This was assessed using eight items from the discrimination section of the APMS2007. Respondents were asked to answer yes (1) or no (0) to identify if they had been unfairly treated in the last 12 months due to skin colour/ethnicity, sex, religious beliefs, age, mental health, other health problems or disability or sexual orientation.

Independent Variable

The Index of Multiple Deprivation.

The index of multiple deprivation (IMD) 2004 data were collected for the APMS 2007. This is a derived measure of social and economic deprivation based on seven domains or neighbourhood variables: income; employment; health and disability; education, skills and training; barriers to housing and services; living environment; and crime. Data for the IMD 2004 was collected between 1997 and 2003 and the APMS2007 reported a 5 point scale, where 1 (0.59–8.35) represents the least deprived and 5 (34.21–86.36) represents most deprived.

Covariates.

Covariates included in the analyses encompassed age (range = 16 to 95 with a mean of 46.35, SD = 18.60). Sex was added as a categorical variable covariate where 48.6% of respondents were male (1 = male, 0 = female). Ethnicity was also added as a categorical variable where 0 equated to white British and 1 equalled other ethnicity. In this dataset 84.6% of respondents were white British. Drug use with the last year was added as a covariate, this was added as a binary variable (1 = yes and 0 = no). In this sample 92% of the total sample reported using any drug in the year leading up to the interview. Highest education level achieved was also entered into the analysis, this included 6 categorical variables from degree (18.6%), teaching, HND and nursing (7.3%), A levels (12.7%), GCSE or equivalent (24.5%), foreign or other (3.9%) and no qualification (30.8%), with 2.3% missing data. Finally AVHs were entered as a covariate in the mediation analysis, this was to address the issue of co-occurrence of symptoms, particularly between paranoia and AVHs) as has been previously identified and used in these large epidemiological datasets (see [33], [34]).

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics were produced using SPSS 21 and regression and multivariate mediation models were specified and estimated using Mplus6.1. Logistic regressions were carried out to assess the association between the independent variable (IV) IMD and the dependent variables (DV) mental health outcomes (probable psychosis, definite psychosis, depression, paranoia, AVHs and mania/hypomania). This was completed three times. In the first regression IMD was regressed to definitive psychosis, as this required different weighting than for other DVs. The second regression allowed IMD to predict probable psychosis, and the final regression allowed IMD to predict to all other DVs (depression, paranoia, AVHs and mania/hypomania). All regressions used the appropriate weighting for survey design and non-response in order to be representative of English population (see [57]).

Mediation was investigated using multivariate binary logistic regression models estimated in Mplus 6.1 [71] using robust maximum likelihood estimation. In in an initial model, direct paths between the IMD and the symptoms associated with IMD were computed. Model 2 added the direct paths from covariates to symptoms including age, sex, ethnicity and AVHs. AVHs were added as a covariate due to its shared variance with both paranoia and depression [33], [34]. All parameters were estimated simultaneously so that the differences in the estimates for the IMD variable between model 1 and model 2 showed the effects of controlling for comorbidity between symptoms and other potential confounders. In model 3, discrimination, stress, lack of support and trust variables were introduced in the model as mediators between IMD and symptom outcome variables, and the mediating variables were allowed to covary. The effects from IMD to the mediating variables were linear regression estimates and the effects from the mediators to the symptoms were logistic estimates reported as odds ratios (see Figure 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Illustration of the final model (model 3) used in the analysis.

Please note that the mediators representing discrimination, support, stress and trust were allowed to covary. Covariates included age, sex, ethnicity and hallucinatory experiences. The mediating variables (discrimination, support, stress and trust) were regressed onto the IV - the index of multiple deprivation (IMD) variable (path a). The DVs (paranoia and depression) were regressed onto the mediating variables (path b) and the DVs were also regressed onto the IV IMD (path c′) simultaneously. The effects from IMD to the mediating variables were linear regression estimates and the effects from the mediators to the symptoms were logistic estimates reported as odds ratios.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105140.g001

The adequacy of each model was assessed by examining three information theory based fit statistics: the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; [72]), the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; [73]), and the sample size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion (ssaBIC; [74]) with lower values indicating better model fit. These fit statistics balance model fit with parsimony in order to determine the optimum model. In addition, chi-square difference tests were used to determine the best fitting model.

Results

Simple bivariate associations between IMD and mental health outcomes are summarized in Table 1. The general trend suggests that as deprivation increases the presence of mental health difficulties also increases. This is reflected in the logistic regression analyses (Table 2), when controlling for age, sex, ethnicity, drug use in the last year and education level achieved, IMD predicts definite and probable psychosis (ß = 34, p<.01; ß = .26, p<.01 respectively), as well as depression (ß = .16, p<.001). However when we investigated specific symptoms experienced within psychosis (paranoia, AVHs and mania/hypomania) direct associations were found for paranoia (ß = .15, p<.01) and not for either AVHs or mania/hypomania.

thumbnail
Table 1. Simple bivariate associations, showing percentages (%) and actual numbers (n) between the index of multiple deprivation (IMD) and mental health outcomes (psychosis, depression, paranoia, auditory-verbal hallucinations (AVHs) and mania).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105140.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Summary of logistic regression analysis for variables predicting mental health outcomes, controlling for background variables (age, sex, ethnicity, education and drug use).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105140.t002

The odds ratios (ORs) from the multivariate binary logistic regression analysis are presented in Table 3. Model 1 estimated the direct effect of IMD on paranoia and depression. For this model the likelihood ratio chi-square was statistically significant (X2 = 54.94, df = 12, p<.01). The addition of the covariates (model 2; change in X2X2) = 186.61, change in df (Δdf) = 12, p<.01) and the mediator variables (model 3; ΔX2 = 2583.82, Δdf = 38, p<.01) made significant improvements to the overall model (Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Logistic regression odds ratios of symptoms by IMD and fit indices for the mediation model.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105140.t003

In model 1, the IMD variable significantly predicted each symptom (paranoia and depression). The effect was highest for paranoia, with an OR of 1.44 (95% CI [1.25, 1.66]). The OR for depression was 1.29 (95% CI [1.16, 1.44]). The introduction of the covariate variables in model 2 resulted in a slight decrease in the ORs, however remained statistically significant (paranoia OR = 1.26, 95% CI [1.07, 1.47]; depression OR 1.23, 95% CI [1.09, 1.39]).

In model 3, the IMD variables significantly predicted the mediating variables stress, (standardised B = 0.03, SE = 0.01, p = .01) lack of trust (B = 0.23, SE = 0.01, p<.001), lack of social support (B = 0.04, SE = 0.01, p<.001), and discrimination (B = 0.03, SE = 0.01, p = .02). Each of the potential mediators significantly predicted the symptom measures with the exception of the path from lack of support to paranoia, which was non-significant. The largest effect for paranoia was discrimination (OR = 1.47, 95% CI [1.07, 2.03]) and the smallest effect was stress (OR = 1.41, 95% CI [1.13, 1.75]). With the DV depression, the largest effect was between stress and depression (OR = 2.30, 95% CI [1.95, 2.70]). The smallest effect was between lack of social support and depression (OR = 1.10, 95% CI [1.04, 1.16]). Model 3 indicates that partial mediation occurred between IMD and paranoia through discrimination, stress and lack of trust. This is due to the fact that c′ (the relationship between IMD and paranoia) remained significant, but reduced between models 2 and 3 (from OR = 1.26, p<.01, to OR = 1.19, p<.05). However, in the case of depression, full mediation occurred as model 3 shows that c′ became non-significant when the mediators were included.

Discussion

Consistent with our first hypothesis neighbourhood deprivation significantly predicted psychosis and depression. When focussing on specific symptoms, again as we hypothesised, IMD was associated with both paranoia and depression, but not with AVHs or mania. In our multivariate mediation model, when controlling for comorbidity between the symptoms and other background variables (age, sex, ethnicity, drug use and education level), IMD was associated with all four mediators; lack of trust, social support and stress and discrimination. Examination of the final model (model 3) indicated that the ORs for the direct effects between deprivation and symptoms reduced once the mediators were entered into the model. In the case of depression the OR became statistically insignificant, indicating full mediation between IMD and depression through discrimination, trust, lack of support and stress [75]. In the case of paranoia, the OR reduced in magnitude but remained statistically significant therefore indicating partial mediation through discrimination, trust, and stress. Overall, this study is generally consistent with previous findings of an association between social and economic deprivation and psychosis [11], [17], [27][29] but our analyses add to previous studies by indicating that this association relates to specific symptoms and can be at least partially explained by several potential underlying mechanisms.

The absence of significant affects for both hallucinations and mania are as worthy of note as the positive findings. As discussed earlier, impairment in the ability to discriminate between self-generated thoughts and external verbal stimuli, a process known as source monitoring, is a crucial mechanism in vulnerability to auditory hallucinations [59], [76]. Although it is well known that childhood trauma is associated with hallucinations in adulthood [33], and although we had previously found an association between childhood IMD and hallucination-proneness in early adulthood, there was no reason to believe that source monitoring would be affected by current exposure to urban environments as measured in this study.

Very little is known about environmental risk factors for mania, but there is evidence that manic episodes can be triggered by goal achievement life events [77] which are, if anything, less likely to be experienced in deprived versus affluent areas. Previous research has failed to find any association between bipolar disorder and urban environments [67] and our findings appear to confirm this observation.

Two findings from covariates included in our analyses are also worth considering. Female sex was associated with an increased risk of depression, as shown in many previous studies [78]. This observation therefore testifies to the validity of our approach. Perhaps more intriguingly, belonging to an ethnic minority group was strongly associated with paranoia but not depression. It has been well-demonstrated that ethnic minority status is a risk factor for psychosis [79] but we are not aware of attempts to tease out which positive symptoms of psychosis are most sensitive to this effect. Because this observation emerged in models which also include trust, stress, social support and discrimination, the implication is that this increased risk from ethnicity cannot be explained by these variables. Further research is required with other datasets to determine whether a specific link between ethnic status and paranoia can be replicated, and what mechanisms might be involved.

A number of limitations to the study need to be considered. Firstly, the PSQ used in the APMS2007 dataset concerns experiences in the last year, rather than across the lifetime. It is possible that people classified as not experiencing paranoia or AVHs in our analyses in fact had these experiences in the more distant past. However, it is worth noting that focusing on recent experiences of psychosis as opposed to lifetime experiences will have most likely led us to underestimate the association between deprivation and symptoms. Second, the measure of social deprivation used in the APMS2007 dataset, IMD scores was divided into quintiles, producing an ordinal scale; due to data protection restrictions raw IMD data was unavailable to us. This loss of information may have attenuated some relationships. Moreover, the measure pertained to current living circumstances, rather than those of, say, childhood or the time of onset of psychotic experiences. Hence we hypothesize that the findings reflect the provocation of psychotic symptoms by the current environment (as in the case of the experimental studies of the effects of the urban environment on paranoia, [43], [44]), rather than developmental pathways to psychotic symptoms. Third, the reliability of our measure of stress may be compromised as it used a single item relating to work stress. Arguably, the findings related to stress need to be assessed using an established measure of stress to incorporate other forms of stress (e.g. emotional and home stressors).

Fourth, and perhaps most importantly, the present study is cross-sectional and correlational, and direction of causality cannot be tested using the statistical models we have employed. Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that symptoms may affect people's judgments about discrimination, trust, stress and social support or, indeed, that, when answering questions about paranoia and depression, feelings of discrimination, mistrust, stress and the experience of poor social support influenced their answers. Indeed, it could be argued that there is conceptual overlap between mistrust and paranoia and that, therefore, a close association between the two was inevitable. (We think this argument is harder to make about the other mediators.)

With respect to our meditational models, there is no certainty that our mediators were temporally consequent to exposure to a deprived urban environment or that they preceded the onset of symptoms. However, it seems unlikely that people suffering specifically from paranoia and depression (as opposed to AVHs and mania/hypomania) would seek out deprived neighbourhoods to live in. Moreover, our findings are not only consistent with our meditational hypotheses, but also with our previous findings on the relationship between perceived social disadvantage and paranoia [42], with current understandings of the psychological mechanisms underlying paranoid beliefs [39], [40], and, as already noted, with experimental studies which show that short-term exposure to deprived environments tends to provoke paranoid thoughts [43], [44]. A further caveat is that epidemiological findings, particularly involving mediation analysis [80], are vulnerable to unmeasured covariates. This is a problem in most areas of research that can only be addressed by considering other potential covariates in future studies.

It might be argued that our results offer some support to the SD hypothesis and related concepts [54], [56], suggesting that living at the bottom end of an unequal society breeds stress and a lack of trust and support a constellation of experiences which might be described as socially defeating. However, one criticism of the SD model is that it is fairly non-specific in terms of what characterises social defeat, which symptoms are most affected and likely mediating mechanisms.

To our knowledge this is the first study to test for specific symptom relationships (paranoia and depression) with deprivation, as opposed to the broadly defined diagnostic categories which have been identified as particularly problematic when researching social determinants of psychopathological phenomena [81]. It will be useful to replicate these findings in other samples to further assess the specific associations identified here. Longitudinal designs would also help clarify the direction of causality. If supported by future research the present findings have important social implications.

Although the effects found in paranoid and depressive symptoms in this study might be described as modest, at the population level the impact of deprivation on mental health is potentially large. If we consider the UK as an example, the inevitable outcome of experiences of deprivation indicates that more people are being pushed towards mental illness [82]. In much of the developed world, income inequalities have increased since the 1970s with current disparities at an all-time high [2]. It may be possible that efforts to reverse this trend, for example through progressive taxation or through enhanced benefits to poor families will, in the long-term, positively impact on public mental health but devising such policies, and gaining popular support for their implementation may prove to be a very considerable challenge for all concerned, particularly as individuals tend to justify the status quo in more unequal societies [83], [84]. It may be more viable to tackle the social stressors that inequality produces through psychological treatments (e.g. cognitive-behaviour therapy (CBT) and interpersonal psychotherapy) which already aim to address self-esteem and are thought to be effective in depression [85], and in the case of CBT at least, effective for psychosis (CBTp [86]). It is possible that these might be enhanced by specifically targeting the mediating mechanisms outlined in the current study, namely support, trust and stress. At a societal level, our findings suggest that public mental health might be promoted by either addressing inequalities, ameliorating the social stressors that arising from inequalities, or both.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: SLW PJT MS RPB. Performed the experiments: SLW PJT MS RPB. Analyzed the data: SLW PJT MS RPB. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SLW PJT MS RPB. Contributed to the writing of the manuscript: SLW PJT MS RPB.

References

  1. 1. Toynbee P, Walker D (2008) Unjust rewards: Ending the greed that is bankrupting Britain. London: Granta Books.
  2. 2. Marmot MG, Allen J, Goldblatt P, Boyce T, McNeish D, et al.. (2010) Fair society, healthy lives: The Marmot review. Strateg Rev Heal Inequalities Engl Post 2010.
  3. 3. Marmot MG, Stansfeld S, Patel C, North F, Head J, et al. (1991) Health inequalities among British civil servants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet 337: 1387–1393 Available: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T1B-49K5F0V-506/2/2dc5b634b421e8988be6ae5c15ce2be8.
  4. 4. Wilkinson RG, Pickett KE (2009) The spirit level: Why more equal societies almost always do better. London: Penguin Group.
  5. 5. Kirkbride J (2010) Impact of contextual environmental mechanisms on the incidence of schizophrenia and other psychoses. In: Gattaz WF, Busatto G, editors. Advances in Schizophrenia Research 2009. Springer New York. pp. 67–96. Available: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0913-8_4.
  6. 6. Lee H (2011) Inequality as an explanation for obesity in the United States. Sociol Compass 5: 215–232 Available: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1751-9020.2010.00355.x.
  7. 7. Kim D, Kawachi I, Vander Hoorn S, Ezzati M (2008) Is inequality at the heart of it? Cross-country associations of income inequality with cardiovascular diseases and risk factors. Soc Sci Med 66: 1719–1732 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18280021. Accessed 2014 March 27.
  8. 8. Fryers T, Melzer D, Jenkins R, Brugha T (2005) The distribution of the common mental disorders: social inequalities in Europe. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health 1: 14 Available: http://www.cpementalhealth.com/content/1/1/14. Accessed 2014 Feb 5.
  9. 9. Lorant V, Croux C, Weich S, Deliège D, Mackenbach J, et al. (2007) Depression and socio-economic risk factors: 7-year longitudinal population study. Br J Psychiatry 190: 293–298 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17401034. Accessed 2014 Feb 25.
  10. 10. Gilman SE, Bruce ML, Have T Ten, Alexopoulos GS, Mulsant BH, et al. (2013) Social inequalities in depression and suicidal ideation among older primary care patients. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 48: 59–69
  11. 11. Kirkbride J, Jones PB (2011) The prevention of schizophrenia–what can we learn from eco-epidemiology? Schizophr Bull 37: 262–271 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3044619&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. Accessed 2012 Nov 6.
  12. 12. Wilkinson RG, Pickett KE (2009) Income inequality and social dysfunction. Annu Rev Sociol 35: 493–511 Available: http://www.annualreviews.org/doi/abs/10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-115926. Accessed 2012 Oct 26.
  13. 13. Zimmerman FJ, Bell JF (2006) Income inequality and physical and mental health: Testing associations consistent with proposed causal pathways. J Epidemiol Community Health 60: 513–521
  14. 14. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME (2004) Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull 130: 355–391 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15122924. Accessed 2012 Oct 26.
  15. 15. Marwaha S, Broome M, Bebbington PE, Kuipers E, Freeman D (2014) Mood instability and psychosis: analyses of British national survey data. Schizophr Bull 40: 269–277.
  16. 16. Lorant V, Deliège D, Eaton W, Robert A, Philippot P, et al. (2003) Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: A meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol 157: 98–112 Available: http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/content/157/2/98.abstract.
  17. 17. Kirkbride J, Jones PB, Ullrich S, Coid J (2014) Social deprivation, inequality, and the neighborhood-level incidence of psychotic syndromes in East london. Schizophr Bull 40: 169–180 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3885290&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. Accessed 2014 Jan 21 January.
  18. 18. Andersen I, Thielen K, Nygaard E, Diderichsen F (2009) Social inequality in the prevalence of depressive disorders. J Epidemiol Community Health 63: 575–581 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19293167. Accessed 2014 Feb 24.
  19. 19. Crump C, Sundquist K, Sundquist J, Winkleby MA (2011) Neighborhood deprivation and psychiatric medication prescription: A Swedish national multilevel study. Ann Epidemiol 21: 231–237 Available: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1047279711000263.
  20. 20. Croudace TJ, Kayne R, Jones PB, Harrison G (2000) Non-linear relationship between an index of social deprivation, psychiatric admission prevalence and the incidence of psychosis. Psychol Med 30: 177–185 Available: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291799001464.
  21. 21. Fone DL, Dunstan F (2006) Mental health, places and people: a multilevel analysis of economic inactivity and social deprivation. Health Place 12: 332–344 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16546698. Accessed 2012 Nov 27.
  22. 22. Goldberg EM, Morrison SL (1963) Schizophrenia and social class. Br J Psychiatry 109: 785–802.
  23. 23. Wicks S, Hjern A, Gunnell D, Lewis G, Dalman C (2005) Social adversity in childhood and the risk of developing psychosis: A national cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 162: 1652–1657 Available: http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/content/abstract/162/9/1652.
  24. 24. Harrison G, Gunnell D, Glazebrook C, Page K, Kwiecinski R (2001) Association between schizophrenia and social inequality at birth: Case–control study. Br J Psychiatry 179: 346–350 Available: http://bjp.rcpsych.org/cgi/content/abstract/179/4/346.
  25. 25. Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB (2001) Evidence of a dose-response relationship between urbanicity during upbringing and schizophrenia risk. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58: 1039–1046 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11695950.
  26. 26. Heinz A, Deserno L, Reininghaus U (2013) Urbanicity, social adversity and psychosis. World Psychiatry 12: 187–197
  27. 27. Wicks S, Hjern A, Dalman C (2010) Social risk or genetic liability for psychosis? A study of children born in sweden and reared by adoptive parents. Am J Psychiatry 167: 1240–1246 Available: http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/content/abstract/167/10/1240.
  28. 28. Burns JK, Esterhuizen T (2008) Poverty, inequality and the treated incidence of first-episode psychosis: An ecological study from South Africa. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 43: 331–335 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18253683. Accessed 2012 Nov 20.
  29. 29. Burns JK, Tomita A, Kapadia AS (2014) Income inequality and schizophrenia: Increased schizophrenia incidence in countries with high levels of income inequality. Int J Soc Psychiatry 60: 185–196 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23594564. Accessed 2014 March 20.
  30. 30. Tsai K-Y, Chung T-C, Lee C-C, Chou Y-M, Su C-Y, et al. (2014) Is low individual socioeconomic status (SES) in high-SES areas the same as low individual SES in low-SES areas: a 10-year follow-up schizophrenia study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 49: 89–96 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23728232. Accessed 2014 Jan 30.
  31. 31. Van Os J, Kapur S (2009) Schizophrenia. Lancet 374: 635–645 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19700006. Accessed 2014 March 19.
  32. 32. Bentall RP (2003) Madness explained: Psychosis and human nature. London: England: Penguin Group.
  33. 33. Bentall RP, Wickham S, Shevlin M, Varese F (2012) Do specific early-life adversities lead to specific symptoms of psychosis? A study from the 2007 adult psychiatric morbidity survey. Schizophr Bull 38: 734–740 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3406525&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. Accessed 2014 Jan 30.
  34. 34. Sitko K, Bentall RP, Shevlin M, O'Sullivan N, Sellwood W (2014) Associations between specific psychotic symptoms and specific childhood adversities are mediated by attachment styles: An analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey. Psychiatry Res 217: 202–209
  35. 35. Oher F, Demjaha A, Jackson D, Morgan C, Dazzan P, et al. (2014) The effect of the environment on symptom dimensions in the first episode of psychosis: A multilevel study. Psychol Med 44: 2419–2430
  36. 36. Cooper C, Morgan C, Byrne M, Dazzan P, Morgan K, et al. (2008) Perceptions of disadvantage, ethnicity and psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 192: 185–190 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18310577. Accessed 2014 March 23.
  37. 37. Rippy A, Newman E (2006) Perceived religious discrimination and its relationship to anxiety and paranoia among muslim americans. J Muslim Ment Health 1: 5–20.
  38. 38. Combs DR (2006) Perceived racism as a predictor of paranoia among african Americans. J Black Psychol 32: 87–104 Available: http://jbp.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.1177/0095798405283175. Accessed 2014 March 19.
  39. 39. Bentall RP, Corcoran R, Howard R, Blackwood N, Kinderman P (2001) Persecutory delusions: A review and theoretical integration. Clin Psychol Rev 21: 1143–1192 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11702511.
  40. 40. Freeman D, Garety P, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Bebbington PE (2002) A cognitive model of persecutory delusions. Br J Clin Psychol 41: 331–347
  41. 41. Mirowsky J, Ross CE (1983) Paranoia and the structure of powerlessness. Am Sociol Rev 48: 228–239 Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2095107.
  42. 42. Wickham S, Lyons M, Dickins T, Shyrane N, Bentall RP (2014) Why does relative deprivation affect mental health? The role of justice, trust and social rank in psychological wellbeing and paranoid ideation. J Public Ment Health 13: 114–126
  43. 43. Ellett L, Freeman D, Garety P (2008) The psychological effect of an urban environment on individuals with persecutory delusions: the Camberwell walk study. Schizophr Res 99: 77–84 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18061407. Accessed 2014 March 27.
  44. 44. Nettle D, Pepper GV, Jobling R, Schroeder KB (2014) Being there: a brief visit to a neighbourhood induces the social attitudes of that neighbourhood. PeerJ 2: e236
  45. 45. Harrison G, Fouskakis D, Rasmussen F, Tynelius P, Sipos A, et al. (2003) Association between psychotic disorder and urban place of birth is not mediated by obstetric complications or childhood socio-economic position: a cohort study. Psychol Med 33: 723–731
  46. 46. Yolken RH, Torrey EF (2008) Are some cases of psychosis caused by microbial agents? A review of the evidence. Mol Psychiatry 13: 470–479
  47. 47. Mednick SA, Machon RA, Huttunen MO, Bonett D (1988) Adult schizophrenia following prenatal exposure to an influenza epidemic. Arch Gen Psychiatry 45: 189–192
  48. 48. Varma VK, Wig NN, Phookun HR, Misra AK, Khare CB, et al. (1997) First-onset schizophrenia in the community: relationship of urbanization with onset, early manifestations and typology. Acta Psychiatr Scand 96: 431–438 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9421339.
  49. 49. Brown AS, Derkits EJ (2010) Prenatal infection and schizophrenia: A review of epidemiologic and translational studies. Am J Psychiatry 167: 261–280 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3652286&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract.
  50. 50. Walker E, Mittal V, Tessner K (2008) Stress and the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis in the developmental course of schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 4: 189–216 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18370616. Accessed 2012 Nov 16.
  51. 51. Walker E, Diforio D (1997) Schizophrenia: a neural diathesis-stress model. Psychol Rev 104: 667–685
  52. 52. Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG, Adler NE (2001) Does subjective social status predict health and change in health status better than objective status? Psychosom Med 67: 855–861 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16314589. Accessed 2012 Nov 26.
  53. 53. Marmot MG (2006) Status syndrome: A challenge to medicine. JAMA J Am Med Assoc 295: 1304–1307 Available: http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.295.11.1304.
  54. 54. Selten J-P, Cantor-Graae E (2005) Social defeat: Risk factor for schizophrenia? Br J Psychiatry 187: 101–102 Available: http://bjp.rcpsych.org/content/187/2/101.abstract.
  55. 55. Barry B (2002) Social exclusion, social isolation and the distribution of income. In: Hills J, Le Grand J, Piachaud D, editors. Understanding Social Exclusion. Oxford University Press. pp. 13–29.
  56. 56. Selten J-P, van der Ven E, Rutten BPF, Cantor-Graae E (2013) The social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia: an update. Schizophr Bull 39: 1180–1186 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24062592. Accessed 2014 Jan 22.
  57. 57. McManus S, Meltzer H, Brugha T, Bebbington PE (2009) Adult Psychiatric Morbidity in England, 2007: Results of a Household Survey. London: The NHS Information Centre.
  58. 58. Galea S, Ahern J, Nandi A, Tracy M, Beard J, et al. (2007) Urban neighborhood poverty and the incidence of depression in a population-based cohort study. Ann Epidemiol 17: 171–179 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2442459&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. Accessed 2014 March 31.
  59. 59. Brookwell ML, Bentall RP, Varese F (2013) Externalizing biases and hallucinations in source monitoring, self-monitoring and signal detection studies: A meta-analytic review. Psychol Med 43: 2465–2475
  60. 60. Sherazi R, McKeon P, McDonough M, Kennedy N (2006) What's new: The clinical epidemiology of bipolar disorder. Harv Rev Psychiatry 14: 274–284.
  61. 61. Kaymaz N, van Os J, de Graaf R, Ten Have M, Nolen W, et al. (2007) The impact of subclinical psychosis on the transition from subclinicial mania to bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 98: 55–64 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16934874. Accessed 2014 March 31.
  62. 62. Freeman D, McManus S, Brugha T, Meltzer H, Jenkins R, et al. (2011) Concomitants of paranoia in the general population. Psychol Med 41: 923–936
  63. 63. Gayer-Anderson C, Morgan C (2013) Social networks, support and early psychosis: a systematic review. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 22: 131–146 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22831843. Accessed 2014 April 1.
  64. 64. Doucet DJ (2003) Poverty, inequality, and discrimination as sources of depression among U.S. women. Psychol Women Q 27: 101–113
  65. 65. Grav S, Hellzèn O, Romild U, Stordal E (2012) Association between social support and depression in the general population: the HUNT study, a cross-sectional survey. J Clin Nurs 21: 111–120 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22017561. Accessed 2014 March 27.
  66. 66. Aylaz R, Aktürk Ü, Erci B, Öztürk H, Aslan H (2012) Relationship between depression and loneliness in elderly and examination of influential factors. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 55: 548–554 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22487148. Accessed 2014 March 20.
  67. 67. Kirkbride J, Errazuriz A, Croudace TJ, Morgan C, Jackson D, et al. (2012) Incidence of schizophrenia and other psychoses in England, 1950–2009: A systematic review and meta-analyses. PLoS One 7: e31660 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3310436&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. Accessed 2014 March 20.
  68. 68. Bebbington PE, Nayani T (1995) The psychosis screening questionnaire. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 5: 11–19.
  69. 69. Wing JK, Babor T, Brugha T, Burke J, Cooper JE, et al. (1990) SCAN: Schedules for clinical assessment in neuropsychiatry. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47: 589–593
  70. 70. Lewis G, Pelosi AJ, Araya R, Dunn G (1992) Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: a standardized assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychol Med 22: 465–486
  71. 71. Muthén LK, Muthén BO (n.d.) Mplus user's guide. Sixth. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén.
  72. 72. Akaike H (1987) Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52: 317–332
  73. 73. Schwarz G (1978) Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann Stat 6: 461–464
  74. 74. Sclove SL (1987) Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika 52: 333–343
  75. 75. Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51: 1173–1182
  76. 76. Bentall RP (1990) The illusion of reality: a review and integration of psychological research on hallucinations. Psychol Bull 107: 82–95
  77. 77. Johnson SL, Sandrow D, Meyer B, Winters R, Miller I, et al. (2000) Increases in Manic Symptoms After Life Events Involving Goal Attainment. 109: 721–727
  78. 78. Nolen-Hoeksema S (2001) Gender differences in depression. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 10: 173–176 Available: http://cdp.sagepub.com/lookup/doi/10.1111/1467-8721.00142. Accessed 2014 July 3.
  79. 79. Veling W (2013) Ethnic minority position and risk for psychotic disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry 26: 166–171 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23286992. Accessed 2014 July 3.
  80. 80. Bullock JG, Green DP, Ha SE (2010) Yes, but what's the mechanism? (don't expect an easy answer). J Pers Soc Psychol 98: 550–558
  81. 81. Harper DJ (2011) Social inequality and the diagnosis of paranoia. Heal Sociol Rev 20: 423–436 Available: http://pubs.e-contentmanagement.com/doi/abs/10.5172/hesr.2011.20.4.423.
  82. 82. Dorling D (2011) So you think you know Britain? The surprising truth about modern Britain. UK: Constable.
  83. 83. Kay AC, Gaucher D, Peach JM, Laurin K, Friesen J, et al. (2009) Inequality, discrimination, and the power of the status quo: Direct evidence for a motivation to see the way things are as the way they should be. J Pers Soc Psychol 97: 421–434 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19685999. Accessed 2012 Oct 27.
  84. 84. Dorling D (2010) Injustice: Why social inequality persists. Bristol: The Policy Press.
  85. 85. Cuijpers P, van Straten A, Andersson G, van Oppen P (2008) Psychotherapy for depression in adults: A meta-analysis of comparative outcome studies. J Consult Clin Psychol 76: 909–922
  86. 86. Wykes T, Steel C, Everitt B, Tarrier N (2008) Cognitive behavior therapy for schizophrenia: effect sizes, clinical models, and methodological rigor. Schizophr Bull 34: 523–537 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2632426&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. Accessed 2014 Feb 4.