Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-xxrs7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T07:48:26.468Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and the shifting sands of psychiatric nosology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Stephen V. Faraone*
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, 750 East Adams St., Syracuse, NY 13210, USA. Email: sfaraone@childpsychresearch.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Larsson et al provide epidemiological evidence for a genetic association between attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and both bipolar disorder and schizophrenia and Hamshere and colleagues confirm the latter association with genome-wide data. Although a genetic link between ADHD and bipolar disorder has been hypothesised for over a decade, the association with schizophrenia fills a notable gap in the literature. This editorial discusses the implications of these findings for clinicians, who must address psychiatric comorbidity in their treatment formulations, and researchers who are learning that the discrete categorical diagnoses of our diagnostic systems may not be up to the task of clarifying the causes and cures of psychopathology.

Type
Editorials
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2013 

When Emil Kraepelin cleaved schizophrenia from bipolar disorder in the 19th century he set the course for a categorical, psychiatric nosology that would reign for more than a century. In the 1980s, the prevailing wisdom of the ICD and the American Psychiatric Association's DSM, began the era of structured criteria with nosologies that operationalised Kraepelinian distinctions among disorders. Based on the DSM/ICD criteria of the 1980s, a hierarchical approach to diagnoses would exclude the diagnosis of some disorders if another disorder, which was higher on the hierarchy, could also be diagnosed. Patients with major depression could not be diagnosed with panic disorder; those with autism could not be diagnosed with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). When faced with complex cases, the DSM/ICD paradigm tells clinicians to differentiate ‘primary’ from ‘secondary’ disorders.

Kraepelin's approach has slowly crumbled under a weighty scientific literature. Today, patients with depression can be diagnosed with panic disorder and DSM-5 finally allows patients with autism to be diagnosed with ADHD. Signs of the DSM/ICD paradigm's pathology are pervasive comorbidity, dimensionality and the existence of clinically important syndromes that do not correspond to diagnoses. Consider comorbidity. Starting with the Epidemiologic Catchment Area study of the 1980s, any epidemiological study assessing comorbidity has found that having one mental disorder puts patients at risk for having other mental disorders. This is true for children and adults, does not depend on methods of diagnosis and occurs in clinics and in the community. Family and twin studies find that comorbidity has biological roots; not only are many disorders comorbid in patients, they are also co-transmitted in families.

New data about comorbidity in ADHD

In this issue of the Journal, Larsson and colleagues continue this line of research. They present compelling evidence that ADHD shares genetic risk factors with both bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Reference Larsson, Rydén, Boman, Långström, Lichtenstein and Landén1 A genetic association between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder has long been suspected and was recently confirmed with genome-wide association data. 2 Hamshere et al now provide genome-wide association data suggesting that schizophrenia and ADHD share many common genetic variants. Reference Hamshere, Stergiakouli, Langley, Martin, Holmans and Kent3 A genetic association between ADHD and bipolar disorder had been hypothesised over a decade ago and a familial link between the disorders was recently confirmed with meta-analysis. Reference Faraone, Biederman and Wozniak4 These data imply that ADHD should show a familial or genetic link to bipolar disorder, but these new data are the first to place that hypothesis on firm empirical ground.

Will these results influence clinical practice? I hope so. To begin with, the diagnosis of bipolar disorder among youth has been controversial, particularly among youth with ADHD. Family studies address such controversies because they can show associations between diagnoses at different developmental stages in the same family. Because the diagnosis of ADHD is not controversial in youth and the diagnosis of bipolar disorder is not controversial in adults, showing a familial link between ADHD in youth and parental bipolar disorder cannot be criticised for loose diagnoses of bipolar disorder in children. This transgenerational validation of the association between the two disorders thus strengthens the hypothesis that the two disorders co-occur in youth as they do in adults.

A recent meta-analysis provided insight into this issue. Reference Faraone, Biederman and Wozniak4 Among studies of adult relatives of patients with ADHD, the relative risk for bipolar disorder was 2.2. Among studies of child relatives, the relative risk for bipolar disorder was 2.1. These nearly identical relative risks suggest that the familial link between ADHD and bipolar disorder cannot be accounted for by misdiagnosing youth with ADHD as having bipolar disorder. Such data help clinicians understand the validity of diagnosing bipolar disorder among youth with ADHD when the formulation of both diagnoses adheres strictly to diagnostic criteria.

Because ADHD comorbid with bipolar disorder is a highly disabling condition, missing the diagnosis of bipolar disorder in a child with ADHD will have deleterious consequences, which might have been avoided given the availability of treatments for bipolar disorder in children. Larsson et al's findings suggest that clinicians be alert to the potential for relatives of patients with ADHD to have bipolar disorder. Reference Larsson, Rydén, Boman, Långström, Lichtenstein and Landén1 Meta-analyses report the risk for bipolar disorder in families with ADHD to be 6.8% for offspring, 5.9% for siblings and 5.1% for parents. Reference Faraone, Biederman and Wozniak4 This suggests that relatives of patients with ADHD be screened for bipolar disorder given that untreated bipolar disorder can lead to substantial adversity in the family environment.

The clinical implications of the genetic link between ADHD and schizophrenia are less clear given that the Larssen et al and Hamshere et al results are the first clear finding of such an association. Moreover, compared with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia is much rarer in youth and less likely to have an impact on clinical practice. But this finding does have clear implications for theory and research in psychiatry. From a theoretical perspective, these cross-disorder associations challenge the validity of the distinct Kraepelian categories instantiated by the DSM/ICD. The cross-disorder findings reported in this issue of the Journal are compelling, not only because of the high quality of the studies, but also because these studies are consistent with recent work in psychiatric genetics. There are many examples of widespread pleiotropic effects of risk variants on DSM/ICD categories, including statistically significant sharing of rare copy number variants for autism, ADHD and schizophrenia (e.g. Williams et al Reference Williams, Franke, Mick, Anney, Freitag and Gill5 ) as well as significant sharing of common variants implicated in genome-wide scans of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, autism, depression and ADHD. 2

Towards dimensional measures of psychopathology

Larsson et al's data also provide a statistically powerful test of the hypothesis that ADHD plus bipolar disorder is a familial syndrome distinct from either ADHD or bipolar disorder when they occur alone. By finding an association between ADHD in the absence of bipolar disorder and bipolar disorder in the absence of ADHD, Larsson et al have refuted findings from several large family studies that found the two disorders to be co-transmitted within families (e.g. Biederman et al Reference Biederman, Faraone, Petty, Martelon, Woodworth and Wozniak6 ). Although more work is needed to clarify these discrepant results, the work of Larsson et al confirms a larger body of work suggesting that, in contrast to the DSM/ICD, it may be best to think of psychopathology in dimensional rather than categorical terms.

A good deal of data support the idea that psychopathology, and its underlying aetiology, are better described by dimensional rather than categorical constructs. In 1967, Gottesman & Shields Reference Gottesman and Shields7 proposed a polygenic theory of schizophrenia that has since been confirmed with molecular genetic data showing not only that the aetiology of schizophrenia has a substantial polygenic component, but also that this polygenic aetiology overlaps with the aetiology of bipolar disorder. 2 The DSM/ICD paradigm deals with the dimensional nature of psychopathology by creating new categories that are mild versions of existing categories (e.g. schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder; major depression and dysthymia). If Larsson et al are correct, ADHD and bipolar disorder may fall on a genetic continuum of severity, with patients with ADHD but not bipolar disorder being at the mild end, patients with bipolar disorder but not ADHD having greater severity and patients with both ADHD and bipolar disorder having the greatest severity.

Other lines of evidence support the hypothesis that a dimensional perspective on ADHD is valid. Many studies have found an excellent correspondence between quantitative measures of ADHD and categorical diagnoses. These studies show that children with ADHD are at one extreme of a quantitative dimension and that, on this quantitative dimension, there is no obvious bimodality that separates children with ADHD from others. Moreover, people with symptoms of ADHD that do not exceed diagnostic thresholds show patterns of comorbidity, familial transmission and cortical thinning that suggest they have a mild form of ADHD. Reference Shaw, Gilliam, Liverpool, Weddle, Malek and Sharp8,Reference Faraone, Biederman, Spencer, Mick, Murray and Petty9 Dimensional measures of ADHD are highly heritable, about 70-90%, which is similar to the heritability of the ADHD diagnosis (e.g. Sherman et al Reference Sherman, Iacono and McGue10 ). And mathematical modelling of twin studies concludes that ADHD is best viewed as the extreme of a behaviour that varies throughout the entire population (e.g. Gjone et al Reference Gjone, Stevenson and Sundet11 ).

Despite these considerations favouring a dimensional model of ADHD, the true underlying architecture of its aetiology may be more complex. Rare cases of ADHD are due to gross abnormalities of chromosomes. Reference Faraone and Mick12 We do not know whether the smaller deletions and duplications known as copy number variants also mark categorical cases. For most patients with ADHD, available data reject the idea that all these cases are caused by the exact same pattern of genetic mutations and adverse environmental exposures. Instead, there are likely to be several aetiological profiles of ADHD, which may correspond to varying degrees of compromise in the neural networks that underlie the disorder.

Unravelling such complexities leaves a difficult task for ADHD researchers. Although hi-tech developments in neuroscience and molecular genetics may be up to that task, Larsson et al's work reminds us that the low-tech tools of genetic epidemiology still play a key role in our quest to understand the aetiology of ADHD and its links to other disorders.

References

1 Larsson, H, Rydén, E, Boman, M, Långström, N, Lichtenstein, P, Landén, M. Risk of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia in relatives of people with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 203: 103–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 2013; 381: 1371–9.Google Scholar
3 Hamshere, ML, Stergiakouli, E, Langley, K, Martin, J, Holmans, P, Kent, L, et al A shared polygenic contribution between childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and adult schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 203: 107–11.Google Scholar
4 Faraone, SV, Biederman, J, Wozniak, J. Examining the comorbidity between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and bipolar I disorder: a meta-analysis of family genetic studies. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169: 1256–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Williams, NM, Franke, B, Mick, E, Anney, RJ, Freitag, CM, Gill, M, et al Genome-wide analysis of copy number variants in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder confirms the role of rare variants and implicates duplications at 15q13.3. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169: 195204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6 Biederman, J, Faraone, SV, Petty, C, Martelon, M, Woodworth, KY, Wozniak, J. Further evidence that pediatric-onset bipolar disorder comorbid with ADHD represents a distinct subtype: results from a large controlled family study. J Psychiatr Res 2013; 47: 1522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Gottesman, II, Shields, J. A polygenic theory of schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1967; 58: 199205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Shaw, P, Gilliam, M, Liverpool, M, Weddle, C, Malek, M, Sharp, W, et al Cortical development in typically developing children with symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity: support for a dimensional view of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168: 143–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Faraone, SV, Biederman, J, Spencer, TJ, Mick, E, Murray, K, Petty, C, et al Diagnosing adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: are late onset and subthreshold diagnoses valid? Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163: 1720–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Sherman, D, Iacono, W, McGue, M. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder dimensions: a twin study of inattention and impulsivity hyperactivity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36: 745–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Gjone, H, Stevenson, J, Sundet, J. Genetic influence on parent-reported attention-related problems in a Norwegian general population twin sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996; 35: 588–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12 Faraone, SV, Mick, E. Molecular genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2010; 33: 159–80.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.