Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-22T13:47:26.682Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotion processing in depression and anxiety disorders in older adults: systematic review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 December 2020

Vanessa Gray*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand
Katie M. Douglas
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand
Richard J. Porter
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago; and Canterbury District Health Board, Christchurch, New Zealand
*
Correspondence: Vanessa Gray. Email: vanessa.gray@otago.ac.nz
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Emotional cognition and effective interpretation of affective information is an important factor in social interactions and everyday functioning, and difficulties in these areas may contribute to aetiology and maintenance of mental health conditions. In younger people with depression and anxiety, research suggests significant alterations in behavioural and brain activation aspects of emotion processing, with a tendency to appraise neutral stimuli as negative and attend preferentially to negative stimuli. However, in ageing, research suggests that emotion processing becomes subject to a ‘positivity effect’, whereby older people attend more to positive than negative stimuli.

Aims

This review examines data from studies of emotion processing in Late-Life Depression and Late-Life Anxiety to attempt to understand the significance of emotion processing variations in these conditions, and their interaction with changes in emotion processing that occur with ageing.

Method

We conducted a systematic review following PRISMA guidelines. Articles that used an emotion-based processing task, examined older persons with depression or an anxiety disorder and included a healthy control group were included.

Results

In Late-Life Depression, there is little consistent behavioural evidence of impaired emotion processing, but there is evidence of altered brain circuitry during these processes. In Late-Life Anxiety and Post-Traumatic Stress disorder, there is evidence of interference with processing of negative or threat-related words.

Conclusions

How these findings fit with the positivity bias of ageing is not clear. Future research is required in larger groups, further examining the interaction between illness and age and the significance of age at disease onset.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

Emotional cognition involves a range of functions, including ‘perceiving, interpreting, managing, and generating responses to socially relevant stimuli, such as intentions and behaviour of others’.Reference Green, Hellemann, Horan, Lee and Wynn1 Being able to undertake these functions efficiently is an important part of interpersonal relationships and social interactions. Interpretation of affective information is vital to these interactions as it influences emotional states and governs behavioural responses in social situations. Difficulties in social situations and avoidance of these, in the context of mental disorders, may maintain clinical levels of distress in patients and hinder attempts to treat their disorder. Further, emotional material can impair efficient cognitive processing by inducing biases in attention or decision-making, and by interfering with efficient cognitive processing, thereby impairing other aspects of cognition.

Emotion Processing in Depression

The cognitive neuropsychological hypothesis of depressionReference Warren, Pringle and Harmer2 posits that in depression there is both behavioural and neurocircuitry evidence of a bias toward negative emotional stimuli. It is suggested that those who are more vulnerable to depression tend to perceive social cues as more negative and attend to, and recall more, negative information.Reference Disner, Beevers, Haigh and Beck3 These biases may play a role in precipitating depression, maintaining depression and conferring susceptibility to relapse. Antidepressants may reverse this bias relatively quickly, but it takes some time for this change to be translated, via improved social interactions, into a reduction in depressive symptoms.Reference Harmer, Bhagwagar, Perrett, Völlm, Cowen and Goodwin4,Reference Pringle, Browning, Cowen and Harmer5 Multiple paradigms have been used to investigate emotion processing, and several, when studied in groups of younger individuals with depression, show differences broadly supporting the cognitive neuropsychological theory. For example, using a dot probe task, several studies have shown a bias toward sad faces compared with neutral faces in patients experiencing a major depressive episode.Reference Bourke, Douglas and Porter6 In addition, there is a bias in individuals with depression toward perceiving positive (happy), neutral or ambiguous facial expressions as more negative or less happy, compared with those in healthy control groups.Reference Hale, Jansen, Bouhuys and van den Hoofdakker7,Reference Leppänen, Milders, Bell, Terriere and Hietanen8 In more complex social processing, examined with the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task (RMET) and other complex tasks, people with depression perform significantly worse than control participants.Reference Bora and Berk9

Emotion Processing in Anxiety

In younger adults with anxiety disorders, there is the tendency for a bias toward threat-related stimuli, which is hypothesised to maintain a heightened sense of threat. For example, in facial expression recognition (FER) tasks, evidence suggests that individuals with social anxiety are not significantly different from healthy controls in identifying facial expressions,Reference Philippot and Douilliez10,Reference Stevens, Gerlach and Rist11 but show a tendency to misidentify neutral facial expressions as angry.Reference Bell, Bourke, Colhoun, Carter, Frampton and Porter12 Attentional bias toward threat-related stimuli has been reported in Generalised Anxiety Disorder (GAD), when using a dot probe task.Reference Bradley, Mogg, White, Groom and De Bono13 There is also evidence of increased response latencies to both anxiety-related words and generally negative words when using emotional Stroop (eStroop) tasks across anxiety disorders, suggesting an attentional bias toward threat-related stimuli.Reference Joyal, Wensing, Levasseur-Moreau, Leblond, Sack and Fecteau14 In younger adults, changes in emotion processing similar to those in other anxiety disorders have also been seen in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). For example, a meta-analysis of eStroop task performance in PTSDReference Cisler, Wolitzky-Taylor, Adams, Babson, Badour and Willems15 found that individuals with PTSD, compared with healthy controls, showed impairments in the eStroop task when processing trauma-related and generally threatening, but not positive, information.

Changes in Emotion Processing in Ageing

Multiple experimental paradigms examining different aspects of attention and memory, as well as meta-analysis of attention and memory in younger compared with older adults,Reference Reed, Chan and Mikels16 have suggested a ‘positivity bias’ in older adults.Reference Carstensen and DeLiema17 For example, older participants have been shown to be more likely to remember positive images compared with negative images, whereas a preference for negative images was observed in young adults.Reference Charles, Mather and Carstensen18 This positivity bias has been demonstrated in multiple other emotion processing paradigms, including those examining autobiographical memory,Reference Cuddy, Sikka, Silveira, Bai and Vanstone19,Reference Kennedy, Mather and Carstensen20 working memory,Reference Mikels, Larkin, Reuter-Lorenz and Carstensen21 attention to emotional facesReference Mather and Carstensen22 and recall of facial expressions.Reference Sava, Krolak-Salmon, Delphin-Combe, Cloarec and Chainay23 Evidence also shows that older adults are slower and less accurate in studies investigating the effects of ageing on face perception by using tasks such as face detection,Reference Norton, McBain and Chen24 face identificationReference Habak, Wilkinson and Wilson25,Reference Megreya and Bindemann26 and emotion recognition.Reference Calder, Keane, Manly, Sprengelmeyer, Scott and Nimmo-Smith27Reference Hildebrandt, Wilhelm, Schmiedek, Herzmann and Sommer29

Several theories attempt to explain this bias. Socioemotional selectivity theory, suggests that as time to the end of life becomes shorter, goals change focus and reflect a preference for emotional meaning and satisfaction.Reference Carstensen30 This change in goals then affects how we process emotions. An alternative theory is dynamic integration theory.Reference Labouvie-Vief31 Since processing of negative affect may be more cognitively demanding,Reference Pratto and John32 this theory suggests that as we age, diminishing cognitive capacity (reduced capacity for processing) makes it more difficult to accept and integrate negative feelings, and therefore older adults disconnect from negative feelings, resulting in a positivity bias.Reference Labouvie-Vief, Diehl, Jain and Zhang33 However, if this theory is accurate then it is argued that this effect would be greatest in those with poorer or impaired cognitive function, whereas in fact, the opposite appears to be the case.Reference Petrican, Moscovitch and Schimmack34,Reference Mather and Knight35 Furthermore, depression is associated with cognitive impairment but, as noted, seems to increase bias for negative material – although not necessarily the integration and processing of this.Reference Porter, Robinson, Malhi and Gallagher36

A further theory, the aging brain model, suggests that the changes in emotional cognition seen in older adults may be associated with age-related changes in adrenergic and amygdala function,Reference Cacioppo, Berntson, Bechara, Tranel, Hawkley, Todorov, Fiske and Prentice37 which leads to an impairment in the processing of negative stimuli. Studies do show differences in brain activation associated with emotion processing as ageing occurs, indirectly suggesting a decline in processing capacity in emotion processing areas. For example, older adults show reduced limbic and greater cortical activation (e.g. insula, frontal cortex) during processing of emotional faces.Reference Fischer, Sandblom, Gavazzeni, Fransson, Wright and Bäckman38,Reference Gunning-Dixon, Gur, Perkins, Schroeder, Turner and Turetsky39 Some of these changes have been shown to correlate with the positivity bias. Sakaki et al,Reference Sakaki, Nga and Mather40 found increased negative coupling between the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and amygdala and enhanced MPFC activity when learning emotional faces. This increase in MPFC activity may indicate an attempt to overcome an age related decline in capacity of MPFC processing areas. In general, a reduction in activity of prefrontal cortical processing areas, with concomitant increased limbic activation (amygdala, basal ganglia), has also been shown in studies of depression both in young and older individuals during emotion processing,Reference Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer and Thase41 in particular in response to sad faces.Reference Surguladze, Brammer, Keedwell, Giampietro, Young and Travis42

Research in Late-Life Depression (LLD) is further complicated by the issue of age at disease onset. Onset of depression at a later age is associated with greater neuropsychological abnormality, white matter hyperintensities and a higher rate of dementia at follow-up.Reference Alexopoulos, Young and Meyers43Reference Jacoby and Levy45 The fact that late-onset depression has not been associated with frequent episodes across the lifespan may also be important in determining emotion processing. It may therefore be that emotion processing deficits, if they exist, are different between early- and late-onset depression.

To date, research examining emotion processing in older adults with mood and anxiety disorders has not been systematically synthesised. This review aims to examine this literature to understand the interactions between depression and anxiety disorders, cognitive decline and the positivity bias in older adults, and their effects on emotion processing.

Based on the factors discussed above, our questions were as follows:

  1. (a) In LLD, Late-Life Anxiety (LLA) and PTSD, does the positivity bias in old age mitigate the emotion processing abnormalities that might be expected given the evidence of negativity bias in younger people with depression or anxiety disorders?

  2. (b) Are changes in emotion processing circuitry in LLD, LLA and PTSD similar to those seen in younger people with depression or anxiety?

  3. (c) Are behavioural and brain changes different in people with late-onset depression compared with early-onset depression, and what is the relationship between these changes and age-related cognitive decline?

Method

Protocol and registration

Details of the protocol for this systematic review were registered with the Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (registration identifier: CRD42020124980; accessed at: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42020124980).

Search strategy

Up to December 2019, a systematic review of electronic databases was carried out for relevant papers, using PubMed and Web of Science. In the initial search, the search terms used were ‘depression’, ‘anxiety’, ‘PTSD’, ‘bipolar’, ‘emotion processing’, ‘older persons’ and ‘elderly’, in different permutations. Reference lists of all relevant papers were then checked to ensure inclusion of all pertinent articles. Citations of relevant articles were then followed using Web of Science to allow for capture of any missed articles.

Inclusion criteria

Peer-reviewed articles involving assessment with an emotion-based processing task and comparison of a clinical sample with a healthy control sample were included in the review. Studies examining all psychiatric disorders were to be considered; however, studies were only found for depression, anxiety and PTSD populations. Sample participants were to be aged over 60 years and samples were to be categorised as ‘older adult’ or similar.

Exclusion criteria

Reasons for exclusion were comorbid major medical or neurological disorder in either group in the study, and studies involving participants with mild or greater levels of cognitive impairment (Mini-Mental State Examination score <25 or equivalent). All studies were limited to English-language publications.

Full study review

This review was undertaken with recommended Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines and using the PRISMA statement to guide the search, screening and extraction process.Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff and Altman46 Articles were screened by one of the reviewers, who independently reviewed the titles and abstracts of studies, to accept or reject for full-text review. The same reviewer then examined the full texts of the studies that had passed initial screening, to determine if they still met inclusion criteria. If inclusion of a paper was unclear, all three co-authors discussed this, to achieve a consensus. For each study, the following data was extracted: characteristics of the sample, including sample size, average age and baseline depression/anxiety severity; study design; cognitive tests used during assessment and study outcomes.

Results

The initial search for this review found 462 articles (Fig. 1). After a title and abstract review, 440 of these were excluded. The full text of the remaining 22 studies were obtained and reviewed. Nineteen additional papers were found through examination of the reference sections of the full-text studies. Of these 41 studies, 21 were then excluded for not being clinical or experimental studies, not including older participants or emotion processing, or not being in English. The remaining 20 papers are reviewed as follows. All of the studies included clinically diagnosed populations, unless otherwise stated. In this area of research, there is little consistency regarding terminology. LLD, depression and late- or early-onset depression are all used, with varying intended meaning; as such, we have deferred to the original authors and used the terminology that was present in the original article.

Fig. 1 Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses diagram of studies retrieved for the review.

Depression

Table 1 displays characteristics and main findings of studies examining behavioural data of samples with depression.

Table 1 Selected demographic characteristics of included studies examining behavioural data of samples with depression

LLD, Late-Life Depression.

Emotion interference

The emotion processing tasks discussed in this section involve inhibition of emotional information to carry out a cognitive task. This is most commonly a modified version of the Stroop paradigm (eStroop), which uses words that are of a positive, negative or neutral valence. Participants are required to name the colour of the presented word, but to ignore the word itself. A pilot study by Dudley et alReference Dudley, O'Brien, Barnett, McGuckin and Britton47 (12 depressed, 12 controls) showed an interaction between group and word valence, which was explained by a greater interference (longer time to colour name) of depression-related words in the depressed group, with an effect size difference compared with controls of 0.9 (Cohen's d). Broomfield et alReference Broomfield, Davies, MacMahon, Ali and Cross48 (16 depressed, 19 controls) also showed a group×valence interaction in participants with depression compared with controls, with participants with depression being slower to respond to negative words compared with neutral words. The group×valence interaction persisted when anxiety was controlled for. Using a similar eStroop paradigm, Callahan and HudonReference Callahan and Hudon49 found those with LLD (n = 10) were generally slower than controls (n = 15), but with no difference seen across valences. No group×valence analysis was reported. Finally, in 55 people with LLD and 40 controls, using an eStroop,Reference Huang, Fan, Lee, Liu, Chen and Lin50 controls were more accurate and had shorter latencies. There was no effect of emotion on accuracy, but there was on latency, with both positive and negative words associated with longer latencies. There was, however, no group×emotion interaction on any of the measures. Apart from the study by Dudley et alReference Dudley, O'Brien, Barnett, McGuckin and Britton47 , no estimates of effect size difference between depressed and control groups are given.

Mah and PollockReference Mah and Pollock51 used a facial emotion-based paradigm to measure emotion inhibition in 11 participants with depression and 11 controls. In this paradigm, participants were presented with faces displaying different emotions (happy, sad, fearful and neutral) and were asked to answer questions about a non-affective aspect of the face (inhibition). There was a significant group×valence interaction, whereby overall latencies were similar in both groups, but were significantly longer for all emotion-laden stimuli (not only negative emotions) compared with neutral stimuli in the control group and did not vary by emotion in the depressed group.

Zhou et alReference Zhou, Dai, Rossi and Li52 studied event-related potentials (ERP), in response to emotional faces, in 14 older adults with depressive symptoms (but specifically not meeting criteria for a DSM-IV diagnosis of depression), and compared this group with 14 controls. The study was included in the review, after discussion, since the patient group had significant depressive symptoms despite no formal diagnosis (Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale mean group score of 20.21 ± 5.65). Participants were presented with an emotional prime (facial expressions: happy, sad, ambiguous) and then asked to identify the emotion of the target which followed. Older adults with depressive symptoms had smaller ERP amplitudes compared with controls, regardless of valence of the priming stimulus. Older adults with depressive symptoms showed no differences in amplitude between the different prime valences. In control participants, there were no differences found between happy and ambiguous primes, but there were significant differences between happy and sad (larger ERP) primes, and between ambiguous and sad (larger ERP) primes. However, it was notable that the group×prime interaction was not statistically significant (p = 0.07), making further analyses and conclusions very tentative. Behavioural data showed no differences between groups.

Emotion recognition and memory

Most commonly used is an FER task,Reference Harmer, Bhagwagar, Perrett, Völlm, Cowen and Goodwin4 in which participants are presented with various facial expressions (usually happy, sad, angry, disgusted, fearful, surprised and neutral) and asked to identify the emotion portrayed. Mah and PollockReference Mah and Pollock51 used an FER task in which participants were presented with happy, sad, fearful or neutral faces. Although overall accuracy was similar between depressed (n = 11) and control (n = 11) groups, there was a significantly increased probability of participants with depression incorrectly identifying neutral faces.

Savaskan et alReference Savaskan, Müller, Böhringer, Schulz and Schächinger53 used a memory for faces paradigm that included both happy and angry faces. At baseline, patients with depression (n = 18) recalled fewer faces overall compared with controls (n = 22). There was no group×valence interaction, although the authors suggested that compared with controls, patients with depression showed lower ability to recognise previously viewed happy facial expressions after a delay. Following 4 weeks of treatment, the patient group showed a significant reduction in depressive symptoms and a significant improvement in general cognitive function. Further, their memory for angry faces significantly improved from baseline, whereas no effect was seen for happy faces.

Two studies examined emotion recognition and memory for emotional material, using verbal stimuli. Brassen et alReference Brassen, Kalisch, Weber-Fahr, Braus and Buechel54 used an emotion recognition task with positive, neutral and negative words to examine neural responses in 13 antidepressant-naïve female patients with LLD versus 13 controls. Participants were shown a positive, negative or neutral adjective, which was then replaced by a response screen where they indicated the valence of the word. No significant differences in response correctness, response time or misattributions of emotion were found at either time point between the two groups.

Callahan et alReference Callahan, Simard, Mouiha, Rousseau, Laforce and Hudon55 examined recall of neutral, positive and negative words from a list including 12 of each. At immediate recall, control participants (n = 28) displayed better recall of positive and negative words compared with neutral words, whereas participants with depression (n = 19) recalled more negative than neutral words. However, performance of the two groups was not directly compared. At delayed recall, both groups generally showed better recall of emotional compared with neutral words. During recognition, all participants were more likely to report false recognition for emotional than neutral words. Once again, there was no group comparison.

Social cognition

The RMETReference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste and Plumb56 requires individuals to identify complex or social emotions from images portraying only the eyes of the face. Szanto et alReference Szanto, Dombrovski, Sahakian, Mulsant, Houck and Reynolds57 examined the RMET in older adults with depression who had (n = 24) or had not (n = 38) attempted suicide. A control group was also examined (n = 28). Individuals who had attempted suicide performed significantly worse than healthy controls. The performance of participants with depression who had not attempted suicide fell between controls and those who had attempted suicide, but was not significantly different from either. Further analysis showed that when global cognitive function was accounted for (Mattis Dementia Rating ScaleReference Mattis58), the significant difference found for individuals who had attempted suicide was not maintained, suggesting that the group's reduced ability to recognise social emotions may have been attributable to global cognitive impairment, rather than a specific impairment in social cognition.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies

Table 2 displays characteristics and main findings of studies examining neuroimaging data of samples with depression.

Table 2 Selected demographic characteristics of included studies examining neuroimaging data of samples with depression

LLD, Late-Life Depression.

Aizenstein et alReference Aizenstein, Andreescu, Edelman, Cochran, Price and Butters59 used a facial expression affective-reactivity task. Participants with depression (n = 33) showed greater subgenual cingulate activity during affective processing compared with controls (n = 27) with a significant correlation between white matter hyperintensity and activity. Huang et alReference Huang, Fan, Lee, Liu, Chen and Lin50 (55 participants with LLD, 40 controls) showed reduced activation in middle frontal gyrus and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and increased activation in anterior cingulate cortex, in the LLD group compared with controls in a study examining activation during an eStroop task. This was mediated by cognitive reserve such that greater cognitive reserve correlated with greater middle frontal gyrus activation in the LLD group. Brassen et alReference Brassen, Kalisch, Weber-Fahr, Braus and Buechel54 reported that in comparison with controls (n = 13), female patients with LLD (n = 13) showed attenuated neural response in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in response to negative compared with positive words. When correlated with depression severity in the depressed group (Geriatric Depression Scale), reduced activation in the medial orbito-frontal cortex was correlated with higher depression scores. Increased activation in the superior medial frontal cortex, for positive compared with negative words, was also correlated with higher depression scores. At the 7-month follow-up, when patients’ symptoms had significantly improved, this attenuated response was found to have normalised.

Briceño et alReference Briceño, Rapport, Kassel, Bieliauskas, Zubieta and Weisenbach60 examined the effects of age and gender on neural circuits in an FER paradigm that involved four emotions (happiness, sadness, fear and anger). The study included participants in younger and older age groups, as well as participants with and without depression (older participants with depression, n = 26; older controls, n = 25). When depressed and non-depressed groups were not separated by age and gender, no overall effects were detected between groups. However, when separated by age and gender, older women with depression showed hypoactivation in emotion processing circuits, particularly the right prefrontal cortex, when compared with older controls. In contrast, older males with depression showed hyperactivation in these areas when compared with the control group.

Vanyukov et alReference Vanyukov, Szanto, Siegle, Hallquist, Reynolds and Aizenstein61 used the faces and shapes task,Reference Hariri, Mattay, Tessitore and Kolachana62 in which participants are required to match a target face to one of two presented faces. Authors used faces showing anger and fear during the face trials. In neutral trials, shapes were used as non-affective controls. The study examined patients with depression (n = 13), patients with depression who had attempted suicide (n = 18) and controls (n = 18). There was no difference in response to angry faces in either depressed group compared with controls. Responses to fear-related stimuli were not discussed.

Anxiety disorders

Table 3 presents characteristics and key findings from reviewed studies examining samples with anxiety disorders.

Table 3 Selected demographic characteristics of included studies examining anxiety and post-traumatic stress disorder

GAD, Generalised Anxiety Disorder; BNST, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; PTSD, Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder.

Price et alReference Price, Siegle and Mohlman63 divided 60 community recruited adults into high-, medium- and low-trait anxiety, based on responses to the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ). A total of 90% of participants in the high-worry group (n = 20) scored ≥50 on the PSWQ, which has been noted as a cut-off for late-life GAD.Reference Webb, Diefenbach, Wagener, Novy, Kunik and Rhoades64 The remaining participants (n = 40) formed comparison groups. An eStroop task containing positive, threat-related, and neutral words was used. In the high-worry group, response time for threat-related words was longer than neutral words (Cohen's d = 0.76). The high-worry group also showed faster reaction times for positive words than neutral. The low- and medium-worry groups showed the opposite pattern: faster response time for threat-related words compared with neutral, and slower response time for positive words compared with neutral. Between-group comparisons of estimated marginal means showed a difference between the high-worry and low-worry group, with bias toward threat-related words being greater (Cohen's d = 0.67).

Mohlman et alReference Mohlman, Price and Vietri65 used a dot probe task which included depression, threat, positive and neutrally valenced words to examine attentional biases. There was no significant between-group differences in reaction time overall or between word types in GAD (n = 34) versus controls (n = 28). Using differences between probe-target congruent word pairs and incongruent pairs, bias scores were calculated. No significant differences in bias scores between GAD and control groups were found. A bias away from positive information in positive–neutral pairs was seen across all participants in the study. No significant change in performance following treatment (cognitive–behavioural therapy versus waitlist) was found.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies

Price et alReference Price, Eldreth and Mohlman66 examined functional magnetic resonance imaging responses to performance on an eStroop task in older adults with late-life GAD. Participants with GAD (n = 16) had slower reaction times when responding to negative versus neutral words, compared with controls (n = 12) (Cohen's d = 0.85), and also showed less activation in the prefrontal cortex in response to negative compared with neutral words. An increase in activation was seen in the GAD group in the left amygdala, compared with the control group.

Two studiesReference Wu, Mennin, Ly, Karim, Banihashemi and Tudorascu67,Reference Karim, Tudorascu, Aizenstein, Walker, Good and Andreescu68 examined functional connectivity associated with emotional reactivity in late-life GAD compared with healthy controls. Neither study found any significant differences between the GAD group and the controls, using a faces and shapes task. Both studies examined the data using the factor of worry, as measured by the PSWQ. Wu et alReference Wu, Mennin, Ly, Karim, Banihashemi and Tudorascu67 found that there was a significant interaction between group and ‘worry’ on connectivity between the left amygdala and left orbitofrontal cortex, MPFC and both anterior cingulate cortices, and on connectivity between the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and the left orbitofrontal cortex. When worry was examined across groups, there was a U-shaped curve, whereby connectivity between limbic and cortical areas was optimal at medium levels of ‘worry’. The effect sizes of these curves varied from r 2 = 0.21 to r 2 = 0.25. Karim et alReference Karim, Tudorascu, Aizenstein, Walker, Good and Andreescu68 found that across the groups, increased global anxiety, measured by the Hamilton Anxiety and Depression Scale, was associated with greater activation in the parahippocampal area and precuneus. In contrast, worry (PSWQ) was associated with decreased precuneus and prefrontal activation. Complex mediation analyses broadly suggested that the mediation between increased white matter hyperintensity burden and anxiety symptoms are mediated by increased activation of limbic and paralimbic structures, and decreased activation of regulatory regions, such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

PTSD

One study, published as two papers, examined emotion processing in older populations with PTSD. Wittekind et alReference Wittekind, Muhtz, Jelinek and Moritz69 used a spatial cueing task involving priming with an emotional facial stimulus (anxiety, depression, trauma and neutral), followed by a spatial (left or right) decision, in response to a non-affective target (26 participants with PTSD, 22 controls). Authors found no attentional biases within the PTSD group for trauma-related stimuli. However, post hoc redistribution of participants into those who did or did not meet the criteria for depression showed that participants in the depressed sample had longer reaction times to depressive stimuli (Cohen's d = 1.5).

The same participants also completed an eStroop taskReference Wittekind, Muhtz, Moritz and Jelinek70 containing words related to depression, trauma, anxiety or neutral words. Participants with PTSD showed longer response latencies to trauma- and depression-related words compared with healthy controls, but no differences in latency for neutral and anxious words. Slowing for trauma compared with neutral words was found across all groups in this study.

Discussion

The main findings of the review were as follows. First, at a behavioural level, evidence regarding differences in emotion processing between individuals with LLD and controls is inconsistent. This is the case for interference of emotional material in cognitive processes (eStroop), memory for emotional compared with other material, and the explicit process of recognition of emotional facial expressions. Second, there are few studies in LLA. Studies suggest interference with processing by threat related words in anxiety and by trauma-related words in PTSD, but there are no replication studies. Finally, studies show differences in activation in emotional processing circuitry in LLD, with the general pattern of increased limbic but reduced prefrontal activity, as in younger individuals with depression.

The review more specifically examined three main questions as below.

How does the positivity bias seen in older persons interact with biases toward negative or threat-related emotional material in depression or anxiety?

There are no consistent findings regarding any of the aspects of emotion processing studied. This was the case both for implicit processes (such as the eStroop) and for explicit processes (such as emotion recognition). Not all of these phenomena have been consistently replicated in younger people with depression either. For example, results on the eStroop have not been found to be consistent.Reference Epp, Dobson, Dozois and Frewen71 The phenomenon seen most consistently in younger depression is the misinterpretation of, or attentional bias toward, neutral faces.Reference Bourke, Douglas and Porter6,Reference Harmer, Goodwin and Cowen72 In LLD, this was only examined in three studies and was not seen consistently.

The lack of consistent evidence of bias toward negative emotional stimuli in LLD may reflect a situation in which, on average, negative bias is less in LLD than in younger depression. It could be hypothesised that this relates to the positivity bias associated with ageing, which counteracts the biases seen in depression. However, the inconsistencies in the data are more likely to be related to the small numbers of studies and limited power of most studies. Further issues with the data are that there are no studies directly comparing emotion processing between younger patients with depression and LLD. Similarly no comparisons across the lifespan have been conducted in anxiety disorders. Therefore, the interaction of age with depression or anxiety cannot be fully evaluated. Finally, studies generally did not examine possible complicating factors, such as concomitant cognitive impairment, age at disease onset and the effects of medication.

Are changes in emotion processing circuitry similar to those seen in younger people with depression or anxiety?

In younger people with depression, studies have generally shown a reduction in activity of dorsal-cognitive structures combined with increased activity of ventral-affective structures during emotion processing.Reference Roiser, Elliott and Sahakian73 The studies in LLD are broadly in line with this pattern with the largest studies showing increased activation of limbic structures or decreased activation of prefrontal structures in LLD compared with controls, in line with a general pattern of reduced top-down processing.Reference Huang, Fan, Lee, Liu, Chen and Lin50,Reference Brassen, Kalisch, Weber-Fahr, Braus and Buechel54,Reference Aizenstein, Andreescu, Edelman, Cochran, Price and Butters59

Of interest, there was also evidence of an interaction between depression and both white matter lesionsReference Aizenstein, Andreescu, Edelman, Cochran, Price and Butters59 and cognitive reserveReference Huang, Fan, Lee, Liu, Chen and Lin50 in determining patterns of activation during emotional processing. White matter changes were associated with an exaggeration of the increase in subgenual cingulate activation seen during emotion processing in LLD.Reference Aizenstein, Andreescu, Edelman, Cochran, Price and Butters59 In the study of Huang et alReference Huang, Fan, Lee, Liu, Chen and Lin50, severity of depression was associated with reduced medial frontal activation during emotion processing, but this was attenuated by having greater cognitive reserve (measured using years of education and verbal fluency). Both findings suggest a situation in which if processing capacity is reduced for a variety of possible reasons, this may impair efficient emotion processing, resulting in processing being driven to a greater extent by limbic structures.

Are behavioural and brain changes different in people with late-onset compared with early-onset depression, and what is the relationship between these changes and age-related cognitive decline?

Of note, our review excluded studies in which participants had a Mini-Mental State Examination score < 25. The rationale was that although we were interested in the interaction between mood and anxiety, cognitive ability and the relationship of these to emotion processing and positivity bias, we wanted to restrict the review to examining LLD and LLA and not to extend this into dementia. Four studies did examine the relationship between emotional processing and other aspects of cognitive functioning. Callahan et alReference Callahan, Simard, Mouiha, Rousseau, Laforce and Hudon55 examined the influence of depression on emotion processing in mild cognitive impairment (MCI), based on the suggestion that MCI plus depression is particularly likely to progress to dementia, and therefore constitutes a prognostically important subtype of MCI.Reference Rosenberg, Mielke, Appleby, Oh, Geda and Lyketsos74,Reference Apostolova and Cummings75 Consistent with the hypothesis that negative information requires greater capacity to process, Callahan et alReference Callahan, Simard, Mouiha, Rousseau, Laforce and Hudon55 showed that for patients with MCI, immediate recall was better for positive words than negative words; however, this was not the case for MCI with depression, healthy controls or LLD. A caveat to this conclusion is the lack of an analysis of group×valence interaction. Furthermore, a similar effect was not seen in a separate examination of effects on an eStroop test.Reference Callahan and Hudon49 Although Huang et alReference Huang, Fan, Lee, Liu, Chen and Lin50 did not find the hypothesised eStroop effect in LLD, there was more preserved middle frontal gyrus activity during eStroop performance in people with greater cognitive reserve. This suggests that cognitive reserve (measured using a combination of years of education and an executive task) mediates a more top-down emotional regulation, i.e. preserved processing capacity in those with greater cognitive reserve. Szanto et alReference Szanto, Dombrovski, Sahakian, Mulsant, Houck and Reynolds57 examined social cognition in LLD. Those who attempted suicide had lower scores than controls but, interestingly, this did not survive covarying for general cognitive function, suggesting that the two functions are related, at least in depression.

Specific issues in late-life anxiety disorders

Five studies examining emotion processing in LLA disorders were identified. Those using an eStroop task both showed increased latency for negatively valenced words.Reference Price, Siegle and Mohlman63,Reference Price, Eldreth and Mohlman66 In one of the studies which examined brain activation,Reference Price, Eldreth and Mohlman66 the hypothesised difference from controls was seen, with an increase in activation of part of the amygdala, accompanied by a decrease in activation of the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex in older patients with GAD. Two further studies showed no difference between patients with GAD and controls,Reference Wu, Mennin, Ly, Karim, Banihashemi and Tudorascu67,Reference Karim, Tudorascu, Aizenstein, Walker, Good and Andreescu68 in brain activation and connectivity. However, in one there was a complex relationship between worry and connectivity, suggesting that connectivity between limbic and cortical areas was maximal at an intermediate level of worry.Reference Price, Eldreth and Mohlman66 In the other study,Reference Wu, Mennin, Ly, Karim, Banihashemi and Tudorascu67 anxiety was associated with greater activation in parahippocampal areas and precuneus. In general this is in keeping with attenuation of activity in, and connectivity with, processing areas seen in younger patients.Reference Goossen, van der Starre and van der Heiden76

Data regarding interference by trauma-related words in eStroop tasks has been consistently demonstrated in younger adults with PTSD.Reference Cisler, Wolitzky-Taylor, Adams, Babson, Badour and Willems15 Both in LLA and PTSD, studies examining simple attention bias toward negative stimuli have not shown a difference from controls.Reference Mohlman, Price and Vietri65,Reference Wittekind, Muhtz, Jelinek and Moritz69 This may suggest that the basic focus of attention is not altered, but that negative emotional stimuli do interfere with processing.

Limitations and recommendations for future research

This review has limitations both directly related to the methodology and to the content of the studies reviewed. Related directly to the review, it considered English-language papers only. Although this is standard practice for an English-language review, it may mean that some relevant studies have been missed. Second, meta-analysis has not been possible given the heterogeneity of paradigms investigated in the studies examined, and in the variety of ways the data has been analysed and presented.

Limitations of the data, which can be translated into recommendations for the field are, as follows. First, although studies are in ‘older adults’, the majority have mean ages from 65 to 75 years, with the lower age cut-off being 60 years in most studies. This may mean that effects of age that might have been seen, for example, in 70–80 year olds, are washed out by there being relatively less effect in the lower age range. This could, of course, be overcome by studies being adequately powered to examine the effects of age in a linear fashion – possibly even over a larger age range so that the effects of age and its interaction with depression or anxiety could be examined. This would better reflect the fact that risk factors and neurobiology likely change in a linear fashion across the lifespan.Reference Schaakxs, Comijs, van der Mast, Schoevers, Beekman and Penninx77 Second, most studies did not analyse the effects of having early-onset compared with late-onset depression. Once again the issue is mainly one of power and as such this should be examined with sufficiently large studies. Third, a variety of paradigms have been used to study emotion processing even within similar processes. This makes it generally difficult to pool or synthesise results from different studies. Consensus on the most useful and clinically relevant paradigms would aid progress in the field. Fourth, studies have rarely attempted to determine the extent to which decline in non-emotional cognitive processes, such as executive function, may be affecting emotional processing directly. Future studies should consider undertaking testing of non-emotional memory and executive function and examining the relationship between this and emotion processing. Fifth, critically, studies have tended to be very small. Future studies should be adequately powered to show differences at least as small as 0.5 s.d. between groups. They should also, ideally, be large enough to take into account the possible effects of varying medication, late compared with early onset and age as a longitudinal factor on emotion processing. Finally, in reporting results, most studies use analysis of variance but do not report estimated marginal means and s.d., making it impossible to calculate the magnitude of differences between groups. These should be reported or effect sizes calculated.

In conclusion, the ability to correctly process and interpret emotions is an important part of social interactions, something that becomes especially important as we age. The cognitive neuropsychological hypothesis of depression also suggests that these interactions are an important part of the aetiology of depression and may provide a target for treatment. Indeed, packages of emotion recognition training are being developed to address this issue (e.g. the study by Penton-Voak et alReference Penton-Voak, Adams, Button, Fluharty, Dalili and Browning78).

The review has highlighted the fact that there are relatively few large studies of emotion processing in older adults with depression and anxiety disorders. We have provided recommendations for future research.

Given the lack of studies that examine emotion processing in depression or anxiety across the life cycle, it is not possible to determine the interaction of abnormalities in these conditions with the ageing positivity bias. In general, there have been findings of a bias toward negative stimuli, and concomitant alteration of brain activity to a pattern of greater limbic and reduced prefrontal cortex activation in younger people with depression and anxiety. Similar patterns have been shown in studies in older persons, although not consistently in some aspects of emotion processing.

Author contributions

V.G. conducted the systematic review of papers and prepared the first draft of the manuscript. R.J.P. and K.M.D. supervised the systematic review and reviewed and updated subsequent drafts.

Funding

We are grateful to the University of Otago for financial support, by providing V.G. with the Gilbert M. Tothill Scholarship in Psychological Medicine.

Declaration of interest

V.G. has nothing to disclose. K.M.D. uses software for cognitive remediation intervention research that is provided free of charge by Scientific Brain Training PRO. R.J.P. uses computer software at no cost, provided by SBT-Pro, for research. He has received support for travel to educational meetings from Servier and Lundbeck.

References

Green, MF, Hellemann, G, Horan, WP, Lee, J, Wynn, JK. From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: modeling the role of ability and motivation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69(12): 1216–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren, MB, Pringle, A, Harmer, CJ. A neurocognitive model for understanding treatment action in depression. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 2015; 370(1677): 20140213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Disner, SG, Beevers, CG, Haigh, EA, Beck, AT. Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat Rev Neurosci 2011; 12(8): 467–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, CJ, Bhagwagar, Z, Perrett, DI, Völlm, BA, Cowen, PJ, Goodwin, GM. Acute SSRI administration affects the processing of social cues in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003; 28(1): 148–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pringle, A, Browning, M, Cowen, PJ, Harmer, CJ. A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2011; 35(7): 1586–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourke, C, Douglas, K, Porter, R. Processing of facial emotion expression in major depression: a review. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2010; 44(8): 681–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, WW III, Jansen, JHC, Bouhuys, AL, van den Hoofdakker, RH. The judgment of facial expressions by depressed patients, their partners and controls. J Affect Disord 1998; 47(1): 6370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppänen, JM, Milders, M, Bell, JS, Terriere, E, Hietanen, JK. Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces. Psychiatry Res 2004; 128(2): 123–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E, Berk, M. Theory of mind in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. J Affect Disord 2016; 191: 4955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philippot, P, Douilliez, C. Social phobics do not misinterpret facial expression of emotion. Behav Res Ther 2005; 43(5): 639–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S, Gerlach, AL, Rist, F. Effects of alcohol on ratings of emotional facial expressions in social phobics. J Anxiety Disord 2008; 22(6): 940–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, C, Bourke, C, Colhoun, H, Carter, F, Frampton, C, Porter, R. The misclassification of facial expressions in generalised social phobia. J Anxiety Disord 2011; 25(2): 278–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, BP, Mogg, K, White, J, Groom, C, De Bono, J. Attentional bias for emotional faces in generalized anxiety disorder. Br J Clin Psychol 1999; 38(3): 267–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joyal, M, Wensing, T, Levasseur-Moreau, J, Leblond, J, Sack, AT, Fecteau, S. Characterizing emotional Stroop interference in posttraumatic stress disorder, major depression and anxiety disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2019; 14(4): e0214998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cisler, JM, Wolitzky-Taylor, KB, Adams, TG Jr, Babson, KA, Badour, CL, Willems, JL. The emotional Stroop task and posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 2011; 31(5): 817–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, AE, Chan, L, Mikels, JA. Meta-analysis of the age-related positivity effect: age differences in preferences for positive over negative information. Psychol Aging 2014; 29(1): 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, LL, DeLiema, M. The positivity effect: a negativity bias in youth fades with age. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2018; 19: 712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charles, ST, Mather, M, Carstensen, LL. Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. J Exp Psychol Gen 2003; 132(2): 310–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuddy, LL, Sikka, R, Silveira, K, Bai, S, Vanstone, A. Music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs) in Alzheimer disease: evidence for a positivity effect. Cog Psychol 2017; 4(1): 1277578.Google Scholar
Kennedy, Q, Mather, M, Carstensen, LL. The role of motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical memory. Psychol Sci 2004; 15(3): 208–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikels, JA, Larkin, GR, Reuter-Lorenz, PA, Carstensen, LL. Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychol Aging 2005; 20(4): 542–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M, Carstensen, LL. Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychol Sci 2003; 14(5): 409–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sava, A-A, Krolak-Salmon, P, Delphin-Combe, F, Cloarec, M, Chainay, H. Memory for faces with emotional expressions in Alzheimer's disease and healthy older participants: positivity effect is not only due to familiarity. Aging. Neuropsychol Cogn 2017; 24(1): 128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norton, D, McBain, R, Chen, Y. Reduced ability to detect facial configuration in middle-aged and elderly individuals: associations with spatiotemporal visual processing. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2009; 64(3): 328–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Habak, C, Wilkinson, F, Wilson, HR. Aging disrupts the neural transformations that link facial identity across views. Vision Res. 2008; 48(1): 915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Megreya, AM, Bindemann, M. Developmental improvement and age-related decline in unfamiliar face matching. Perception 2015; 44(1): 522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calder, AJ, Keane, J, Manly, T, Sprengelmeyer, R, Scott, S, Nimmo-Smith, I, et al. Facial expression recognition across the adult life span. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41(2): 195202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hildebrandt, A, Wilhelm, O, Herzmann, G, Sommer, W. Face and object cognition across adult age. Psychol Aging 2013; 28(1): 243–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hildebrandt, A, Wilhelm, O, Schmiedek, F, Herzmann, G, Sommer, W. On the specificity of face cognition compared with general cognitive functioning across adult age. Psychol Aging 2011; 26(3): 701–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, LL. The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science 2006; 312(5782): 1913–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labouvie-Vief, G. Dynamic integration: affect, cognition, and the self in adulthood. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2003; 12(6): 201–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pratto, F, John, OP. Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. J Pers Soc Psychol 1991; 61(3): 380–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labouvie-Vief, G, Diehl, M, Jain, E, Zhang, F. Six-year change in affect optimization and affect complexity across the adult life span: a further examination. Psychol Aging 2007; 22(4): 738–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrican, R, Moscovitch, M, Schimmack, U. Cognitive resources, valence, and memory retrieval of emotional events in older adults. Psychol Aging 2008; 23(3): 585–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M, Knight, M. Goal-directed memory: the role of cognitive control in older adults’ emotional memory. Psychol Aging 2005; 20(4): 554–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, RJ, Robinson, LJ, Malhi, GS, Gallagher, P. The neurocognitive profile of mood disorders–a review of the evidence and methodological issues. Bipolar Disord 2015; 17: 2140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, JT, Berntson, GG, Bechara, A, Tranel, D, Hawkley, LC. Could an aging brain contribute to subjective well-being? The value added by a social neuroscience perspective. In Oxford Series in Social Cognition and Social Neuroscience. Social Neuroscience: Toward Understanding the Underpinnings of the Social Mind (eds Todorov, A, Fiske, ST, Prentice, DA): 249–62. Oxford University Press, 2011.Google Scholar
Fischer, H, Sandblom, J, Gavazzeni, J, Fransson, P, Wright, CI, Bäckman, L. Age-differential patterns of brain activation during perception of angry faces. Neurosci Lett 2005; 386(2): 99104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunning-Dixon, FM, Gur, RC, Perkins, AC, Schroeder, L, Turner, T, Turetsky, BI, et al. Age-related differences in brain activation during emotional face processing. Neurobiolf Aging 2003; 24(2): 285–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakaki, M, Nga, L, Mather, M. Amygdala functional connectivity with medial prefrontal cortex at rest predicts the positivity effect in older adults’ memory. J Cogn Neurosci 2013; 25(8): 1206–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegle, GJ, Thompson, W, Carter, CS, Steinhauer, SR, Thase, ME. Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: related and independent features. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61(2): 198209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surguladze, S, Brammer, MJ, Keedwell, P, Giampietro, V, Young, AW, Travis, MJ, et al. A differential pattern of neural response toward sad versus happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57(3): 201–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS, Young, RC, Meyers, BS. Geriatric depression: age of onset and dementia. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 34(3): 141–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexopoulos, GS, Young, RC, Shindledecker, RD. Brain computed tomography findings in geriatric depression and primary degenerative dementia. Biol Psychiatry 1992; 31(6): 591–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, RJ, Levy, R. Computed tomography in the elderly 3. Affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry 1980; 136(3): 270–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D, Liberati, A, Tetzlaff, J, Altman, DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ 2009; 339: b2535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dudley, R, O'Brien, J, Barnett, N, McGuckin, L, Britton, P. Distinguishing depression from dementia in later life: a pilot study employing the emotional Stroop task. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002; 17(1): 4853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broomfield, NM, Davies, R, MacMahon, K, Ali, F, Cross, SMB. Further evidence of attention bias for negative information in late life depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2007; 22(3): 175–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callahan, BL, Hudon, C. Emotional Stroop performance: further evidence for distinct cognitive patterns in depressed and non-depressed aMCI. Alzheimer's & Dement 2014; 10(suppl 4): P434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, C-M, Fan, Y-T, Lee, S-H, Liu, H-L, Chen, Y-L, Lin, C, et al. Cognitive reserve-mediated neural modulation of emotional control and regulation in people with late-life depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2019; 14(8): 849–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mah, L, Pollock, BG. Emotional processing deficits in late-life depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 18(7): 652–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, H, Dai, B, Rossi, S, Li, J. Electrophysiological evidence for elimination of the positive bias in elderly adults with depressive symptoms. Front Psychiatry 2018; 9: 62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savaskan, E, Müller, SE, Böhringer, A, Schulz, A, Schächinger, H. Antidepressive therapy with escitalopram improves mood, cognitive symptoms, and identity memory for angry faces in elderly depressed patients. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2008; 11(3): 381–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brassen, S, Kalisch, R, Weber-Fahr, W, Braus, DF, Buechel, C. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex processing during emotional evaluation in late-life depression: a longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64(4): 349–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callahan, BL, Simard, M, Mouiha, A, Rousseau, F, Laforce, R, Hudon, C. Impact of depressive symptoms on memory for emotional words in mild cognitive impairment and late-life depression. J Alzheimers Dis 2016; 52(2): 451–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, Hill, J, Raste, Y, Plumb, I. The ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 42(2): 241–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szanto, K, Dombrovski, AY, Sahakian, BJ, Mulsant, BH, Houck, PR, Reynolds, CF 3rd, et al. Social emotion recognition, social functioning, and attempted suicide in late-life depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 20(3): 257–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattis, S. Dementia Rating Scale (DRS): Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, 1988.Google Scholar
Aizenstein, H, Andreescu, C, Edelman, K, Cochran, J, Price, J, Butters, M, et al. fMRI correlates of white matter hyperintensities in late-life depression. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168(10): 1075–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briceño, EM, Rapport, LJ, Kassel, MT, Bieliauskas, LA, Zubieta, J-K, Weisenbach, SL, et al. Age and gender modulate the neural circuitry supporting facial emotion processing in adults with major depressive disorder. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2015; 23(3): 304–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanyukov, PM, Szanto, K, Siegle, GJ, Hallquist, MN, Reynolds, CF III, Aizenstein, HJ, et al. Impulsive traits and unplanned suicide attempts predict exaggerated prefrontal response to angry faces in the elderly. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2015; 23(8): 829–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hariri, AR, Mattay, VS, Tessitore, A, Kolachana, B, et al. Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science 2002; 297(5580): 400–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, RB, Siegle, G, Mohlman, J. Emotional Stroop performance in older adults: effects of habitual worry. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 20(9): 798–805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, SA, Diefenbach, G, Wagener, P, Novy, DM, Kunik, M, Rhoades, HM, et al. Comparison of self-report measures for identifying late-life generalized anxiety in primary care. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2008; 21(4): 223–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohlman, J, Price, RB, Vietri, J. Attentional bias in older adults: effects of generalized anxiety disorder and cognitive behavior therapy. J Anxiety Disord 2013; 27(6): 585591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, RB, Eldreth, DA, Mohlman, J. Deficient prefrontal attentional control in late-life generalized anxiety disorder: an fMRI investigation. Transl Psychiatry 2011; 1: e46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, M, Mennin, DS, Ly, M, Karim, HT, Banihashemi, L, Tudorascu, DL, et al. When worry may be good for you: worry severity and limbic-prefrontal functional connectivity in late-life generalized anxiety disorder. J Affect Disord 2019; 257: 650–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karim, H, Tudorascu, DL, Aizenstein, H, Walker, S, Good, R, Andreescu, C. Emotion reactivity and cerebrovascular burden in late-life GAD: a neuroimaging study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016; 24(11): 1040–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittekind, CE, Muhtz, C, Jelinek, L, Moritz, S. Depression, not PTSD, is associated with attentional biases for emotional visual cues in early traumatized individuals with PTSD. Front Psychol 2015; 5: 1474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittekind, CE, Muhtz, C, Moritz, S, Jelinek, L. Performance in a blocked versus randomized emotional Stroop task in an aged, early traumatized group with and without posttraumatic stress symptoms. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2017; 54: 3543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epp, AM, Dobson, KS, Dozois, DJ, Frewen, PA. A systematic meta-analysis of the Stroop task in depression. Clin Psychol Rev 2012; 32(4): 316–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, CJ, Goodwin, GM, Cowen, PJ. Why do antidepressants take so long to work? A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. Br J Psychiatry 2018; 195(2): 102–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roiser, JP, Elliott, R, Sahakian, BJ. Cognitive mechanisms of treatment in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2012; 37(1): 117–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, PB, Mielke, MM, Appleby, BS, Oh, ES, Geda, YE, Lyketsos, CG. The association of neuropsychiatric symptoms in MCI with incident dementia and Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2013; 21(7): 685–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apostolova, LG, Cummings, JL. Neuropsychiatric manifestations in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review of the literature. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2008; 25(2): 115–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goossen, B, van der Starre, J, van der Heiden, C. A review of neuroimaging studies in generalized anxiety disorder: ‘So where do we stand?’. J Neural Transm 2019; 126(9): 1203–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaakxs, R, Comijs, HC, van der Mast, RC, Schoevers, RA, Beekman, AT, Penninx, BW. Risk factors for depression: differential across age? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2017; 25(9): 966–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penton-Voak, IS, Adams, S, Button, KS, Fluharty, M, Dalili, M, Browning, M, et al. Emotional recognition training modifies neural response to emotional faces but does not improve mood in healthy volunteers with high levels of depressive symptoms. Psychol Med [Epub ahead of print] 17 Feb 2020. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719004124.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses diagram of studies retrieved for the review.

Figure 1

Table 1 Selected demographic characteristics of included studies examining behavioural data of samples with depression

Figure 2

Table 2 Selected demographic characteristics of included studies examining neuroimaging data of samples with depression

Figure 3

Table 3 Selected demographic characteristics of included studies examining anxiety and post-traumatic stress disorder

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.