Skip to main content
  • Original article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) rs53576 polymorphism and emotional processing of social and nonsocial cues: an event-related potential (ERP) study

Abstract

Background

Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene polymorphisms are related to individual differences in emotional processing of social cues. However, whether OXTR polymorphisms affect emotional processing of nonsocial cues remains unclear. The present study investigated the relationship between the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism and emotional processing of social cues and nonsocial cues.

Methods

Event-related potentials were recorded from 88 male participants while images of humans and images of objects were presented as social cues and nonsocial cues, respectively.

Results

First, the results showed that GG carriers of OXTR rs53576 showed more negative N1 (50–200 ms) than AA carriers in response to images of both humans and objects. Second, GG carriers showed more negative N2 (200–320 ms) than AA carriers in response to images of humans but not in response to images of objects. Third, GG carriers showed more negative N2 in response to images of humans than images of objects, whereas AA carriers showed the opposite pattern. Fourth, we observed no difference in late positive potential (600–1000 ms) to images of humans or objects that depended on the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism.

Conclusions

These results suggest that the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism affects emotional processing of not only social cues but also nonsocial cues in the very early stage (reflected in N1); however, the data also suggest that the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism is related specifically to increased emotional processing of social cues in the middle stage (reflected in N2).

Background

Social behaviors refer to “the reciprocal interactions of two or more animals and the resulting modification of the individual's action system” [1]. Oxytocin, a neuroactive hormone produced in the hypothalamus, is closely related to human social behaviors (reviewed in [2, 3]). For example, intranasal administration of oxytocin improves the ability to infer the mental state of others [4] and increases gazing toward the eye region of human faces [5]. Moreover, genetic variations in the gene for the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) are related to individual differences in responses to social cues. In particular, recent studies have found that rs53576, a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in OXTR, is related to such individual differences. Behavioral studies have indicated that homozygous carriers of the G allele (GG carriers) show higher trait empathy [6, 7], prosocial behavior [8], trust behavior [9], and lower social loneliness [10] than those with the A allele (AA/GA carriers). These findings are also supported by physiological results indicating that GG carriers show facilitated brain activity to human faces [11, 12] and increased blood pressure and cortisol levels in response to social rejection [13] than AA/GA carriers. These studies suggested that the G allele of the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism is related to a higher sensitivity to social cues.

Some previous studies have examined whether administration of oxytocin affects responses to nonsocial cues [14, 15]. For instance, administration of oxytocin improves recognition memory for images including human figures (e.g., images of human faces) but not for images not including human figures (e.g., images of houses, art sculptures, and landscapes) [14]. Meanwhile, a recent study reported that administration of oxytocin enhances the social meaning of images of objects [15]. Although the relationship between the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism and the oxytocin level is still unclear (reviewed in [16]), a later study [15] suggests a possibility that genetic variations in OXTR may be related not only to individual differences in the response to social cues but also to differences in the response to nonsocial cues. However, the association between the rs53576 polymorphism and the response to nonsocial cues such as images of objects remains unclear, because most reported studies on this OXTR polymorphism have focused on responses to social cues such as human faces [11, 12] and social situations [6, 8, 9, 13]. Thus, the present study focused on a possible association between the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism and the response to nonsocial cues.

Event-related potential (ERP), the electroencephalogram (EEG) response to specific events such as presentation of emotional stimuli, reflects the time course of information processing in the brain due to its high temporal resolution (reviewed in [17, 18]). Studies of passive image viewing tasks have reported that mainly the following three ERP components are sensitive to emotional content; N1, N2/early posterior negativity (EPN), and late positive potential (LPP) [1921]. N1 is a negative peak observed at around 130 ms, N2/ERN is a negativity observed at around 250 ms, and LPP is a sustained positivity that becomes evident 300 ms after stimulus onset. N2 is usually analyzed when a mastoid electrode reference is used, whereas ERN is usually analyzed when an average electrode reference is used (reviewed in [17]). The N1, N2/ERN, and LPP components are greater (more negative for N1 and N2/EPN; more positive for LPP) in response to unpleasant images than emotionally neutral images [19, 20, 22, 23]. Moreover, some studies suggested that N2 reflects an individual difference in response to social and nonsocial cues [21, 24, 25]. Taken together, the N1, N2/ERN, and LPP components are thought to reflect the time course of emotional processing.

In the present study, we aimed to investigate associations between the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism and the time course of emotional processing of social and nonsocial cues by measuring ERP responses. To do so, we analyzed the N1, N2, and LPP components of ERP responses from 88 young male individuals while images of humans and images of objects were presented as social cues and nonsocial cues, respectively. Given that previous studies showed a higher sensitivity to social cues in GG carriers [613], we hypothesized that GG carriers would show a greater ERP response (more negative N1 and N2; more positive LPP) than GA or AA carriers in response to images of humans. More importantly, if an OXTR polymorphism affects emotional processing of not only social cues, but also nonsocial cues, we would expect to see differences in ERP responses between the OXTR rs53576 genotype groups in response to images of objects.

Methods

Participants

Ninety-two male Japanese undergraduate or graduate students (age range 19–25 years) participated in this study. In the present study, we recruited only male participants because previous studies reported that males show clearer differences in brain structures [10] and emotional traits [11] according to the rs53576 polymorphism than females. Participants reported that they had no psychiatric disorders. Eighty-eight participants were included in the final analysis, because the quality of the EEG was poor for four participants (for specific details, refer to “ERP measurements and analysis” section). After receiving an explanation of the details of the study, participants provided written informed consent prior to participation.

Genotyping

Genomic DNA was extracted from the saliva of participants using a Saliva DNA Isolation Kit (Norgen Biotek Corporation, Thorold, Ontario, Canada). Genotyping for the rs53576 polymorphism was then performed using TaqMan SNP Genotyping Assays (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA). PCR amplification was carried out in a LightCycler Nano real-time PCR system (Roche Diagnostics, Mannheim, Germany). All samples were run twice, and they all provided consistent results. The genotype distribution (10 GG, 46 GA, and 32 AA carriers) was in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (p = 0.280) and was in line with previous studies showing that AA carriers are more common than GG carriers in Asian populations [2629].

Stimuli

A total of 270 images were selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) [30]. The images consisted of three content categories: objects, humans, and animals. The images of objects and images of animals did not include figures of humans, whereas the images of humans included figures of more than one person. The images of animals were presented as fillers to buffer against possible habituation, and thus, the results from these were not included in the analysis. Within each content category, the images were further subdivided into the following three valence categories: neutral, pleasant, and unpleasant. Examples of each category of images include a tissue box (neutral image of objects), flowers (pleasant image of objects), a dirty toilet (unpleasant image of objects), a man with an emotionally neutral face (neutral image of humans), a man with a baby (pleasant image of humans), an injured person (unpleasant image of humans), a fox (neutral image of animals), puppies (pleasant image of animals), and cockroaches (unpleasant image of animals). The category of pleasant images of humans did not include erotic images, because balancing arousal levels between pleasant images of humans and pleasant images of objects was difficult. Specific IAPS picture identification numbers [30] are presented in the Appendix.

Procedures

Participants were seated approximately 80 cm from a screen (20-in. monitor). They were asked to focus on the screen and to look at the images presented. During EEG recording, three blocks of image presentations were shown. In each block, 90 images (10 images for each category) were presented three times for a total of 270 trials. In each trial, a white fixation cross was presented on a black screen for 500 ms, and then an image was presented for 1000 ms. The inter-trial interval was 1250–1750 ms, and the order of the trials was random. The images presented were different among the three blocks.

After EEG recording, each participant filled out a subjective assessment. They once again observed the images presented during the EEG recording and judged the valence and arousal of each image based on a 9-point Likert scale (for valence, “very pleasant” was assigned 9 points, whereas “very unpleasant” was assigned 1 point; for arousal, “very arousing” was assigned 9 points, whereas “very relaxing” was assigned 1 point).

ERP measurements and analysis

The EEG was recorded using a 64-channel Geodesic Sensor Net (Electrical Geodesics, Inc., Eugene, OR, USA) based on the 10/20 system and was amplified by a high-input impedance (200 MΩ) amplifier (Net Amps 200 Amplifier, Electrical Geodesics, Inc.). During recording, EEG signals were recorded at electrode site Cz as a reference with a sampling frequency of 500 Hz. Electrode impedances were maintained below 50 kΩ.

After recording, EEG data were re-referenced offline to the average of the left and right mastoids and band-pass filtered with cutoffs of 0.1 and 30 HzFootnote 1 using EMSE software (Source Signal Imaging Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). The trials of image presentation were averaged for the time window between −200 to 1000 ms for each category of images. Trials including artifacts (eye blinks, muscle artifacts, and body movements) above ±100 μV were rejected. Four participants were excluded from the final analysis because their mean number of trials for each image category was less than 30. The mean number of trials (M) and the standard deviation (SD) of the final samples were the following: neutral images of objects, M = 70.2, SD = 14.2; pleasant images of objects, M = 70.2, SD = 14.9; unpleasant images of objects, M = 71.8, SD = 13.6; neutral images of humans, M = 70.9, SD =14.7; pleasant images of humans, M = 71.3, SD = 14.3; and unpleasant images of humans, M = 71.3, SD = 14.1.

We calculated three ERP components as follows: N1 (average amplitude between 50 and 200 ms), N2 (average amplitude between 200 and 320 ms), and LPP (average amplitude between 600 and 1000 ms). N1, N2, and LPP were all averaged from centro-parietal sites (CP1/2, P1/2, Pz, and POz) based on previous findings that the effect of emotional content on these ERP components is generally maximal in centro-parietal areas (for example, [19, 20] for N1 and LPP and [25, 31] for N2).

Statistical analysis

For the ERP responses (N1, N2, and LPP) and the subjective ratings (valence and arousal ratings), we conducted a generalized linear mixed model with OXTR (GG vs. GA vs. AA), content (object vs. human), and valence (neutral vs. pleasant vs. unpleasant) as fixed factors, and participant (a personal code assigned to each subject) as a random factor. In case of significant main effects or interactions, a post hoc t test was performed with Bonferroni corrections. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS software (version 23, IBM, Chicago, IL, USA), and statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Results

ERP responses

Grand averaged ERP waveforms in the centro-parietal area are shown in Fig. 1. A summary of the results of the generalized linear mixed model for the ERP responses is shown in Table 1, and the mean scores of the ERP responses are shown in Table 2.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Grand averaged event-related potential (ERP) in centro-parietal sites for the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) genotype groups (GG vs. GA vs. AA carriers). LPP late positive potential

Table 1 Results of the generalized linear mixed model for event-related potential (ERP) responses
Table 2 Mean amplitude (μV) of event-related potential (ERP) responses

For N1, we observed a significant main effect of OXTR (Table 1). Post hoc analysis (independent samples t test, critical p value = 0.017 for three comparisons) revealed that GG carriers showed significantly more negative N1 than GA (t(334) = −3.90, p < 0.001) and AA carriers (t(250) = −5.51, p < 0.001) and that GA carriers showed significantly more negative N1 than AA carriers (t(466) = −3.35, p = 0.001) (Fig. 2). We also observed a significant main effect of content for N1 (Table 1), indicating that N1 is significantly more negative in response to images of objects (M = −0.72 μV, standard error (SE) = 0.11) than images of humans (M = −0.26 μV, SE = 0.11).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean amplitude of N1 of event-related potential (ERP) for the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) genotype groups (GG vs. GA vs. AA carriers). Data were collapsed across valence and content of images. Error bars indicate standard error. (a) p < 0.001 (critical p value = 0.017); (b) p = 0.001 (critical p value = 0.017)

For N2, we observed a reliable interaction of OXTR × content (Table 1). As shown in Fig. 3, post hoc analysis between OXTR within each content (independent samples t test, critical p value = 0.017 for three comparisons) revealed that GG carriers showed significantly more negative N2 than AA carriers in response to images of humans (t(41.0) = −2.50, p = 0.016), whereas we found no significant difference in N2 among GG, GA, and AA carriers in response to images of objects (all p > 0.017) (Fig. 3). Post hoc analysis between content within each OXTR (paired samples t test) was also conducted; GG carriers showed significantly more negative N2 in response to images of humans than images of objects (t(29) = 2.15, p = 0.040). GA carriers did not show significant differences in N2 in response to images of objects compared to images of humans (t(137) = −1.40, p = 0.165), and AA carriers showed significantly more negative N2 in response to images of objects than images of humans (t(95) = −2.19, p = 0.031) (Fig. 3). We also observed a main effect of valence (Table 1) for N2. Post hoc analysis (paired samples t test, critical p value = 0.017 for three comparisons) indicated that N2 was significantly more negative in response to neutral images (M = −1.24 μV, SE = 0.20) and unpleasant images (M = −1.41 μV, SE = 0.21) than pleasant images (M = −0.80 μV, SE = 0.19) (neutral vs. pleasant: t(175) = −4.63; unpleasant vs. pleasant: t(175) = 6.15; all p < 0.001).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean amplitude of N2 of event-related potential (ERP) for the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) genotype groups (GG vs. GA vs. AA carriers). Data were collapsed across valence of images. Error bars indicate standard error. (a) p = 0.016 (critical p value = 0.017); (b) p = 0.040 (critical p value = 0.050); (c) p = 0.031 (critical p value = 0.050)

For LPP, we found no main effect of OTXR, and related interactions were not significant (Table 1). We observed a significant main effect of content for LPP (Table 1), indicating that LPP was significantly more positive in response to images of humans (M = 3.46 μV, SE = 0.13) than images of objects (M = 2.51 μV, SE = 0.12). We also observed a significant main effect of valence for LPP (Table 1). Post hoc analysis (paired samples t test, critical p value = 0.017 for three comparisons) revealed that LPP was significantly more positive in response to unpleasant images (M = 3.21 μV, SE = 0.17) than neutral images (M = 2.88 μV, SE = 0.16) and pleasant images (M = 2.86 μV, SE = 0.15) (neutral vs. unpleasant: t(175) = −2.98, p = 0.003; pleasant vs. unpleasant: t(175) = −2.74, p = 0.007).

Subjective ratings

Table 3 summarizes the results of the generalized linear mixed model for the subjective ratings, and Table 4 shows the mean scores of subjective ratings.

Table 3 Results of the generalized linear mixed model for subjective ratings
Table 4 Mean scores of subjective ratings

For valence ratings, we observed a reliable interaction of OXTR × valence (Table 3). Post hoc analysis between OXTR within each valence (independent samples t test, critical p value = 0.017 for three comparisons) was conducted; however, we found no significant differences in valence rating among GG, GA, and AA carriers for neutral, pleasant, or unpleasant images (all p > 0.017). We identified main effects of content and valence, and a reliable interaction of content × valence for valence ratings (Table 3). Post hoc analysis between valence within each content (paired samples t test, critical p value = 0.017 for three comparisons) indicated that participants reported pleasant images to be more pleasant than neutral images and unpleasant images to be more unpleasant than neutral images for both images of objects and images of humans (all p < 0.001).

For arousal ratings, we observed a reliable interaction of OXTR × content (Table 3). Post hoc analysis between OXTR within each content (independent samples t test, critical p value = 0.017 for three comparisons) was conducted; however, we found no significant differences in the arousal rating among GG, GA, and AA carriers for either images of objects or images of humans (all p > 0.017). Post hoc analysis between content within each OXTR (paired samples t test) was also conducted; GG, GA, and AA carriers all reported that images of humans were significantly more arousing than images of objects (all p < 0.001). We also observed a reliable interaction of OXTR × valence for arousal rating (Table 3). Post hoc analysis between OXTR within each valence (independent samples t test, critical p value = 0.017 for three comparisons) was conducted; however, we found no significant differences in arousal rating among GG, GA, and AA carriers for neutral, pleasant, or unpleasant images (all p > 0.017). The main effect of content was also significant for arousal ratings (Table 3), indicating that participants reported images of humans (M = 5.26, SE = 0.07) to be significantly more arousing than images of objects (M = 4.63, SE = 0.67). The main effect of valence was also significant for arousal ratings (Table 3). Post hoc analysis (paired samples t test, critical p value = 0.017 for three comparisons) revealed that participants reported unpleasant images (M = 5.68, SE = 0.07) to be more arousing than pleasant images (M = 4.81, SE = 0.08) and pleasant images to be more arousing than neutral images (M = 4.36, SE = 0.08) (all p < 0.001).

Discussion

The present study investigated whether the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism affects emotional processing of social and nonsocial cues. To do so, we compared the ERP responses (N1, N2, and LPP) evoked by images of humans (social cues) and objects (nonsocial cues) among OXTR rs53576 genotype groups.

Association between the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism and the ERP responses

In the present study, N1 was more negative in GG carriers of OXTR rs53576 than AA carriers, and intermediate in GA carriers, regardless of the response to images of objects or humans. Previous studies have reported that N1 is sensitive to highly emotional stimuli [19, 20]. Although the effect of valence of images on N1 did not reach a significant level in the present study (Table 1), the present result suggests that GG carriers of OXTR rs53576 show enhanced emotional processing compared to GA and AA carriers in the very early stage (50–200 ms) in response to both social and nonsocial cues. Regarding social cues, this result supports previous findings of a higher sensitivity to social cues in GG carries than in AA/GA carriers [613]. In particular, the present result replicated the previous ERP study [12] showing that the effect OXTR rs53576 on emotional processing of social cues occurs from the very early stage (reflected in N1). Moreover, given that Peltola et al. [12] adapted a task to discriminate facial expressions and the present study adapted a passive picture viewing task, we suggest that associations between OXTR rs53576 and early processing of social cues is evident regardless of whether active attention to social cues is required or not.

Regarding nonsocial cues, the present result of N1 provides new evidence that the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism affects emotional processing of nonsocial cues. One previous study [15] found that administration of oxytocin improves the subjective rating of emotional intensity of images of objects including the social context (touch between objects) but not the subjective rating of emotional intensity of images of objects not including social context (no touch between objects). In the present study, although images of objects did not include specific social context such as touching, N1 for images of objects was different depending on the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism. One study reported no difference in the oxytocin level between GG/GA carriers and AA carriers of the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism [32]; however, the relationship between the rs53576 polymorphism and the oxytocin level is still unclear (reviewed in [16]). Thus, interpretation of the mechanism of how the rs53576 polymorphism modulates responses to nonsocial cues remains difficult. Future studies are needed to examine the relationships among the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism, oxytocin levels, and emotional processing of nonsocial cues.

For N2, we found that GG carriers of OXTR rs53576 showed more negative N2 than AA carriers in response to images of humans but not in response to images of objects. Moreover, GG carriers showed a greater N2 in response to images of humans than to images of objects, whereas AA carriers showed a greater N2 in response to images of objects than to images of humans. In the present study, N2 was more negative in response to negative images than to pleasant images, supporting previous results showing that N2 is sensitive to highly emotional stimuli [19, 20, 22]. Thus, we suggest that GG carriers and AA carriers of OXTR rs53576 show opposite patterns regarding emotional processing of social cues and nonsocial cues in the middle stage (200–320 ms); GG carriers may show enhanced emotional processing of social cues compared to nonsocial cues, whereas AA carriers may show enhanced emotional processing of nonsocial cues compared to social cues. Similarly, Proverbio et al. [24, 25] reported that N2 is more negative in response to images portraying persons than images portraying landscapes in women, but not in men, suggesting that this result is caused by a greater interest in social stimuli for women compared with men. From this interpretation, the association between OXTR rs53576 and N2 that is shown in the present study may also be explained by the idea that GG carriers have a greater interest in social cues than AA carriers. This supports the previous findings of a higher sensitivity to social cues in GG carries than AA/GA carriers [613].

LPP did not show any differences related to OXTR rs53576, unlike N1 and N2. Thus, the present results for LPP suggest that OXTR rs53576 does not affect the processing of emotional stimuli in the relatively late stage (600–1000 ms), regardless of the existence of social content in the stimuli. The observation of no association between OXTR rs53576 on late processing of social cues is in line with the previous result by Peltola et al. [12], who reported an association between OXTR rs53576 and ERP responses to human faces in N1, but not in LPP. Taken together, we suggest that the association between OXTR rs53576 and emotional processing may be more evident in the early stage than in the late stage. However, because the present study adapted a passive image viewing task and the previous study adapted a relatively simple cognitive task [12], future studies are needed to examine possible effects of OXTR rs53576 on emotional processing in the late stage during complex cognitive tasks such as the memory task that was adopted in Rimmele et al. [14].

Association between the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism and the subjective ratings

We found no difference in subjective ratings for valence and arousal of images among GG, GA, and AA carriers of the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism, although the results of our ERP response indicated differences in emotional processing of social cues and nonsocial cues among the different carriers of the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism. Although some studies (for example, [33]) suggest that physiological responses are more direct indices of responses than subjective ratings, future studies are needed to examine the association between the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism and subjective rating of nonsocial cues.

Implications for anthropology

The distribution of the OXTR rs53576 genotype is different between Asians and European Americans; more GG carriers than AA carriers are found among European Americans, whereas more AA carriers than GG carriers are found among Asians [26, 27, 34, 35]. The present study also replicated previous results showing that AA carriers are more common than GG carriers in Asian populations (10 GG, 46 GA, and 32 AA carriers). This difference in the distribution of OXTR rs53576 carriers seems to be related to cultural differences in human behavior and emotion. For instance, one study [34] found that the frequency of the A allele of OXTR rs53576 is related to collectivistic cultural values. Another study on the distribution of OXTR rs53576 in Africa, Asia, and South Europe [35] suggested that the A allele of OXTR rs53576 may be related to favoritism toward sons. As one factor affecting the interaction between genes and culture, future studies should investigate the evolutionary route that resulted in the difference in the distribution of OXTR rs53576 among regions.

Limitations and future directions

The present study has some important limitations. First, our sample size was small (n = 88) compared with most previous studies on associations between genotype and behavior or brain activity (for example, N = 94 [12], N = 108 [9], N = 179 [7], N = 228 [11], N = 285 [10]). Furthermore, the number of GG carriers in the present study was small (N = 10). As mentioned above, the distribution of the OXTR rs53576 genotype is different between Asians and European Americans. For this reason, several previous studies combined GA and AA carriers in their analyses [6, 7, 12], whereas other previous studies combined GG and GA carriers [26, 29, 32, 36]. However, other previous studies did not combine genotypes and compared GG, GA, and AA carriers [11, 13, 27, 28]. These different methods of grouping participants make the comparison of the findings between the present study and previous studies somewhat difficult.

Second, we examined only one OXTR SNP—rs53576. Although rs53576 is the most widely investigated SNP regarding the association between OXTR polymorphisms and variations in social behavior, previous studies have shown that other OXTR SNPs, such as rs7632287 [37], rs401015 [38], and rs2254298 [10, 35], also affect human social behavior. For instance, the OXTR rs7632287 polymorphism is related to individual differences in pair-bonding behavior [37]. Thus, future studies need to be conducted with analyses of other OXTR SNPs.

Third, our participants were all male. Thus, we could not investigate the possible interaction among gender, the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism, and emotional processing that has been suggested in previous studies [10, 11]. Future studies adopting the same methods as the present study with the inclusion of female participants are necessary.

Conclusion

The present study investigated an association between the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism and the time course of emotional processing of social and nonsocial cues by measuring the ERP response. From the present results, we suggest that the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism affects emotional processing of not only social cues but also nonsocial cues in the very early stage (before 200 ms); however, we also suggest that the OXTR rs53576 polymorphism is related specifically to increased emotional processing of social cues in the middle stage (200–320 ms).

Notes

  1. We note that 0.1-Hz high-pass filter may not be appropriate to calculate LPP (please refer to [39]).

Abbreviations

EEG:

Electroencephalogram

EPN:

Early posterior negativity

ERP:

Event-related potential

IAPS:

International affective picture system

LPP:

Late positive potential

M:

Mean

OXTR :

Oxytocin receptor gene

SD:

Standard deviation

SE:

Standard error

SNP:

Single nucleotide polymorphism

References

  1. Carpenter CR. Section of anthropology: characteristics of social behavior in non‐human primates. Trans N Y Acad Sci. 1942;4:248–58. doi:10.1111/j.2164-0947.1942.tb00856.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Feldman R. Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Horm Behav. 2012;61:380–91. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Insel TR. The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron. 2010;65:768–79. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A, Berger C, Herpertz SC. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:731–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Dadds MR. Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:3–5. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.026.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Smith KE, Porges EC, Norman GJ, Connelly JJ, Decety J. Oxytocin receptor gene variation predicts empathic concern and autonomic arousal while perceiving harm to others. Soc Neurosci. 2014;9:1–9. doi:10.1080/17470919.2013.863223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Rodrigues SM, Saslow LR, Garcia N, John OP, Keltner D. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation related to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:21437–41. doi:10.1073/pnas.0909579106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Kogan A, Saslow LR, Impett EA, Oveis C, Keltner D, Saturn SR. Thin-slicing study of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and the evaluation and expression of the prosocial disposition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:19189–92. doi:10.1073/pnas.1112658108.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Krueger F, Parasuraman R, Iyengar V, Thornburg M, Weel J, Lin M, et al. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation promotes human trust behavior. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:4. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00004.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Lucht MJ, Barnow S, Sonnenfeld C, Rosenberger A, Grabe HJ, Schroeder W, et al. Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and affect, loneliness and intelligence in normal subjects. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:860–6. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.04.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Tost H, Kolachana B, Hakimi S, Lemaitre H, Verchinski BA, Mattay VS, et al. A common allele in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human hypothalamic-limbic structure and function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:13936–41. doi:10.1073/pnas.1003296107.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Peltola MJ, Yrttiaho S, Puura K, Proverbio AM, Mononen N, Lehtimäki T, et al. Motherhood and oxytocin receptor genetic variation are associated with selective changes in electrocortical responses to infant facial expressions. Emotion. 2014;14:469–77. doi:10.1037/a0035959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. McQuaid RJ, McInnis OA, Matheson K, Anisman H. Distress of ostracism: oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism confers sensitivity to social exclusion. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10:1153–9. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu166.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Rimmele U, Hediger K, Heinrichs M, Klaver P. Oxytocin makes a face in memory familiar. J Neurosci. 2009;29:38–42. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4260-08.2009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Peled-Avron L, Perry A, Shamay-Tsoory SG. The effect of oxytocin on the anthropomorphism of touch. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;66:159–65. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.015.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. A sociability gene? Meta-analysis of oxytocin receptor genotype effects in humans. Psychiatr Genet. 2014;24:45–51. doi:10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283643684.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Hajcak G, Weinberg A, MacNamara A, Foti D. ERPs and the study of emotion. In: Luck SJ, Kappenman ES, editors. The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012. p. 441–74.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Olofsson JK, Nordin S, Sequeira H, Polich J. Affective picture processing: an integrative review of ERP findings. Biol Psychol. 2008;77:247–65. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Foti D, Hajcak G, Dien J. Differentiating neural responses to emotional pictures: evidence from temporal-spatial PCA. Psychophysiology. 2009;46:521–30. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00796.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Weinberg A, Hajcak G. Beyond good and evil: the time-course of neural activity elicited by specific picture content. Emotion. 2010;10:767–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. diFilipo D, Grose-Fifer J. An event-related potential study of social information processing in adolescents. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0154459. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0154459.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Carretié L, Hinojosa JA, Martín‐Loeches M, Mercado F, Tapia M. Automatic attention to emotional stimuli: neural correlates. Hum Brain Mapp. 2004;22:290–9. doi:10.1002/hbm.20037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Schupp HT, Junghöfer M, Weike AI, Hamm AO. Attention and emotion: an ERP analysis of facilitated emotional stimulus processing. Neuroreport. 2003;14:1107–10. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000075416.59944.49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Proverbio AM, Zani A, Adorni R. Neural markers of a greater female responsiveness to social stimuli. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:56. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-9-56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Proverbio AM, Adorni R, Zani A, Trestianu L. Sex differences in the brain response to affective scenes with or without humans. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2374–88. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Kim HS, Sherman DK, Sasaki JY, Xu J, Chu TQ, Ryu C, et al. Culture, distress and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:15717–21. http://dx.doi.org/15717-21. 10.1073/pnas.1010830107.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Kim HS, Sherman DK, Mojaverian T, Sasaki JY, Park J, Suh EM, Taylor SE. Gene–culture interaction oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) and emotion regulation. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2011;2:665–72. doi:10.1177/1948550611405854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Luo S, Ma Y, Liu Y, Li B, Wang C, Shi Z, et al. Interaction between oxytocin receptor polymorphism and interdependent culture values on human empathy. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10:1273–81. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv019.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Ohtsubo Y, Matsunaga M, Komiya A, Tanaka H, Mifune N, Yagi A. Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism and self-punishment after an unintentional transgression. Pers Indiv Differ. 2014;69:182–6. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. In: Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Kayser J, Tenke C, Nordby H, Hammerborg D, Hugdahl K, Erdmann G. Event‐related potential (ERP) asymmetries to emotional stimuli in a visual half‐field paradigm. Psychophysiology. 1997;34:414–26. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02385.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Chang WH, Lee IH, Chen KC, Chi MH, Chiu NT, Yao WJ, et al. Oxytocin receptor gene rs53576 polymorphism modulates oxytocin–dopamine interaction and neuroticism traits—a SPECT study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;47:212–20. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.05.020.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Schwerdtfeger A. Predicting autonomic reactivity to public speaking: don't get fixed on self-report data! Int J Psychophysiol. 2004;52:217–24. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.10.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Luo S, Han S. The association between an oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism and cultural orientations. Culture Brain. 2014;2:89–107. doi:10.1007/s40167-014-0017-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Butovskaya PR, Lazebny OE, Sukhodolskaya EM, Vasiliev VA, Dronova DA, Fedenok JN, Butovskaya ML. Polymorphisms of two loci at the oxytocin receptor gene in populations of Africa, Asia and South Europe. BMC Genet. 2016;17:17. doi:10.1186/s12863-015-0323-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Chen FS, Kumsta R, von Dawans B, Monakhov M, Ebstein RP, Heinrichs M. Common oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism and social support interact to reduce stress in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:19937–42. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113079108.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Walum H, Lichtenstein P, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, et al. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with pair-bonding and social behavior. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:419–26. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Montag C, Sauer C, Reuter M, Kirsch P. An interaction between oxytocin and a genetic variation of the oxytocin receptor modulates amygdala activity toward direct gaze: evidence from a pharmacological imaging genetics study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;263:169–75. doi:10.1007/s00406-013-0452-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Picton TW, Bentin S, Berg P, Donchin E, Hillyard SA, Johnson R, Miller GA, Ritter W, Ruchkin DS, Rugg MD, Taylor MJ. Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:127–52. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3720127.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors sincerely thank Assistant Professor Chihiro Hiramatsu for providing guidance on the OXTR analysis and suggestions.

Funding

This work was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI Grant Numbers JP15F15013.

Authors’ contributions

The experiment design was conceived by DC and SW. The laboratory work was conducted by MN. The data analyses were performed by DC and MN. The manuscript was written by DC. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocols were approved by the ethics committee of Kyushu University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Damee Choi.

Appendix

Appendix

International Affective Picture System (IAPS) picture identification numbers:

Neutral images of objects: 5535, 6150, 6900, 7000, 7002, 7003, 7004, 7006, 7009, 7010, 7012, 7014, 7016, 7017, 7025, 7032, 7034, 7035, 7038, 7041, 7043, 7045, 7055, 7056, 7059, 7077, 7090, 7150, 7950, 9422.

Pleasant images of objects: 5000, 5001, 5010, 5020, 5030, 5040, 5200, 5202, 5450, 5471, 5480, 5800, 5890, 6910, 7042, 7053, 7058, 7061, 7095, 7096, 7100, 7140, 7900, 8162, 8170, 8325, 8501, 8502, 8510, 8531.

Unpleasant images of objects: 6020, 6930, 7013, 7023, 7046, 7054, 7078, 7135, 7137, 9080, 9090, 9110, 9290, 9295, 9300, 9320, 9440, 9480, 9600, 9610, 9611, 9620, 9621, 9622, 9630, 9830, 9904, 9909, 9911, 9912.

Neutral images of humans: 2191, 2215, 2221, 2273, 2384, 2397, 2410, 2442, 2445, 2484, 2485, 2487, 2570, 2575, 2580, 2745.1, 2770, 4520, 8010, 8065, 8121, 8160, 8191, 8250, 8251, 8260, 8320, 8341, 8465, 9210.

Pleasant images of humans: 2080, 2150, 2331, 2791, 4542, 5470, 5626, 5628, 5629, 5836, 8001, 8021, 8031, 8034, 8041, 8130, 8158, 8161, 8163, 8186, 8193, 8200, 8208, 8220, 8340, 8350, 8370, 8400, 8467, 8492.

Unpleasant images of humans: 2694, 2750, 6021, 6190, 6211, 6212, 6213, 6300, 6312, 6370, 652, 6831, 6832, 8320, 8480, 8485, 9002,9007, 9230, 9270, 9404, 9413, 9414, 9420, 9425, 9426, 9427, 9428, 9635, 9810.

Neutral images of animals: 1026, 1030, 1040, 1114, 1121, 1122, 1240, 1302, 1313, 1321, 1333, 1350, 1390, 1505, 1560, 1595, 1616, 1640, 1645, 1661, 1670, 1675, 1726, 1820, 1850, 1908, 1931, 1935, 1945, 1947.

Pleasant images of animals: 1410, 1419, 1440, 1441, 1460, 1463, 1500, 1510, 1540, 1590, 1600, 1603, 1604, 1605, 1610, 1620, 1630, 1640, 1650, 1660, 1710, 1720, 1721, 1722, 1740, 1750, 1810, 1812, 1900, 1920.

Unpleasant images of animals: 1050, 1051, 1052, 1090, 1110, 1113, 1120, 1200, 1202, 1205, 1220, 1270, 1271, 1274, 1300, 1301, 1525, 1930, 1932, 9180, 9181, 9182, 9183, 9184, 9185, 9186, 9187, 9560, 9561, 9571.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Choi, D., Minote, N. & Watanuki, S. Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) rs53576 polymorphism and emotional processing of social and nonsocial cues: an event-related potential (ERP) study. J Physiol Anthropol 36, 12 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40101-016-0125-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40101-016-0125-3

Keywords