Skip to main content

Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample

Abstract

Background

Although there is high co-occurrence between ASD and ADHD, the nature of this co-occurrence remains unclear. Our study aimed to examine the underlying relationship between ASD and ADHD symptoms in a combined sample of children with a primary clinical diagnosis of ASD or ADHD.

Methods

Participants included children and youth (aged 3-20 years) with a clinical diagnosis of ASD (n = 303) or ADHD (n = 319) for a total of 622 participants. Parents of these children completed the social communication questionnaire (SCQ), a measure of autism symptoms, and the strengths and weaknesses of ADHD and normal behavior (SWAN) questionnaire, a measure of ADHD symptoms. A principal component analysis (PCA) was performed on combined SCQ and SWAN items, followed by a profile analysis comparing normalized component scores between diagnostic groups and gender.

Results

PCA revealed a four-component solution (inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, social-communication, and restricted, repetitive, behaviors, and interests (RRBI)), with no overlap between SCQ and SWAN items in the components. Children with ASD had higher component scores in social-communication and RRBI than children with ADHD, while there was no difference in inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive scores between diagnostic groups. Males had higher scores than females in social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity components in each diagnostic group.

Limitations

We did not formally assess children with ASD for ADHD using our research-criteria for ADHD, and vice versa. High rates of co-occurring ADHD in ASD, for example, may have inflated component scores in inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. A disadvantage with using single informant-based reports (i.e., parent-rated questionnaires) is that ASD and ADHD symptoms may be difficult to distinguish by parents, and may be interpreted differently between parents and clinicians.

Conclusions

ASD and ADHD items loaded on separate components in our sample, suggesting that the measurement structure cannot explain the covariation between the two disorders in clinical samples. High levels of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity were seen in both ASD and ADHD in our clinical sample. This supports the need for a dimensional framework that examines neurodevelopmental domains across traditional diagnostic boundaries. Females also had lower component scores across social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity than males, suggesting that there may be gender-specific phenotypes related to the two conditions.

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are both relatively common neurodevelopmental conditions [1, 2]. Despite there being no overlap in criteria between the two disorders as described in the DSM-5 [3], empirical studies have demonstrated a high level of co-occurrence between ASD and ADHD in both clinical and population samples [4]. In population-based samples, 22% (95% confidence interval, 95% CI 17-26%) of people with ASD are clinically diagnosed with ADHD, with even higher rates in clinical samples (34%, 95% CI 29-39%) [5]. In children and adolescents with ADHD, 21% (95% CI 18-24%) reach the diagnostic threshold for ASD [6]. A better understanding of the relationship between ASD and ADHD is required in order to understand the mechanism of the co-occurrence, minimize diagnostic error, and personalize treatment opportunities.

One possible explanation for the co-occurrence between ASD and ADHD is that they share common etiologies [7]. Evidence for shared genetic liability is supported by family and twin studies [8, 9], alongside general population studies [10, 11]. Twin studies have also examined the correlations between specific ASD and ADHD domains, and found a high genetic correlation between restricted repetitive behaviors and interests (RRBI) and domains of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity [9, 12, 13], and between social-communication and hyperactivity/impulsivity [10].

Despite evidence of possible shared etiologies between ASD and ADHD, our understanding of the underlying relationship of combined ASD and ADHD behavior symptoms is limited. Delineating the relationship between core domains of different neurodevelopmental conditions can help us better understand the nature of their co-occurrence. If the core domains underlying ASD and ADHD overlap, this could suggest either that (1) the two disorders share a common latent phenotypic construct, or (2) there is substantial measurement error or reporting bias.

A common latent phenotypic construct is found when item responses on a questionnaire are so highly correlated that they represent a common underlying domain. For example, if certain ASD and ADHD questionnaire items are highly correlated, they may load together in a principal component analysis and reveal a common underlying ASD/ADHD domain. A common ASD/ADHD domain might mean that certain ASD and ADHD traits are actually shared between the two disorders. Reporting bias can be observed when the same informant reports on both disorders, i.e., high scores on one disorder may bias an informant to rate an individual high (or low) on another disorder. A common latent phenotypic construct and measurement error may suggest an artificial co-occurrence between the two disorders. In contrast, if ASD and ADHD are associated with independent latent phenotypic constructs, this could suggest a “true” comorbidity in which measurement structure has no influence on the covariation between the two disorders.

Factor analysis techniques are data reduction techniques that decompose symptoms into their underlying constructs or dimensions. One commonly used factor analysis technique is principal component analysis (PCA). The underlying dimensions that are extracted in PCA are referred to as components [14]. It is well established from factor analysis studies that ADHD is composed of separate inattentive and hyperactivity/impulsivity domains [15, 16]. Likewise, recent factor analysis studies of ASD reveal separate social/communicative and RRBI domains [17, 18].

To our knowledge, only four studies to date have examined the latent constructs of combined ASD and ADHD symptoms in the same sample. One study used a clinical sample of children with ADHD [19], one used a clinical sample of children with ASD [20], and two used samples of children from the general population [12, 21]. Three studies found that ASD and ADHD symptoms mapped onto separate factors/components [12, 20, 21] and only one study [19] supported an overlapping three-factor solution composed of a social factor, an inattentiveness factor, and a third factor in which rigidity symptoms and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms grouped together. Given the paucity of studies on the topic and the heterogeneity in samples and methods, it is still unclear whether ASD and ADHD domains load together or onto separate domains. None of the prior studies was done on a combined sample of ASD and ADHD participants, which would provide a more robust test of the possibility that a shared latent construct underlies the co-occurrence of ASD and ADHD. No study that we are aware of has also looked into the variation in factor/component scores across the two diagnostic groups. Furthermore, gender-specific phenotypes of ASD and ADHD have been proposed [22, 23], and yet only one study that we are aware of has looked at variation in factor/component scores across gender [19].

The aims of our study were (i) to determine the principal components of combined ASD and ADHD symptoms in a clinical sample of children with a primary diagnosis of ASD or ADHD and (ii) to investigate whether there are differences in component scores across diagnosis and gender.

Methods

Participants

Children and youth (aged 3-20 years) with a clinical diagnosis of ASD or ADHD were recruited via the Province of Ontario Neurodevelopmental Disorders (POND) Network, Canada. Participants were included if they had a primary clinical diagnosis of ADHD or ASD, and participants and their caregivers had sufficient English comprehension to complete required testing. Participants in the current study were enrolled between 2012-2017. Ethics approval was received from each participating institution’s ethics review board.

Children with a primary diagnosis of ASD that were enrolled in the study had previously received a clinical diagnosis of autism, Asperger’s disorder/syndrome, or PDD-NOS by either a psychiatrist, clinical psychologist, developmental pediatrician, pediatrician, family physician or pediatric neurologist. Likewise, children with a primary clinical diagnosis of ADHD had previously received the diagnosis by either a psychiatrist, clinical psychologist, developmental pediatrician, pediatrician, family physician, or pediatric neurologist. Upon study enrollment, previous clinical diagnoses of ASD were confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2) [24, 25] and Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) [26]. Previous clinical diagnoses of ADHD were confirmed using the parent interview for child symptoms (PICS) for ADHD [27]. The PICS is a semi-structured interview developed particularly for the diagnosis of disruptive behavior disorders, including ADHD. All diagnoses were made based on criteria in the DSM-IV-TR [28] or DSM-5 [3] depending on the time of diagnosis.

A total of 439 ASD participants and 425 ADHD participants had both the ASD symptom (SCQ) and the ADHD symptom (SWAN) measures completed by their caregivers. Those with any missing data on either the SWAN or SCQ were excluded (ASD: n = 101; ADHD: n = 106). We also excluded non-verbal children (n = 35) as these children by necessity had questions 2-7 incomplete on the SCQ. This resulted in a complete dataset of n = 303 in the ASD group and n = 319 in the ADHD group.

The number of children who were listed as having a co-occurring prior clinical ASD or ADHD diagnosis by their caregivers was also identified. Fifty-five children with a primary diagnosis of ASD were listed as having a co-occurring clinical ADHD diagnosis and 4 children with a primary diagnosis of ADHD were listed as having a co-occurring clinical ASD diagnosis. In cases of such co-occurring diagnoses, the diagnosis the participant had received first is referred to as the “primary diagnosis.” In the PCA analysis and subsequent profile analysis children remained grouped solely by their primary diagnosis.

Gender information was obtained from caregivers upon study enrollment. The two options provided on the patient enrollment form under “gender” were “male” and “female.” This information was used for subsequent profile analysis.

Measures

ASD symptoms were assessed using the social communication questionnaire (SCQ) [29], a 40-item questionnaire that asks parents or caregivers to indicate the presence or absence of certain behaviors to help screen for autism. A score of 1 indicates the presence of an atypical behavior and a score of 0 indicates the atypical behavior is absent. The scale has good reliability and validity [30]. A score of 15 and above suggests the need for a formal assessment for ASD [30]. We used this score cutoff to define “caseness” in our descriptive data. Since item 1 is a language screening question it is not included in calculating the total of autistic symptoms.

ADHD symptoms were assessed using the strengths and weaknesses of ADHD symptoms and normal behavior rating scale (SWAN) [31], an 18-item questionnaire based on DSM-IV ADHD criteria. The SWAN has good reliability and validity [32, 33]. Caregivers, or other informants, rate each item on a seven-point scale with average behavior in the middle and positive and negative extremes of the behavior scored on either end. If a participant has six or more items on the two most negative extremes of behavior (“far below” or “below”) for the inattentive or the hyperactive/impulsive domain, they are determined to score within the ADHD clinical range, which we refer to as ADHD “caseness.” SWAN items were re-coded on a seven-point Likert scale from 0 to 6. For consistency in directionality with the SCQ, all items were reverse coded so that negative extremes of behavior received the highest score.

IQ was assessed with age-appropriate Weschler or Stanford-Binet scales. When more than one IQ score was available for a participant, the most recent IQ assessment was used. Adaptive functioning was assessed with the adaptive behavior assessment system (ABAS) first or second edition [34, 35]. The ABAS manual documents good validity and reliability [34]. Composite scores were determined for the conceptual, social and practical domains.

Analysis

Agreement between questionnaire caseness and clinical diagnosis

The level of agreement beyond chance between ASD or ADHD caseness and having a clinical diagnosis of ASD or ADHD as noted by the parent or caregiver was calculated for children with a primary diagnosis of ADHD and ASD, respectively, using Cohen’s kappa.

Preliminary analysis

To remove redundant items, kappa was calculated between pairs of SCQ items (SCQ2-SCQ40) and between pairs of SWAN items (SWAN1-SWAN18). A kappa above 0.80 is considered “strong agreement” [36] and this value was used to remove redundant items. Kappa scores between SCQ items 24 and 25 indicated “strong agreement” (> 0.80), so item 25 was removed from the analysis. No items on the SWAN had such high kappa scores.

The correlation between SCQ and SWAN variables was examined using a correlation matrix. Tetrachoric and Pearson correlations were used to examine correlations between dichotomous (SCQ) and continuous (SWAN) variables, respectively. Biserial correlations were used to examine correlations between SCQ and SWAN items.

The following sets of items had very high correlations (> 0.80): SCQ 3 and 7; SCQ 28 and 30; SCQ 35 and 39; and SWAN 10 and 11. In all cases, the second question in the pair was excluded from further analysis to reduce multicollinearity (SCQ7, SCQ30, SCQ39, and SWAN11). SCQ9 and SCQ23 were also removed from the combined SCQ-SWAN analysis as they had a low Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) (< 0.85) [37]. The KMO helps determine whether there is a strong correlation between variables, and therefore helps determine whether they are suitable for PCA. Low KMO values were not found for any SWAN items.

Principal components analysis

We chose to use a PCA instead of an exploratory factor analysis as it is a simpler data reduction technique to use as a first step in understanding underlying ASD and ADHD dimensions. All principal component analyses were performed using R v3.5.1. Component scores were generated in R.

We performed a PCA on SCQ items combined with SWAN items in the combined clinical sample of children with ASD and ADHD. The KMO = 0.92 was “superb” [37], indicating sampling size and data adequacy. All KMO values for individual items were > 0.86. Barlett’s test of sphericity was significant, χ (1225) = 25,880, p < 0.01, indicating that the data were suitable to be used in a PCA.

For all PCAs, an oblique rotation (oblimin) was used to permit components to correlate with one another. To select the most appropriate principal components solution for each analysis, we considered the following criteria: (1) eigenvalues, (2) scree plots, (3) percentage of variance explained, (4) minimum number of item cross-loadings, and (5) clinical interpretability of the components.

After a component solution was derived, correlations between components were examined. We defined correlation coefficients of 0.10-0.29 as small, 0.30-0.49 as medium and ≥ 0.50 as large [38].

Comparison of ASD and ADHD population with sample constructs

Before we could use the component scores of our combined ASD-ADHD sample PCA in a profile analysis, we needed to be confident that the components of our ASD sample and ADHD sample were similar. A PCA of the SCQ and SWAN items was performed separately in the ASD and ADHD samples. The PCAs of the ASD and ADHD samples were then compared on the number of components, the composition of the components, variance explained, and correlations between the components.

When ASD and ADHD PCAs were performed separately they revealed similar components (Supplemental Tables 3 and 4). There was no overlap between SCQ and SWAN items in the ASD group except for the loading of SCQ16, SCQ2, and SCQ17 in the “hyperactivity/impulsivity” component. There was no overlap between SCQ and SWAN items in the ADHD group except for the loading of SCQ4 in the “hyperactivity/impulsivity” component.

Component scores

Component scores for each participant in the combined ASD-ADHD analysis were generated using tenBerge method in the psych library to preserve the oblique solution. Component scores were normalized to vary between 0 and 1 across groups using observed min/max to permit comparisons between groups and components in the profile analysis.

Profile analysis with component scores

Profile analysis was conducted in SPSS version 22 [39] using the normalized (0-1) component scores generated in the PCA. Profile analysis aims to test whether groups have different profiles on certain measures and is considered to be a special application of the multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) [40]. We explored the profiles for the symptom components between clinical diagnoses (ASD, ADHD) and between genders (male, female). We considered whether the overall levels were equal, i.e., whether across the groups (diagnosis, gender) one diagnosis or gender had a higher total score than another diagnosis or gender when scores on all four symptom components were combined. We also considered whether the lines were parallel, i.e., whether across the groups (diagnosis, gender) the different diagnoses and genders were similar in profile of the four symptom components but possibly different in the level of each symptom component. We then assessed the flatness of the four symptom components, i.e., whether there was similarity in the amount of each of the four symptom components within the different groups (diagnosis, gender). The four-component scores were the within-participant contrast and both diagnosis (ASD, ADHD) and gender (male, female) were between-participants contrasts. Where profile analysis identified significant differences, individual group-wise (diagnosis, gender, and the interaction of diagnosis and gender) ANOVAs with Bonferroni correction for multiple testing (α = 0.05/4 = 0.0125) were used to determine specifically which groups were different for each symptom component.

Results

Missing vs. complete datasets

ASD participants from the complete dataset did not differ from those with missing data in terms of age, gender, SCQ score, SWAN inattention score, and SWAN hyperactivity/impulsivity score (Supplemental Table 1). There was a significant difference on IQ and ABAS scores with those with complete data having significantly higher IQ (p = 0.02) and ABAS composite scores for all domains (p < 0.01).

ADHD participants from the complete dataset did not differ from those with missing data in terms of gender, SCQ score, SWAN inattention score, SWAN hyperactivity/impulsivity score, IQ, and ABAS scores (Supplemental Table 2). ADHD children with complete data were significantly older than ADHD children with missing data (p = 0.03).

ASD and ADHD samples

Descriptive data of the complete study sample is shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Descriptive data of study sample

Agreement between questionnaire caseness and clinical diagnosis

While 53.14% of children with a primary diagnosis of ASD met the threshold for ADHD caseness on the SWAN, only 18.97% were listed as having a known secondary ADHD diagnosis. While 13.48% of children with a primary diagnosis of ADHD met the threshold for ASD caseness on the SCQ, only 1.36% were listed as having a known secondary ASD diagnosis. The level of agreement between ADHD caseness and having a co-occurring ADHD diagnosis by caregiver report was “slight” [36] (κ = 0.12) for children with a primary diagnosis of ASD. The level of agreement between ASD caseness and having a co-occurring ASD diagnosis was also “slight” [36] (κ = 0.07) for children with a primary diagnosis of ADHD. Most cases of ADHD or ASD identified by questionnaires were not reflected in co-occurring clinical diagnoses.

Combined ASD-ADHD sample principal component analysis

In the combined sample of clinically diagnosed ASD and ADHD participants, a PCA was conducted on the 34 SCQ and 17 SWAN items with oblique rotation on the 622 children with a primary clinical diagnosis of ASD or ADHD. The scree plot showed an inflection occurring after 4 points, which explained 55% of the variance. All 4 components had an eigenvalue greater than 1. A four-component solution was chosen based on the scree plot, eigenvalues, proportion of explained variance, and clinical interpretability of the components.

Item loadings are shown in Table 2. The first component consisted mainly of social and communication items from the SCQ and was labeled the “social-communication” component. The second component consisted mainly of RRBI items from the SCQ and was labeled the “RRBI” component. The third component consisted of inattentive items from the SWAN and was labeled the “inattentive” component. Finally, the fourth component consisted of hyperactivity/impulsivity items from the SWAN and was labeled the “hyperactivity/impulsivity” component. There was no overlap between SCQ and SWAN items in any of the components.

Table 2 Pattern matrix of PCA loadings of SCQ and SWAN items in combined sample

Component correlations

The “social-communication” component had a moderate correlation with the “RRBI” component (0.41), and low correlations with the “inattentive” and “hyperactivity/impulsivity” components, (0.11 and 0.10, respectively). The “RRBI” component had a low correlation with the “hyperactivity-impulsivity” component (0.17) and minimal correlation with the “inattentive” component (< 0.01). The “hyperactivity-impulsivity” component showed a moderate correlation with the “inattentive” component (0.34).

Component scores

The sum of the normalized component scores for each domain for each participant is shown in Fig. 1. Each child could have a score on the component domain that ranged from 0 to 1.0 or a total score ranging from 0 to 4.0. Figure 1 shows that the component scores generally line up with the clinical diagnosis. However, there seems to be a sub-group of ADHD children with high social-communication component scores. A total of 11.91% of ADHD children had normalized social-communication component scores > 0.5 in this domain. In comparison, 42.24% of ASD children had a normalized component score of > 0.5 in this domain.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Normalized component scores, stacked by individual. Normalized component scores are summed for each individual in this stacked graph. Each child can have a score between 0 and 1 for each of the four components. Data are sorted from largest to smallest based on the sum of the social-communication and RRBI symptom domain component scores within each diagnosis

Profile analysis

Overall, mean levels were not equal by diagnosis (F (1620) = 174.26, p < 0.001) or by gender (F (1620) = 613.63, p < 0.001), and there was no interaction between diagnosis and gender (F (1618) = 0.91, p = 0.34) with regard to overall levels of all four combined components. The tests of levels indicate that across all four components, there was a higher overall level of total scores in children in ASD than ADHD and in males than females. The test of parallel lines was significant for symptom components by diagnosis (F (3618) = 139.93, p < 0.001) and for symptom components by gender (F (3616) = 2.64, p = 0.05), but not for the interaction between symptom components by diagnosis by gender (F (3616) = 0.07, p = 0.97). The test of parallel lines indicates that the groups are not parallel by diagnosis, meaning that ADHD is not simply a milder version of ASD with lower levels of all symptom domains. Similarly, genders are not parallel, indicating that females are not scoring equally lower in all domains compared to males. The test of flatness was significant for diagnosis (F (31854) = 88.72, p < 0.001), but not for gender (F (31854) = 2.06, p = 0.10) and there was no interaction between diagnosis and gender (F (31854) = 0.06, p = 0.98). The test of flatness indicates that the diagnostic groups have significantly different amounts of one or more symptom components than that of other symptom components. The non-significant effect of gender and the interaction term indicates that it is the diagnosis, more so than gender, that drives the differences in the individual levels of the four symptom components.

A profile plot comparing component scores between diagnostic groups and genders across the components is shown in Fig. 2. Subsequent ANOVAs revealed that, while there was a significant difference in social-communication (F (1618) = 75.48, p < 0.01) and RRBI (F (1618) = 258.96, p < 0.01) component scores between the two diagnostic groups, there was no significant difference in inattentive (F (1618) = 5.67, p = 0.02) or hyperactive/impulsive (F (1618) = 0.31, p = 0.58) component scores between diagnostic groups. Males had higher social-communication (F (1618) = 9.10, p < 0.01), hyperactivity/impulsivity (F (1618) = 9.00, p < 0.01), and RRBI (F (1618) = 10.60, p < 0.01) component scores than females within each diagnostic group. There was no significant difference between males and females in inattentive (F (1618) = 0.93, p = 0.34) component scores.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Profile plot with diagnosis by gender. There was no difference in inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive component scores between the two diagnostic groups (p < 0.01). Males had higher social-communication, hyperactivity/impulsivity, and RRBI component scores regardless of diagnosis (p < 0.01). Two asterisks (**) indicate a diagnosis effect. A degree sign (°) indicates a gender effect

Discussion

High rates of co-occurrence between ASD and ADHD have been reported [5, 6]. To date, few studies have examined the underlying relationship between the core domains of combined ASD and ADHD symptoms. Our PCA revealed a four-component solution with no overlap between SCQ and SWAN items in the components. Children with ASD had higher scores in social-communication and RRBI components than children with ADHD, while there was no difference in inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive component scores between diagnostic groups. Males had higher component scores than females in social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity within each diagnostic category.

In our sample, while 53.14% of children with a primary diagnosis of ASD met the threshold for ADHD caseness on the SWAN, only 18.97% were listed as having a known co-occurring ADHD diagnosis. While 13.48% of children with a primary diagnosis of ADHD met the threshold for ASD caseness on the SCQ, only 1.36% were listed as having a known co-occurring ASD diagnosis. This suggests that co-occurring ASD in children with ADHD (and vice versa) may be under-identified and highlights the importance of assessing for co-occurring symptomatology in this population. Since the SCQ and SWAN were filled out by caregivers it is also important to consider that rates of ASD and ADHD caseness on the SCQ and SWAN, respectively, may have been overrepresented in our sample.

Even though ASD has an earlier age of onset than ADHD, studies have shown that many children with ASD are first diagnosed with having ADHD, subsequently leading to a delay in a diagnosis of ASD [41, 42]. This can have important consequences for access to services. Clinically, ADHD symptoms can overshadow those of ASD or clinicians may “expect” to see a more common disorder such as ADHD and might not carry out a careful assessment of other diagnoses. While National Institute for Health Care and Excellence ADHD guidelines caution that there are high levels of ADHD in children with ASD [43], the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry ADHD practice parameter guidelines do not mention ASD [44] in the differential diagnosis. Based on our findings, co-occurring ASD in children with ADHD may be under-identified and we suggest that the importance of assessing for co-occurring ASD symptomatology in the ADHD population be considered in future guideline revisions.

When conducting a PCA on combined ASD and ADHD symptom measures in a sample of children with a primary diagnosis of ASD or ADHD, we found no overlap between ASD and ADHD items comprising the components. SCQ items loaded onto two ASD components (social-communication and RRBI) and SWAN items loaded onto two ADHD components (inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity). Therefore, the explanation for the co-occurrence between the two disorders cannot simply be due to an overlap in their core domains. Also, there were low correlations between domains, so it is not a matter of scoring high on all components, which might reflect reporting bias. This suggests that the measurement structure cannot explain the covariation between the two disorders in clinical samples. We cannot assess “true comorbidity” based on the present findings since parent report bias has not been addressed in this study.

Our findings are in contrast to the study by Martin et al. [19] which found overlap between hyperactivity/impulsivity and RRBI domains in a sample of children with ADHD. Although we used PCA instead of exploratory factor analysis, both our study and the Martin et al. [19] study used an oblique rotation (allowing components to correlate) and the SCQ as a measure of ASD symptoms. Interestingly, in their sample of ADHD children, Martin et al. [19] reported finding another factor solution in which ASD and ADHD did not overlap, and consisted of three ASD factors and two ADHD factors. Such a finding is in line with the results of our study, which also found that ASD and ADHD items loaded independently onto separate components. Of note, Martin et al. [19] chose the competing three-factor solution composed of a social (ASD) factor, an inattentive (ADHD) factor, and a factor in which RRBI (ASD) and hyperactivity/impulsivity (ADHD) traits grouped together because it had a lower range of factor inter-correlations and greater parsimony. It is also important to consider that our sample had a relatively higher IQ score than Martin et al.’s. Given that IQ is negatively correlated with ASD and ADHD symptoms [45, 46], it is unclear how much IQ may have explained the difference in findings between the two studies.

Interestingly, our PCA of ASD children (Supplemental Table 3) revealed cross-loading of a few ASD items onto the ADHD hyperactivity/impulsivity component, and our PCA of ADHD children (Supplemental Table 4) revealed cross-loading of one ASD item onto the ADHD hyperactivity/impulsivity component. Since these items suggest potential misattribution of ASD traits for those of ADHD, and vice versa, we re-ran the PCA without these items, as a sensitivity analysis, to determine whether it resulted in a model with more variance explained. This PCA produced a similar model, with four components with similar variance explained (58 % vs. 55% in the original model).

In our study, 11.91% of ADHD children had normalized component scores > 0.5 in the social-communication domain. This agrees with latent class analysis studies that have shown considerable heterogeneity when looking at the intersection of ASD and ADHD symptoms across subgroups [47,48,49]. Given the heterogeneity seen in ADHD, it is possible that there is a subgroup of ADHD children with significant and impairing problems in social-communication without accompanying RRBI, similar to the DSM-5 social (pragmatic) communication disorder or the so-called broader autism phenotype [50, 51] seen in first degree relatives of probands with ASD. Further study of this group might prove revealing in terms of biomarker and genetic variant overlap with ASD.

Using profile analysis, our study showed that there was no difference in inattentive and hyperactivity component scores between ASD and ADHD groups. This suggests a possible mechanism for the increasing literature showing an overlap between ASD and ADHD on genetic and neuroimaging levels [7, 52]. This overlap might be accounted for by similar levels of inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive phenotypes in the two groups instead of variation in social-communication and RRBI.

By finding high levels of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity in ASD and ADHD, our study supports the need for a dimensional framework that examines neurodevelopmental domains (social-communication, RRBI, inattention, and hyperactivity/impulsivity) across traditional ASD and ADHD diagnostic boundaries. This notion is also supported by recent neuroimaging studies in children with ASD and ADHD [53,54,55]. In the POND cohort, Baribeau et al. [55] found that structural neuroimaging correlates of social deficits were similar across ASD and ADHD, and Kushki et al. [54] found that clusters identified among children with ASD, ADHD, and OCD based on cortical thickness and behavioral phenotypic features did not map well onto traditional diagnostic categories. In a separate cohort, Aoki et al. [53] further showed that ASD severity across both children with ASD or ADHD was associated with specific white matter organization indices and that a dimensional approach provided a more comprehensive picture of white matter associations than a categorical approach. Our study adds to the literature by showing that ASD and ADHD groups differ on component scores in social-communication and RRBI domains, suggesting that some domains may be relatively more disorder-specific and may be useful in identifying certain subgroups in ASD and ADHD.

We also found that females had lower component scores across social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity domains. This suggests that there may be gender-related phenotypes associated with the two neurodevelopmental disorders. A “female phenotype” of ASD has been proposed in the literature [22], one aspect being that females generally show less RRBIs than males [56]. However, it has also been argued that “gold standard” diagnostic instruments are potentially male-biased and RRBIs that are more prevalent in females may be harder to capture using these instruments. Females may also show different forms of social-communication difficulties than males, being better able to express themselves socially but having similar levels of social difficulties as males [56, 57]. This suggests that females may tend to have a better ability to “camouflage” or compensate their autism, for example, by imitating social scripts [58, 59]. In terms of ADHD, females tend to have less hyperactive symptoms and externalizing behaviors than males [23], which agrees with our findings that females have lower component scores in the hyperactivity/impulsivity domain than males.

Interestingly, when looking at factor scores across genders in their combined ASD-ADHD symptom analysis, Martin et al. [19] did not find any differences in “social,” “rigidity-hyperactivity,” or “inattentiveness” domains between males and females. Compared to this study, our participants had higher IQ; therefore, it is possible that females in this group were better able to “camouflage” or compensate their ASD characteristics, hence such presentations were less noticeable and therefore scored lower by caregivers. ADHD characteristics in females with higher IQ may also be less noticeable due to compensation of the child or recognition biases of caregivers, especially if there are fewer concerns about their school performance. These influences may have widened the difference in social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity component scores between males and females in our sample.

The strengths of our study include the large sample size of participants with ASD (n = 303) and ADHD (n = 319). The primary clinical diagnoses of all the study participants were also confirmed with valid and reliable measures: the PICS for ADHD [27] and the ADOS-2 [24] and ADI-R [26] for ASD. Our study also used a combined sample of children with a primary diagnosis of ASD or ADHD which provides a larger sample size and a more robust test of the underlying relationship between the core domains of ASD and ADHD symptoms than a sample of children with ASD or ADHD alone.

Limitations

Our study had several limitations. First, we did not perform the ASD diagnostic interviews on the clinically referred ADHD participants, and we did not perform an ADHD diagnostic interview on the clinically referred ASD participants. It is therefore unclear which ASD and ADHD participants had a co-occurring ADHD or ASD diagnosis, respectively, defined using the same research criteria. Such information would be helpful as an increased number of ASD participants with co-occurring ADHD might increase levels of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity and an increased number of ADHD participants with co-occurring ASD might increase levels of social-communication and RRBI symptoms. Future studies should consider comparing ASD, ADHD, and co-occurring ASD + ADHD groups separately when examining neurodevelopmental domain scores. Although our study did collect information from caregivers about co-occurring diagnoses, including ASD and ADHD, it is unclear if this information is sufficiently valid, and many participants may not have been assessed for co-occurring diagnoses or may have been misdiagnosed. It is important for future studies to formally assess for co-occurring diagnoses using validated instruments across all participants.

Second, we used parent-rated questionnaires. A disadvantage with using single informant-based reports is that some ASD and ADHD symptoms may be difficult to distinguish by parents, and may be interpreted differently between parents and clinicians [60].

Third, we excluded participants with missing data on the rating scales (which included non-verbal or minimally verbal participants). Children with incomplete rating scales had significantly lower IQ and ABAS scores, which likely resulted in a biased sample of participants for our PCA.

Finally, we combined two different rating scales with continuous (SWAN) and dichotomous (SCQ) variables in the PCA. It is therefore unclear to what extent our findings might be influenced by items from the same questionnaire grouping together as a result of being in the same measurement scale. It would be interesting to conduct a PCA on ASD and ADHD items from the same questionnaire, such as the child behavior checklist (CBCL); however, such questionnaires are typically based on broad DSM-based criteria and as such do not adequately capture ASD symptomatology.

Conclusions

ASD and ADHD items loaded on separate components in our sample, suggesting that the measurement structure cannot explain the covariation between the two disorders in clinical samples. Our results also show that high levels of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity are seen in both ASD and ADHD. This supports the need for a dimensional framework that examines neurodevelopmental domains across traditional diagnostic boundaries. Clinically, this emphasizes the importance of careful screening for social communication difficulties and RRBIs in children presenting with high levels of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. Moreover, females had lower component scores across social-communication, RRBI, and hyperactivity/impulsivity than males, suggesting that there may be gender-related phenotypes in the two neurodevelopmental conditions. An increasing amount of literature suggests that females show fewer observable difficulties than males, secondary to their tendency to “camouflage” or compensate for difficulties. This highlights the need for careful screening of ASD symptomatology in females, especially those with ADHD.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are publicly available through Brain-CODE. https://www.braincode.ca/

References

  1. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S. Autism. Lancet. 2014;383(9920):896–910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Polanczyk GV, Willcutt EG, Salum GA, Kieling C, Rohde LA. ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(2):434–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  4. Rommelse N, Franke B, Geurts HM, Hartman CA, Buitelaar JK. Shared heritability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19:281–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Lai MC, Kassee C, Besney R, Bonato S, Hull L, Mandy W, et al. Prevalence of co-occurring mental health diagnoses in the autism population: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(10):819–29.

  6. Hollingdale J, Woodhouse E, Young S, Fridman A, Mandy W. Autistic spectrum disorder symptoms in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytical review. Psychol Med. 2019:1–14.

  7. Rommelse N, Geurts HF, Buitelaar J, Hartman C. A review on cognitive and brain endophenotypes that may be common in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and facilitate the search for pleiotropic genes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(6):1363–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ghirardi L, Brikell I, Freitag C, Franke B, Asherson P, Lichtenstein P, et al. The familial co-aggregation of ASD and ADHD- a register-based cohort study. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23:257–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Ghirardi L, Pettersson E, Taylor MJ, Freitag CM, Franke B, Asherson P, et al. Genetic and environmental contribution to the overlap between ADHD and ASD trait dimensions in young adults: a twin study. Psychol Med. 2018:1–9.

  10. Stergiakouli E, Davey Smith G, Martin J, Skuse DH, Viechtbauer W, Ring SM, et al. Shared genetic influences between dimensional ASD and ADHD symptoms during child and adolescent development. Mol Autism. 2017;8(1).

  11. Miller M, Musser ED, Young GS, Olson B, Steiner RD, Nigg JT. Sibling recurrence risk and cross-aggregation of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(2):147–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ronald A, Larsson H, Anckarsater H, Lichtenstein P. Symptoms of autism and ADHD: a Swedish twin study examining their overlap. J Abnorm Psychol. 2014;123(2):440–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Polderman T, Hoekstra R, Posthuma D, Larsson H. The co-occurrence of autistic and ADHD dimensions in adults: an etiological study in 17 770 twins. Translat Psychiatry. 2014;4:e435.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Field A, Miles J, Field Z. Discovering statistics using R. London: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Willcutt EG, Nigg JT, Pennington BF, Solanto MV, Rohde LA, Tannock R, et al. Validity of DSM-IV attention–deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. J Abnormal Psychol. 2012;121(4):991–1010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Bauermeister JJ, Canino G, Polanczyk G, Rohde LA. ADHD across cultures: is there evidence for a bidimensional organization of symptoms? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2010;39(3):362–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Mandy WPL, Skuse DH. Research review: what is the association between the social-communication element of autism and repetitive interests, behaviours and activities? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(8):795–808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Shuster J, Perry A, Bebko J, Toplak ME. Review of factor analytic studies examining symptoms of autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:90–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Martin J, Hamshere ML, O’Donovan MC, Rutter M, Thapar A. Factor structure of autistic traits in children with ADHD. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:204–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ghanizadeh A. Co-morbidity and factor analysis on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder DSM-IV-derived items. J Res Med Sci. 2012;17(4):368–72.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Ghanizadeh A. Factor analysis on ADHD and autism spectrum disorder DSM-IV-derived items shows lack of overlap. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19:797–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Auyeung B, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S. Sex/gender differences and autism: setting the scene for future research. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54(1):11–24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Gaub M, Carlson CL. Gender differences in ADHD: a meta-analysis and critical review. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(8):1036–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule: ADOS-2. Western Psychol Serv Torrance. 2012.

  25. Hus V, Lord C. The autism diagnostic observation schedule, module 4: revised algorithm and standardized severity scores. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(8):1996–2012.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Lord C, Rutter M, Couteur AL. Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24(5):659–85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Ickowicz A, Schachar RJ, Sugarman R, Chen SX, Millette C, Cook L. The parent interview for child symptoms- a situation specific clinical research interview for attention-deficit hyperactivity and related disorders. Can J Psychiatry. 2006;51(5).

  28. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual - IV - TR. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Rutter M, Bailey A, Lord C. SCQ manual-2; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Berument SK, Rutter M, Lord C, Pickles A, Bailey A. Autism screening questionnaire: diagnostic validity. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;175:444–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Swanson JM, Schuck S, Porter MM, Carlson C, Hartman CA, Sergeant JA, et al. Categorical and dimensional definitions and evaluations of symptoms of ADHD: history of the SNAP and SWAN rating scales. Int J Educ Psychol Assess. 2012;10(1):51–70.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Lakes KD, Swanson JM, Riggs M. The reliability and validity of the English and Spanish strengths and weaknesses of ADHD and normal behavior rating scales in a preschool sample: continuum measures of hyperactivity and inattention. J Atten Disord. 2012;16(6):510–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Arnett AB, Pennington BF, Friend A, Willcutt EG, Byrne B, Samuelsson S, et al. The SWAN captures variance at the negative and positive ends of the ADHD symptom dimension. J Atten Disord. 2013;17(2):152–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Harrison PL, Oakland T. Adaptive behavior assessment system – Second Edition. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Harrison PL, Oakland T. Adaptive behavior assessment system. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33(1):159–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Kaiser. An index of factor simplicity. Psychometrika. 1974;39:31–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Cohen. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nded.). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  39. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. Version 22.0 ed. Armonk: IBM Corp; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Pearson; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Miodovnik A, Harstad E, Sideridis G, Huntington N. Timing of the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics. 2015;136(4):e830–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Kentrou V, Veld DM d, Begeer S. Delayed autism spectrum disorder recognition in children and adolescents previously diagnosed with attention-deficit: hyperactivity disorder. Autism. 2018:1–8.

  43. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis and management (NICE Quality Standard No. 87). 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Pliszka S. Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(7):894–921.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Simonoff E, Pickles A, Wood N, Gringras P, Chadwick O. ADHD symptoms in children with mild intellectual disability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(5):591–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Constantino JN, Przybeck T, Friesen D, Todd RD. Reciprocal social behavior in children with and without pervasive developmental disorders. Dev Behav Pediatr. 2000;21(1):2–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Pourcain BS, Mandy WPL, Heron J, Golding J, Smith GD, Skuse DH. Links between co-occurring social-communication and hyperactive-inattentive trait trajectories. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(9):892–902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. van der Meer JMJ, Oerlemans AM, Steijn DJv, Lappenschaar MGA, Sonneville LMJd, Buitelaar JK, et al. Are autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder different manifestations of one overarching disorder? Cognitive and symptom evidence from a clinical and population-based sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(11):1160–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. van der Meer JMJ, Lappenschaar MGA, Hartman CA, Greven CU, Buitelaar JK, Rommelse NNJ. Homogeneous combinations of ASD-ADHD traits and their cognitive and behavioral correlates in a population-based sample. J Atten Disord. 2017;21(9):753–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Piven J, Palmer P, Jacobi D, Arndt DCS. Broader autism phenotype: evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:185–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Rubenstein E, Chawla D. Broader autism phenotype in parents of children with autism: a systematic review of percentage estimates. J Child Fam Stud. 2018;27(6):1705–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Rommelse N, Buitelaar JK, Hartman CA. Structural brain imaging correlates of ASD and ADHD across the lifespan: a hypothesis-generating review on developmental ASD-ADHD subtypes. J Neural Transm. 2016.

  53. Aoki Y, Yoncheva YN, Chen B, Nath T, Sharp D, Lazar M, et al. Association of white matter structure with autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(11):1120–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Kushki A, Anagnostou E, Hammill C, Duez P, Brian J, Iaboni A, et al. Examining overlap and homogeneity in ASD, ADHD, and OCD: a data-driven, diagnosis-agnostic approach. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):318.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Baribeau DA, Dupuis A, Paton TA, Hammill C, Scherer SW, Schachar RJ, et al. Structural neuroimaging correlates of social deficits are similar in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: analysis from the POND network. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Lai MC, Szatmari P. Sex and gender impacts on the behavioural presentation and recognition of autism. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2020;33(2):117–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Hiller RM, Young RL, Weber N. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder based on DSM-5 criteria: evidence from clinician and teacher reporting. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42(8):1381–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Ruigrok AN, Chakrabarti B, Auyeung B, Szatmari P, et al. Quantifying and exploring camouflaging in men and women with autism. Autism. 2017;21(6):690–702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Livingston LA, Shah P, Happé F. Compensatory strategies below the behavioural surface in autism: a qualitative study. Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(9):766–77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Grzadzinski R, Dick C, Lord C, Bishop S. Parent-reported and clinician-observed autism spectrum disorder (ASD) symptoms in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): implications for practice under DSM-5. Mol Autism. 2016;7(7).

Download references

Acknowledgments

Presentation information: This study was presented as a part of a symposium at the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry’s 65th annual meeting, Seattle, Washington, October 22nd-27th, 2018.

Funding

This research was conducted with the support of the Ontario Brain Institute (POND – PI: Anagnostou), an independent non-profit corporation, funded partially by the Ontario government. The opinions, results, and conclusions are those of the authors and no endorsement by the Ontario Brain Institute is intended or should be inferred.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

PS and AK conceived of the study. AK contributed to the analyses, synthesized and interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript. KTC performed the statistical analyses, aided in interpretation, and contributed to writing and editing of the manuscript. EA, RS, JC, and SG helped with recruitment of participants, interpretations of results, and contributed to editing of the manuscript. MCL and ED aided in the interpretation of the results and contributed to editing of the manuscript. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aneta D. Krakowski.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Research ethics board approval was obtained at the following POND institutions: Holland Bloorview Research Ethics Board (#11-280); Queen’s University Health Sciences & Affiliated Teaching Hospitals Research Ethics Board (#6005107); The Hospital for Sick Children Research Ethics Board (#1000000346); Hamilton Integrated Research Ethics Board (#12-050); and Western University’s Health Sciences Ethics Board (#103326).

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

RS is in on the scientific advisory board for highland therapeutics and on the scientific advisory board for ehave (psychological software company). RS also has equity in ehave. EA has received consultation fees from Roche and Takeda, royalties from APPI and Springer, and funding from SynapDx and Sanofi-Aventis. PS has received royalties from Guilford Press. AK, KTC, MCL, JC, ED, and SG have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Comparison of complete versus missing data for ASD study sample. Table S2. Comparison of complete versus missing data for ADHD study sample. Table S3. Pattern matrix of PCA loadings of SCQ and SWAN items in ASD sample. Table S4. Pattern matrix of PCA loadings of SCQ and SWAN items in ADHD sample.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Krakowski, A.D., Cost, K.T., Anagnostou, E. et al. Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample. Molecular Autism 11, 28 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-020-00338-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-020-00338-1

Keywords