Skip to main content

Atypical lateralization of motor circuit functional connectivity in children with autism is associated with motor deficits

Abstract

Background

Atypical lateralization of language-related functions has been repeatedly found in individuals with autism spectrum conditions (ASC). Few studies have, however, investigated deviations from typically occurring asymmetry of other lateralized cognitive and behavioural domains. Motor deficits are among the earliest and most prominent symptoms in individuals with ASC and precede core social and communicative symptoms.

Methods

Here, we investigate whether motor circuit connectivity is (1) atypically lateralized in children with ASC and (2) whether this relates to core autistic symptoms and motor performance. Participants comprised 44 right-handed high-functioning children with autism (36 males, 8 females) and 80 typically developing control children (58 males, 22 females) matched on age, sex and performance IQ. We examined lateralization of functional motor circuit connectivity based on homotopic seeds derived from peak activations during a finger tapping paradigm. Motor performance was assessed using the Physical and Neurological Examination for Subtle Signs (PANESS).

Results

Children with ASC showed rightward lateralization in mean motor circuit connectivity compared to typically developing children, and this was associated with poorer performance on all three PANESS measures.

Conclusions

Our findings reveal that atypical lateralization in ASC is not restricted to language functions but is also present in circuits subserving motor functions and may underlie motor deficits in children with ASC. Future studies should investigate whether this is an age-invariant finding extending to adolescents and adults and whether these asymmetries relate to atypical lateralization in the language domain.

Background

The macroscopic functional organization of the human brain is characterized by lateralized specialization of the two cerebral hemispheres for cognitive and behavioural abilities. The left hemisphere exhibits dominance for language and motor control, whereas the right hemisphere is specialized for visuospatial attention [1, 2]. This division of function has been explained as an evolutionary advantage to avoid the duplication of cognitive processing and cortical representations [3]. In line with this, we previously showed that leftward lateralization of functional motor circuit connectivity is associated with better motor performance in typically developing children [4] and others have shown that the degree of language and visuospatial lateralization relates to enhanced cognitive functioning [5, 6].

Studies converge to show that this typical pattern of hemispheric specialization is not seen in individuals with autism spectrum conditions (ASC). A consistent finding is that language-related structures such as the planum temporale [7] and inferior frontal gyrus [8] are atypically rightward asymmetric in both children and adults with ASC. These findings are corroborated by functional studies reporting atypical lateralization on linguistic tasks in individuals with ASC [915]. Early development of atypical language lateralization might potentially serve as a biomarker and be clinically relevant if it precedes atypical language development in children with ASC [16]. Language is not the only lateralized functional domain in which individuals with ASC exhibit deficits. Clumsiness and impaired fine motor control are among the most prevalent and earliest identifiable symptoms in individuals with ASC [1719]. Imaging studies report atypical activation of the premotor cortex [20] and the cerebellum [2123] during motor execution and decreased connectivity of the motor execution network [22]. However, functional studies of atypical lateralization in ASC have mainly been confined to the language domain. Only a few studies have explored lateralized activation during motor tasks in ASC during imitation [24], sequence [25] or procedural learning [26].

Motor symptoms are of considerable functional and clinical relevance in ASC, given that they are among the earliest detectable behavioural problems, preceding social and language deficits and have been found to be correlated with core social and communicative impairments in ASC [27, 28]. Indeed, the neural contributions to motor skill impairments in ASC may parallel those underlying the broader range of autistic features, leading to impaired development of skills necessary for motor, social and communicative behaviours [29]. Thus, identifying the neurobiological underpinnings of motor signs in ASC is of upmost importance in view of their potential role as biological markers of the condition.

Lateralized functions are subserved by functional coordination among regions in a network rather than by individual brain loci separately. Recent advances in neuroimaging have provided tools to measure the synchronization of cortical regions that share functional properties and thus form complex neural networks. Coherent temporal correlations in slow, spontaneous low-frequency fluctuations in the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal are the basis for functional resting-state connectivity, which may reflect the intrinsic organization of the human brain [3032]. Recent resting-state studies have confirmed the presence of lateralized brain functions and have helped to elucidate differential patterns of connectivity. Left hemisphere functions such as language and fine motor control have been suggested to be more focal preventing conduction delays and enabling rapid processing [33, 34], whereas visuospatial functions require more interhemispheric integration to represent the bilateral visual space [35]. Consistent with this, Gotts et al. [5] showed that functional lateralization differs between the two hemispheres, with left-lateralized hubs including language and motor control networks showing a bias towards intrahemispheric interactions, whereas right-lateralized hubs including those subserving attention tending to operate in a more integrative manner between the two hemispheres.

Mounting evidence suggests that the neural substrates of ASC involve atypical neural connectivity and deficient neural synchronization of multiple functional networks [3638]. Many recent studies in ASC have moved away from task-based fMRI, instead focusing on aberrant functional connectivity of networks subserving cognitive and social functions, such as the default mode network [3942]. To date, only a few studies have explored atypical motor circuit connectivity in ASC in association with motor performance [22, 43, 44] or atypical lateralization of resting-state (motor) networks in ASC [45, 46]. This is despite the accumulated evidence of autism-associated impairments in motor control and learning [18, 28, 4749]. Cardinale et al. [45] reported atypical rightward lateralization of multiple functional brain networks in individuals with ASC, including language, motor and visuospatial circuits, concluding that rightward lateralization constitutes a fundamental characteristic of cerebral organization in ASC.

Here, we extend the current literature on atypical cerebral lateralization in ASC to examine the lateralization of functional motor circuit connectivity. We focus on atypical intrahemispheric functional connectivity, based on studies showing greater within-hemisphere interactions for left-lateralized functions [5] and previous studies applying a similar rationale [4, 46, 50]. We hypothesized that children with ASC would show reduced leftward or increased rightward lateralization of motor connectivity, associated with poorer performance on motor tasks.

Methods

Participant characteristics

Participants comprised 44 right-handed children with ASC (36 males, 8 females) and 80 right-handed typically developing children (58 males, 22 females) between 8 and 12 years of age (ASC mean = 10.23, standard deviation (SD) = 1.51, range = 8.01–12.99; controls mean = 10.15, SD = 1.08, range = 8.07–12.76) (see Additional file 1; for participant characteristics of the final sample, see Table 1). Individuals with ASC were recruited through advertisements placed within community-wide service groups, schools and hospitals, as well as from outpatient clinics at the Kennedy Krieger Institute in Baltimore. Participants with ASC received a clinical diagnosis according to the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–IV [51], and diagnoses were confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R; [52]) and Module 3 of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-G; [53]). Participants were excluded if they met any of the following criteria: (1) a history of seizures and/or traumatic brain injury; (2) a full-scale IQ (FIQ) less than 80 (in cases where the FIQ was less than 80, participants were still included if their verbal comprehension index (VCI) or perceptual reasoning index (PRI) was higher than 80), as assessed by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children- fourth edition (WISC-III/IV; [54, 55]); (3) a developmental language disorder; (4) reading disability; (5) visual impairment and (6) neurologic disorder such as epilepsy. The Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents, fourth edition (DICA-IV; [56]) was used to determine the presence of additional psychiatric diagnoses. Children with ASC met the following additional DICA-IV diagnoses: ADHD (16), OCD (4), specific phobia (9), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) (4) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) (8). Twenty-four children with ASC were actively taking psychoactive medications, including stimulants (13), selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) (6), clonidine (2), risperidone (2) and lithium (2). Stimulant medications were discontinued the day prior to testing; all other medications were taken as prescribed. There was no diagnosis or family history of ASC in the typically developing control group. We included only individuals in the current sample with less than 3 mm translational and 3° rotational movement over the course of the resting scan.

Table 1 Participant characteristics of final sample

Cognitive and behavioural measures

Physical and Neurological Examination of Subtle Signs

Motor skills were assessed outside the scanner using the Physical and Neurological Examination of Subtle Signs (PANESS; [57]), a battery of motor control tasks designed for children and standardized for age, sex and handedness. It is sensitive to children’s developmental changes in motor skills such as balance, coordination and speed and has adequate test-retest reliability, inter-rater reliability and internal consistency. Motor signs are quantified as dysrhythmia (inappropriate timing or sequencing of movements) and overflow (unintended and unnecessary movements) examined while performing gait, station and timed limb movements. Gaits and station measures are based on gait and balance assessment (gaits on heels, toes and sides of feet and tandem, standing and hopping on one foot, etc.). Timed limb movements are assessed during performance of repetitive and sequential movements of the hands and feet such as finger tapping, hand patting and toe tapping. Speed, overflow and dysrhythmia are incorporated into the total timed score. In addition to examining the total gait score and total timed score, the total PANESS score was also examined. For all three measures, better performance is associated with lower scores.

Handedness

Handedness was assessed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI; [58]), a self-completed questionnaire for determining hand preference. The sample comprised only right-handed individuals with EHI scores >40.

Structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging acquisition

All participants performed a mock scan the day before the actual scan. All individuals underwent scanning on one of two 3-T Philips scanners (2D-SENSE EPI, 8-channel head coil, SENSE acceleration = 2.0), and axially oriented volumes were acquired using T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging (field of view: 256 × 256 mm, matrix size 64 × 64, repetition time = 2500 ms, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 75°). Resting-state scans were acquired for 5 min and 20 s. Children were asked to stay as still as possible and fixate on a centre cross. T1-weighted high-resolution anatomical images were acquired coronally (field of view 256 × 200 mm2, matrix size 256 × 256, repetition time = 7.99 ms, echo time = 3.76 ms, flip angle = 8°, 1 mm isotropic voxels, slice thickness = 1 mm). These were used to generate age- and gender-matched symmetrical tissue priors.

Image preprocessing

Functional T2*-weighted images were preprocessed using statistical parametric mapping (SPM12; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience Group, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Images were slice-time corrected using the middle slice as reference slice and realigned relative to their mean. The high-resolution anatomical images were then co-registered to the functional images, segmented and normalized using a symmetrical, age- and gender-matched tissue prior generated with the Template-O-Matic toolbox [59]. The use of a symmetrical template prevents an additional introduction of anatomical asymmetries that might potentially interfere with functional asymmetries [60]. The normalization transformation was then applied to the functional images. Further steps included linear detrending at each voxel in the brain to correct for scanner drift, removal of nuisance variables such as the white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) using CompCor [61] (note that we did not use global signal regression (GSR) to avoid introduction of spurious anticorrelations in the data [62]) and six absolute and six differential motion parameters, spatial smoothing (6-mm full width at half maximum (FWHM)), and temporal band-pass filtering constraining the frequency window to 0.01–0.1 Hz. To minimise the confounding influence of micromovement, we computed the average framewise displacement (FD) (based on the median due to a non-normal distribution of movement) according to Power et al. [63] and excluded any participant with a z score of >2.58 [64].

Creation of seed regions

Mostofsky et al. [22] identified the right hemisphere (RH) and left hemisphere (LH) circuits involved in motor execution during a finger-sequencing paradigm in both children with ASC and neurotypical controls between 8 and 12 years. They identified regions of interest by single-group, whole-brain random effects analyses across both groups by executing one-sample t tests on the individual subject’s right- and left-rest contrast images. Individual within-group whole-brain activation analyses were then run for the two groups separately identifying peak activations by group. Here, peak coordinates were derived from typical children and served as centres of seeds.

Based on the ‘left hemisphere dysfunction’ theory of autism, which states that neural impairments are lateralized to the LH in individuals with ASC, we selected peak coordinates from this LH motor circuit (see Table 2). The LH motor circuit (right-sided movement) included the left sensorimotor cortex (SMC), left thalamus (TH), left putamen (PUT), bilateral supplementary motor area (SMA)-rostral (SMA-r), bilateral SMA-dorsal (SMA-d) and right anterior cerebellum (AC) seeds [4].

Table 2 Motor coordinates (mm) based on Mostofsky et al. [22]

Six-millimetre-radius 3D seeds were then created using the SPM toolbox PickAtlas (http://fmri.wfubmc.edu/software/PickAtlas) with the peak coordinates as the centre of spheres. All seeds were flipped across the midline (x = 0) in order to obtain a set of homotopic RH and LH seeds (see Fig. 1). Time series were then extracted from the seed regions of interest, and pairwise Pearson’s correlations were performed for the LH and RH circuits separately. Correlation coefficients were transformed to a normal distribution via Fisher’s z transform. Standardized correlations between all seed pairs were then averaged to obtain mean connectivity in the motor circuit in each hemisphere (RH connectivity (R con); LH connectivity (L con)) using MATLAB. For the assessment of functional lateralization, we calculated a laterality index (LI) for each pairwise connection and for the overall mean network connectivity by subtracting the connections in the LH from homotopic connections in the right hemisphere: R con − L con. Contrary to the usually applied formula for the calculation of structural asymmetry indices ((R − L)/(R + L)), here we did not include a denominator as functional connectivity measures comprise both positive and negative values, adversely influencing the index values [50].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Homotopic motor seeds derived from Mostofsky et al. [22]. Abbreviations: SMA-d dorsal SMA, SMA-r rostral SMA, Put putamen, Thal thalamus, Cerb cerebellum. Red: left hemisphere seeds; Blue right hemisphere seeds

Control networks

To test whether atypical lateralization is specific to the motor network and its underlying lateralized functions, we selected two control networks: the default mode network (DMN) and a visual network (VN). DMN seeds were based on peak seed regions of the task-negative network identified by Fox et al. [65] using resting-state connectivity. Seeds were located in the cerebellum, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), retro-splenial cortex (RSC), lateral parietal cortex (LP), superior frontal gyrus (SFG), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and parahippocampal gyrus (PHG). The VN seeds were based on visual seeds applied by Yeo et al. [66] and were comprised of the central and peripheral primary visual cortex (V1c and V1p), the central and peripheral regions near the visual area V3v (V3pv and V3cv) and the extrastriate regions of the central and peripheral visual subnetworks (ExP and ExC).

Statistical analyses

Group differences in mean connectivity and individual connection pairs

Participants were excluded as outliers if the average FD or laterality values had a z score of >2.58 [64, 67, 68]. For the analysis of the between-group differences in mean network connectivity, a univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted. To compare the LIs of all the individual connectivity pairs between the two groups (ASC vs. controls), separate ANCOVAs were conducted with each individual connectivity pair as dependent variables (DV) and the global mean of the LIs of the remaining individual connectivity pairs of the network (Global-Meannetwork) as nuisance covariates in order to discount their global effect. ANCOVAs were corrected for multiple comparisons by controlling the false discovery rate (FDR) at q = 0.05. Age and scanning machine were included as nuisance variables in all models. To control for micromovements, average FD was included as an additional covariate [67]. Furthermore, results were re-evaluated controlling for FIQ to observe if they remained significant. To determine whether significant group differences were driven by rightward over-connectivity or leftward under-connectivity, we also compared the Fisher transformed correlation coefficients of the LH and RH circuits (R con and L con) between the groups with the same model as that exploring the LI differences. To determine the direction of within-group circuit laterality, one-sample t tests were carried out. In an additional step, analyses were repeated in sex-stratified samples. Effect sizes were calculated based on Cohen’s d.

Association with cognitive measures within ASC

To test whether atypical lateralization was related to function, we conducted one-tailed (based on our hypothesis that stronger rightward lateralization would be related to more symptoms and poorer performance) Pearson’s correlations controlling for scanner, age and average FD within individuals with ASC. For individual connectivity pairs, the Global-Meannetwork was additionally included as a covariate. Motor LIs showing group differences were correlated with (a) the repetitive behaviour subscores of the ADI-R (ADI-C) and ADOS (ADOS-D) and (b) the total PANESS score, total gait score (composite measure of speed, overflow and dysrhythmia during gaits and stations) and total timed score (composite measure of speed, overflow and dysrhythmia during all timed movements). Level of significance was set at p = 0.05, as all correlation analyses were targeted. All statistical analyses were carried out in SPSS (version 21, SPSS Inc.).

Based on previous reports showing a linkage between motor and social development [69, 70] and the hypothesis that motor deficits might underpin some of the core social-communicative symptoms in ASC [71], an additional, exploratory analysis was conducted posing the question whether lateralization of motor circuit connectivity was related to core social and communicative symptoms, with stronger rightward lateralization being related to more deficits as measured by the ADOS-A, ADOS-B, ADI-A and ADI-B.

Results

Participant characteristics and task performance

Four control individuals and one individual with ASC were excluded due to z values of >2.58 on average FD. One additional individual with ASC was excluded due to z values of >2.58 on more than three laterality measures. The final sample consisted of 42 individuals with ASC (34 males, 8 females) and 76 controls (54 males, 22 females). After exclusion, the two groups were matched on age (t(62.51) = −0.092, p = 0.927), handedness (U = 1590.5, z = −0.032, p = 0.974), sex (χ 2(1) = 0.925, p = 0.336), average FD (U = 1481, z = −0.646, p = 0.518) and PRI (t(116) = 0.768, p = 0.444). There were, however, significant differences in FIQ (t(64.87) = 3.136, p = 0.003) and VCI (t(70.10) = 3.353, p = 0.001). Individuals with ASC had significantly poorer performance on the total score of the PANESS (F(1,115) = 45.081, p < 0.001), and on the two sub-measures: total gait (F(1,115) = 36.804, p < 0.001) and total timed (F(1,115) = 32.339, p < 0.001).

Group differences in lateralization of motor circuit connectivity

There was a significant difference for the LI of mean motor circuit connectivity between the ASC and control group (F(1,113) = 6.814, p = 0.010; Cohen’s d = 0.506), with children with ASC being more strongly rightward lateralized (see Fig. 2a). The result remained significant when controlling for FIQ (F(1,112) = 6.094, p = 0.015; Cohen’s d = 0.479). This difference was driven by both rightward over- and leftward under-connectivity in mean motor connectivity in children with ASC (group difference R con F(1,113) = 1.110, p = 0.294; group difference L con F(1,113) = 0.536, p = 0.466). A one-sample t test revealed that children with ASC were rightward lateralized (t(41) = 2.390, p = 0.022), whereas control children showed a symmetrical organization (t(75) = −1.255, p = 0.213). Individual LIs of ‘motor’ connections were not significantly different or did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (see Table 3). After stratifying analyses by sex, the result was trending in males only (F(1,83) = 3.920, p = 0.051; Cohen’s d = 0.438; FIQ F(1,82) = 2.512, p = 0.117; Cohen’s d = 0.351) and not significant in females (F(1,25) = 1.311, p = 0.263; Cohen’s d = 0.489; FIQ F(1,24) = 3.701, p = 0.066; Cohen’s d = 0.822) (for a distribution of values across sex, see Fig. 2b).

Fig. 2
figure 2

a Group differences in LIs of mean motor network connectivity. Abbreviations: NT neurotypicals (controls). Positive values indicate rightward lateralization, negative values indicate leftward lateralization. Children with ASC show reversed rightward lateralization of mean motor connectivity. b LIs of motor network connectivity across sexes

Table 3 Group differences in LIs of individual motor connections

Association with cognitive measures in children with ASC

The LI of total mean motor connectivity was positively associated with the total PANESS scores (r = 0.455, p = 0.002), total gait subscores (r = 0.434, p = 0.003) and total timed subscores (r = 0.357, p = 0.013) (see Table 4). In each case, stronger rightward lateralization was associated with worse performance on the motor task (see Fig. 3a–c). There were no associations with either the ADOS-D (r = 0.117, p = 0.238) or ADI-C (r = −0.026, p = 0.438). There were no significant associations between the LI of mean motor connectivity and the ADOS-A (r = 0.127, p = 0.220), ADOS-B (r = 0.217, p = 0.093), ADI-A (r = 0.065, p = 0.348) and ADI-B (r = −0.075, p = 0.325). Correlations with the PANESS were not significant in typical children (total PANESS score r = −0.059, p = 0.310; total gait subscores r = 0.094, p = 0.215; total timed subscores r = −0.132, p = 0.133).

Table 4 Correlations between the LI of mean motor circuit connectivity and motor-related symptoms
Fig. 3
figure 3

Association between LI of mean motor network connectivity and motor-related symptoms. a Association between LI of mean motor network connectivity and PANESS total scores. b Association between LI of mean motor network connectivity and PANESS total gaits scores. c Association between LI of mean motor network connectivity and PANESS total timed scores

Control networks

Default mode network

There was no significant difference in LI of mean DMN connectivity (F(1,113) = 2.12, p = 0.143) between individuals with ASC and controls. Individual LIs of DMN connections were not significantly different or did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (for details, see Additional file 1).

Visual network

There was no significant difference in the LI of mean VN connectivity (F(1,113) = 0.467; p = 0.496) between individuals with ASC and controls. Individual LIs of VN connections were not significantly different (for details, see Additional file 1).

Discussion

In the current study, we investigated whether the functional motor execution network shows an atypical pattern of hemispheric specialization in ASC. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found stronger rightward lateralization in children with ASC compared to typically developing children. Disturbance in motor functioning in children with ASC has been linked to dysfunction in multiple motor regions/systems, including the fronto-striatal and cerebellar systems [22, 43, 7274]. However, few studies have explored the resting-state functional connectivity within the motor execution system in children with ASC [22, 43, 44]. Mostofsky et al. [22] report the overall under-connectivity, which is more pronounced during motor execution than during rest. Nebel et al. [44] focused specifically on M1 connectivity, examining autism-associated differences in regional M1 functional parcellation; they found organizational differentiation in the motor homunculus pointing to an early failure in functional specialization in the motor cortex in children with ASC. Here, we further show that the neurobiological underpinnings of motor impairments in ASC are related to atypical hemispheric lateralization in motor circuits.

Our finding is particularly interesting in the light of rising theories suggesting that autism is a condition of atypical connectivity involving multiple cerebral networks deficient in synchronization and information integration [36, 37]. However, so far, most functional connectivity studies in ASC have focused on under- or over-connectivity of networks critically involved in cognitive and social functions such as the default mode network [3942] but have not addressed atypical lateralization of these circuits. Our findings are in line with Cardinale et al.’s [45] report of rightward lateralization of the motor networks. They reported on a wider range of networks, raising the question whether atypical rightward asymmetry is a more fundamental feature of network organization in ASC and not restricted to specific functions such as language and motor behaviour. However, we did not find atypical lateralization patterns across all networks; that is, we observed no group differences in the visual and default mode networks. These discrepancies might be due to differences in methodology between the two studies; Cardinale et al. [45] applied a model-free whole-brain approach aggregating information across many regions, whereas we took a hypothesis-driven approach, focusing on specific regions and their interconnections using seed-based analysis. While seed-based and ICA-based analyses yield similar results [75], using a model-free exploratory approach may identify additional unexpected patterns at the network level without making prior assumptions about functional localization.

Rightward asymmetry of motor circuit functional connectivity is associated with motor performance in children with ASC

This study is the first to report a relationship between autistic symptoms and hemispheric lateralization outside the language domain in individuals with ASC; namely, the more rightward asymmetry of the functional correspondence within the motor circuit, the poorer motor performance on three different measures of the PANESS. Both Nielsen et al. [46], who reported atypical lateralization in a language-related network, and Cardinale et al. [45], who reported rightward lateralization of the motor network, did not find such brain-behavioural association. Here, we highlight the importance of their findings by showing functional relevance in children with ASC. Lateralization has been linked to fine motor skills, primarily in relation to handedness. Interestingly, here, we showed that, in addition to fine motor skills, gross motor performances such as gait, balance, timing and sequencing of movements were also associated with atypical hemispheric specialization in children with ASC. Therefore, rightward lateralization may underlie gross motor deficits and atypical gait commonly found in individuals with ASC.

Leftward lateralization of the same circuit has previously been shown to be associated with better motor performance in typically developing children [4]; however, we did not replicate this result here. It remains to be established whether this is due to differing sample characteristics and methods (note that Barber et al. used a different registration approach and laterality index) and whether lateralization of this network constitutes the biological underpinnings for typical motor development or for motor deficits in ASC specifically.

Lateralization in the motor system is present in both structure and function, although it is generally less pronounced compared to the language system lateralization, which would explain why typical children show a subtle shift only towards left here. Structurally, this is characterized by a deeper left central sulcus, increased leftward neuropil in BA4 [76] and expansion of the left hand motor cortex in right-handers [77]. Guye et al. [78] show that whole-brain functional connectivity is more extensive with the left than with the right primary motor cortex. Left premotor and parietal regions are more strongly implicated in higher order actions [79]. Also, the planning of complex, sequential movements [7981], bimanual coordination [82, 83] and response selection [84] have been attributed to the left hemisphere, which is interesting considering the problems individuals with ASC experience with fine motor skills [85] and planned sequencing of actions [86]. In contrast, sensory processing and spatial aspects of sensorimotor actions have been reported to show right hemisphere dominance [8789]. It has been suggested that posture and limb position [90], as well as the use of proprioceptive feedback [91, 92], are preferentially lateralized to the right hemisphere in right-handed individuals. Interestingly, we have previously reported that individuals with ASC rely to a much greater extent on proprioceptive feedback during motor learning, which is related to more severe motor impairments and other social symptoms [93]. Atypical rightward lateralization of motor circuits might constitute a neural mechanism contributing to this finding.

Reports of disturbance in establishing typical patterns of hemispheric specialization in ASC date back to the consistently replicated observation of increased rates of left-handedness among individuals with ASC [94101]. Left-handers activate the right premotor cortex during contra- and ipsilateral finger movements [102] and have increased intrasulcal surface in the right precentral gyrus [76]. Interestingly, our results are not driven by differences in handedness, as participants were matched on a measure of handedness and were right-handed. Still, it will be interesting to explore whether the observed pattern is even more pronounced in left-handed individuals with ASC.

Implications of motor deficits in ASC

Motor deficits are present in at least 80 % of children with ASC [28, 103, 104] comprising some of the earliest signs, such as delays in motor milestones [105, 106]. Later on in development, motor-related deficits persist and become more apparent, with deficits in gross and fine motor coordination [19, 47, 107, 108], atypical gait and posture [109, 110] and particularly difficulty with performance of skilled gestures (i.e. praxis) [18, 27, 28].

In line with this, it has been argued that core social and communicative symptoms in autism might be subserved by the same neural systems underpinning motor-related dysfunction [111, 112]. In fact, there is a close relationship between motor and social-communicative behaviours: motor development is the prerequisite for speech, articulation and engagement in the social world through directing attention, grasping and sharing things, and approaching, imitating and responding to others. In line with this, Iverson and Braddock [113] have shown that children with language deficits have poorer fine and gross motor skills. At the same time, production and understanding of actions, such as performance of sequences of actions and following verbal commands, are dependent on the development of language skills. Thus, atypical lateralization in related cognitive domains such as language might be influenced by the same developmental mechanisms [114, 115]. Notably, here, we did not find a significant association between atypical motor circuit lateralization and social-communicative sub-measures of either the ADOS or ADI. Given that other studies find such relationship [116], future research needs to examine a potential association with more specific measures of atypical social development in ASC and explore the link between motor and social networks directly at the neuronal level.

Limitations and future directions

Here, we report atypical laterality of the motor network in children with ASC. Given that ASC is a neurodevelopmental condition, it remains to be established whether the observed pattern is stable across the lifespan. Many studies show temporal test-retest reliability and reproducibility of functional resting-state networks [117119]. It has been argued that neural coupling configurations are dynamic and transient over time with fluctuations in connection strengths [120, 121]. As arousal, sleep [122], conscious [123], cognitive [124] and emotional [125] states constitute influencing factors, only replication in independent, longitudinal samples can confirm the stability of the reported findings. Age-related changes have been described in a range of different networks, with decreasing connectivity representing increasing hemispheric specialization and increasing connectivity representing increasing functional cooperation [126]. The same authors also reported not only linear but also quadratic and cubic age effects on changes of functional connectivity, emphasizing the need for longitudinal samples to fully explore the stability and plausible developmental trend of the reported findings.

One limitation concerns the heterogeneity within ASC. Children with ASC frequently have other comorbid conditions, such as ADHD, which is another condition associated with atypical lateralization [127]. Also, Mahajan et al. [128] showed that individuals with ASC with and without ADHD show differences in motor cortex laterality with children with ASC without ADHD exhibiting rightward increases in M1, whereas children with ASC and comorbid ADHD showing left-lateralized increases. Medication exposure is another possible confound which might influence connectivity strength [129131]. Overall, a large portion of individuals with ASC have psychiatric comorbidities during their lifespan (ADHD 28–44 %; anxiety 42–56 %; depression 12–70 %; oppositional defiant disorder 16–28 %; [132]) and around 70 % of children with ASC use some form of psychoactive medication [133, 134], making any sample that excludes these individuals less representative of the autistic population.

It remains to be established whether these results are restricted to lateralized left hemisphere functions or whether right hemisphere functions such as visuospatial abilities are also affected. Cardinale et al. [45] report rightward asymmetries of more widespread networks including those underlying visuospatial attention, but no other studies have looked at hemispheric specialization of visuospatial information processing in ASC. Future research should explore whether stronger rightward lateralization might underpin superior visual attention and weak central coherence in ASC and whether this potentially differentiates between individuals with ASC with and without enhanced visuospatial processing abilities.

Heterogeneity within the autism spectrum is the biggest challenge in the current research. It will be of interest to explore whether atypical motor network lateralization is characteristic of an as-yet undefined subgroup within ASC, such as language delay, for example, [60]. Other factors constituting heterogeneity include handedness, sex or IQ. We previously showed that handedness can be a marker of rightward lateralization of structural connectivity of regions connecting sensorimotor cortical areas [135]. Whether this also applies to functional lateralization remains to be established. As for sex, our findings here were most pronounced when including both males and females. Sex-stratified analyses show an overlapping functional organization in males and females with ASC regarding lateralization of the motor network. In general, few studies have looked at sex differences in the laterality of resting-state connectivity. Tian et al. [136] reported that males show more locally efficient right hemisphere networks, whereas females have more locally efficient left hemisphere networks. Zuo et al. [126] reported that homotopic resting-state connectivity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the amygdala show opposite developmental trajectories in males and females. How functional connectivity and its lateralization further differ across genders in ASC awaits investigation. Regarding IQ, our sample was only matched on perceptual reasoning IQ; however, additional analyses showed that results remained largely unaffected by full-scale IQ (i.e. when controlling for FIQ). As motor delay is mostly evident in low-functioning individuals [137, 138], it will be important to explore whether this result is more pronounced in this sub-population with ASC.

The present findings were based on patterns observed in the so-called resting state, and it will be interesting to explore whether atypical rightward lateralization remains robust under conditions when the motor execution network is active, as well as whether they are more pronounced in particular parcels underlying the motor homunculus. Lastly, here, we focused on atypical intrahemispheric connectivity. However, many studies have shown that atypical interhemispheric connectivity (such as reduced corpus callosum size) contributes to atypical connectivity in ASC [139141]. Efficient information processing across the whole brain substantially depends on interhemispheric integration, which may be impaired in ASC [142]. Herbert et al. [143] show that only intrahemispheric connectivity is impaired in ASC, whereas others demonstrate that intra- and interhemispheric connectivities are both affected [141, 144]. Particularly, the right hemisphere regions involved in visuospatial processing have been suggested to rely on more integrative, interhemispheric processing [5]. Thus, future studies need to explore whether atypical lateralization in ASC extends to cross-hemispheric networks, particularly for functions such as visuospatial skills.

Conclusions

Our novel findings show that atypical functional lateralization in ASC extends beyond the language domain to functional circuits underlying motor execution. The deficit in children with ASC in establishing a typical pattern of hemispheric specialization for motor control may contribute to difficulties with motor skill development and might even form the early basis for the development of social and communicative impairments. It remains to be established whether atypical lateralization in functionally related domains (motor and language functions) share common neurodevelopmental origins and trajectories.

Abbreviations

AC, anterior cerebellum; ADI-R, Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; BOLD, blood-oxygen-level-dependent; ASC, autism spectrum conditions; CSF, cerebrospinal fluid; DMN, default mode network; EHI, Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; FD, framewise displacement; FIQ, full-scale IQ; GSR, global signal regression; LH, left hemisphere; LI, laterality index; PANESS, Physical and Neurological Examination of Subtle Signs; PRI, perceptual reasoning index; PUT, putamen; RH, right hemisphere; SMA-d, bilateral supplementary motor area-dorsal segment; SMA-r, bilateral supplementary motor area-rostral segment; SMC, sensorimotor cortex; TH, thalamus; VCI, verbal comprehension index; WM, white matter

References

  1. Gazzaniga MS. Principles of human brain organization derived from split-brain studies. Neuron. 1995;14(2):217–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Mesulam MM. Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Ann Neurol. 1990;28(5):597–613. doi:10.1002/ana.410280502.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Hugdahl K. Fifty years of dichotic listening research—still going and going and…. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):211–3. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.006.

  4. Barber AD, Srinivasan P, Joel SE, Caffo BS, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH. Motor “dexterity”?: evidence that left hemisphere lateralization of motor circuit connectivity is associated with better motor performance in children. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(1):51–9. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr062.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Gotts SJ, Jo HJ, Wallace GL, Saad ZS, Cox RW, Martin A. Two distinct forms of functional lateralization in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(36):E3435–44. doi:10.1073/pnas.1302581110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Mellet E, Zago L, Jobard G, Crivello F, Petit L, Joliot M, et al. Weak language lateralization affects both verbal and spatial skills: an fMRI study in 297 subjects. Neuropsychologia. 2014;65:56–62. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Rojas DC, Bawn SD, Benkers TL, Reite ML, Rogers SJ. Smaller left hemisphere planum temporale in adults with autistic disorder. Neurosci Lett. 2002;328(3):237–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Deutsch CK, O’Brien LM, Kennedy DN, Filipek PA, et al. Brain asymmetries in autism and developmental language disorder: a nested whole-brain analysis. Brain. 2005;128(Pt 1):213–26. doi:10.1093/brain/awh330.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Dawson G, Finley C, Phillips S, Lewy A. A comparison of hemispheric asymmetries in speech-related brain potentials of autistic and dysphasic children. Brain Lang. 1989;37(1):26–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Seery AM, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Atypical lateralization of ERP response to native and non-native speech in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2013;5:10–24. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Boddaert N, Belin P, Chabane N, Poline JB, Barthélémy C, Mouren-Simeoni MC, et al. Perception of complex sounds: abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autism. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(11):2057–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Müller RA, Behen ME, Rothermel RD, Chugani DC, Muzik O, Mangner TJ, et al. Brain mapping of language and auditory perception in high-functioning autistic adults: a PET study. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29(1):19–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Knaus TA, Silver AM, Kennedy M, Lindgren KA, Dominick KC, Siegel J, et al. Language laterality in autism spectrum disorder and typical controls: a functional, volumetric, and diffusion tensor MRI study. Brain Lang. 2010;112(2):113–20. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Eyler LT, Pierce K, Courchesne E. A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain. 2012;135(Pt 3):949–60. doi:10.1093/brain/awr364.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Kleinhans NM, Müller RA, Cohen DN, Courchesne E. Atypical functional lateralization of language in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. 2008;1221:115–25. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.04.080.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Lombardo MV, Pierce K, Eyler LT, Carter Barnes C, Ahrens-Barbeau C, Solso S, et al. Different functional neural substrates for good and poor language outcome in autism. Neuron. 2015;86(2):567–77. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.023.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Teitelbaum P, Teitelbaum O, Nye J, Fryman J, Maurer RG. Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95(23):13982–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Mostofsky SH, Dubey P, Jerath VK, Jansiewicz EM, Goldberg MC, Denckla MB. Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to imitation in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12(3):314–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Green D, Charman T, Pickles A, Chandler S, Loucas T, Simonoff E, et al. Impairment in movement skills of children with autistic spectrum disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;51(4):311–6. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03242.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Müller RA, Kleinhans N, Kemmotsu N, Pierce K, Courchesne E. Abnormal variability and distribution of functional maps in autism: an FMRI study of visuomotor learning. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(10):1847–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Allen G, Courchesne E. Differential effects of developmental cerebellar abnormality on cognitive and motor functions in the cerebellum: an fMRI study of autism. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(2):262–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Mostofsky SH, Powell SK, Simmonds DJ, Goldberg MC, Caffo B, Pekar JJ. Decreased connectivity and cerebellar activity in autism during motor task performance. Brain. 2009;132(Pt 9):2413–25. doi:10.1093/brain/awp088.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Allen G, Müller RA, Courchesne E. Cerebellar function in autism: functional magnetic resonance image activation during a simple motor task. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56(4):269–78. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Dawson G, Warrenburg S, Fuller P. Hemisphere functioning and motor imitation in autistic persons. Brain Cogn. 1983;2(4):346–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Müller RA, Cauich C, Rubio MA, Mizuno A, Courchesne E. Abnormal activity patterns in premotor cortex during sequence learning in autistic patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56(5):323–32. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. D’Cruz AM, Mosconi MW, Steele S, Rubin LH, Luna B, Minshew N, et al. Lateralized response timing deficits in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(4):393–7. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.01.008.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Dowell LR, Mahone EM, Mostofsky SH. Associations of postural knowledge and basic motor skill with dyspraxia in autism: implication for abnormalities in distributed connectivity and motor learning. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(5):563–70. doi:10.1037/a0015640.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Dziuk MA, Gidley Larson JC, Apostu A, Mahone EM, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH. Dyspraxia in autism: association with motor, social, and communicative deficits. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2007;49(10):734–9. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00734.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Mostofsky SH, Ewen JB. Altered connectivity and action model formation in autism is autism. Neuroscientist. 2011;17(4):437–48. doi:10.1177/1073858410392381.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1995;34(4):537–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:1–38. doi:10.1196/annals.1440.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(9):700–11. doi:10.1038/nrn2201.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Lackner JR, Teuber HL. Alterations in auditory fusion thresholds after cerebral injury in man. Neuropsychologia. 1973;11(4):409–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Semmes J. Hemispheric specialization: a possible clue to mechanism. Neuropsychologia. 1968;6(1):11–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2011;34:569–99. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Belmonte MK, Allen G, Beckel-Mitchener A, Boulanger LM, Carper RA, Webb SJ. Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity. J Neurosci. 2004;24(42):9228–31. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3340-04.2004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Courchesne E, Pierce K. Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: local over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15(2):225–30. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ. Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain. 2004;127(Pt 8):1811–21. doi:10.1093/brain/awh199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Weng SJ, Wiggins JL, Peltier SJ, Carrasco M, Risi S, Lord C, et al. Alterations of resting state functional connectivity in the default network in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. 2010;1313:202–14. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.057.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lynch CJ, Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Khouzam A, Phillips J, Menon V. Default mode network in childhood autism: posteromedial cortex heterogeneity and relationship with social deficits. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(3):212–9. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Assaf M, Jagannathan K, Calhoun VD, Miller L, Stevens MC, Sahl R, et al. Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism spectrum disorder patients. Neuroimage. 2010;53(1):247–56. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.067.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Spencer MD, Chura LR, Holt RJ, Suckling J, Calder AJ, Bullmore ET, et al. Failure to deactivate the default mode network indicates a possible endophenotype of autism. Mol Autism. 2012;3(1):15. doi:10.1186/2040-2392-3-15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Nebel MB, Eloyan A, Barber AD, Mostofsky SH. Precentral gyrus functional connectivity signatures of autism. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8:80. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2014.00080.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Nebel MB, Joel SE, Muschelli J, Barber AD, Caffo BS, Pekar JJ, et al. Disruption of functional organization within the primary motor cortex in children with autism. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(2):567–80. doi:10.1002/hbm.22188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Cardinale RC, Shih P, Fishman I, Ford LM, Müller RA. Pervasive rightward asymmetry shifts of functional networks in autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(9):975–82. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.382.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Nielsen JA, Zielinski BA, Fletcher PT, Alexander AL, Lange N, Bigler ED, et al. Abnormal lateralization of functional connectivity between language and default mode regions in autism. Mol Autism. 2014;5(1):8. doi:10.1186/2040-2392-5-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Jansiewicz EM, Goldberg MC, Newschaffer CJ, Denckla MB, Landa R, Mostofsky SH. Motor signs distinguish children with high functioning autism and Asperger’s syndrome from controls. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36(5):613–21. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0109-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Fournier KA, Hass CJ, Naik SK, Lodha N, Cauraugh JH. Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40(10):1227–40. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Izawa J, Pekny SE, Marko MK, Haswell CC, Shadmehr R, Mostofsky SH. Motor learning relies on integrated sensory inputs in ADHD, but over-selectively on proprioception in autism spectrum conditions. Autism Res. 2012;5(2):124–36. doi:10.1002/aur.1222.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Nielsen JA, Zielinski BA, Ferguson MA, Lainhart JE, Anderson JS. An evaluation of the left-brain vs. right-brain hypothesis with resting state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging. PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e71275. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0071275.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. 4th ed., Text revision. ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.

  52. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24(5):659–85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook Jr EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(3):205–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children—III. 3rd ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1991.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children—IV. 4th ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Reich W. Diagnostic interview for children and adolescents (DICA). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(1):59–66. doi:10.1097/00004583-200001000-00017.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Denckla MB. Revised neurological examination for subtle signs (1985). Psychopharmacol Bull. 1985;21(4):773–800.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Wilke M, Holland SK, Altaye M, Gaser C. Template-O-Matic: a toolbox for creating customized pediatric templates. Neuroimage. 2008;41(3):903–13. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Floris DL, Lai MC, Auer T, Lombardo MV, Ecker C, Chakrabarti B, et al. Atypically rightward cerebral asymmetry in male adults with autism stratifies individuals with and without language delay. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(1):230–53. doi:10.1002/hbm.23023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage. 2007;37(1):90–101. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Gotts SJ, Saad ZS, Jo HJ, Wallace GL, Cox RW, Martin A. The perils of global signal regression for group comparisons: a case study of autism spectrum disorders. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:356. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00356.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Steps toward optimizing motion artifact removal in functional connectivity MRI; a reply to Carp. Neuroimage. 2013;76:439–41. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Di Martino A, Zuo XN, Kelly C, Grzadzinski R, Mennes M, Schvarcz A, et al. Shared and distinct intrinsic functional network centrality in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(8):623–32. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102(27):9673–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0504136102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–65. doi:10.1152/jn.00338.2011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Di Martino A, Yan CG, Li Q, Denio E, Castellanos FX, Alaerts K, et al. The autism brain imaging data exchange: towards a large-scale evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(6):659–67. doi:10.1038/mp.2013.78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Gonzalez-Gadea ML, Baez S, Torralva T, Castellanos FX, Rattazzi A, Bein V, et al. Cognitive variability in adults with ADHD and AS: disentangling the roles of executive functions and social cognition. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(2):817–30. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.11.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Brian J, Bryson SE, Garon N, Roberts W, Smith IM, Szatmari P, et al. Clinical assessment of autism in high-risk 18-month-olds. Autism. 2008;12(5):433–56. doi:10.1177/1362361308094500.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Sutera S, Pandey J, Esser EL, Rosenthal MA, Wilson LB, Barton M, et al. Predictors of optimal outcome in toddlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(1):98–107. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0340-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Bhat AN, Landa RJ, Galloway JC. Current perspectives on motor functioning in infants, children, and adults with autism spectrum disorders. Phys Ther. 2011;91(7):1116–29. doi:10.2522/ptj.20100294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Sears LL, Vest C, Mohamed S, Bailey J, Ranson BJ, Piven J. An MRI study of the basal ganglia in autism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1999;23(4):613–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Siegel BV, Asarnow R, Tanguay P, Call JD, Abel L, Ho A, et al. Regional cerebral glucose metabolism and attention in adults with a history of childhood autism. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1992;4(4):406–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. McAlonan GM, Daly E, Kumari V, Critchley HD, van Amelsvoort T, Suckling J, et al. Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger’s syndrome. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 7):1594–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Joel SE, Caffo BS, van Zijl PC, Pekar JJ. On the relationship between seed-based and ICA-based measures of functional connectivity. Magn Reson Med. 2011;66(3):644–57. doi:10.1002/mrm.22818.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Amunts K, Schlaug G, Schleicher A, Steinmetz H, Dabringhaus A, Roland PE, et al. Asymmetry in the human motor cortex and handedness. Neuroimage. 1996;4(3 Pt 1):216–22. doi:10.1006/nimg.1996.0073.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Volkmann J, Schnitzler A, Witte OW, Freund H. Handedness and asymmetry of hand representation in human motor cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79(4):2149–54.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Guye M, Parker GJ, Symms M, Boulby P, Wheeler-Kingshott CA, Salek-Haddadi A, et al. Combined functional MRI and tractography to demonstrate the connectivity of the human primary motor cortex in vivo. Neuroimage. 2003;19(4):1349–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Haaland KY, Elsinger CL, Mayer AR, Durgerian S, Rao SM. Motor sequence complexity and performing hand produce differential patterns of hemispheric lateralization. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(4):621–36. doi:10.1162/089892904323057344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Verstynen T, Diedrichsen J, Albert N, Aparicio P, Ivry RB. Ipsilateral motor cortex activity during unimanual hand movements relates to task complexity. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93(3):1209–22. doi:10.1152/jn.00720.2004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Schluter ND, Rushworth MF, Passingham RE, Mills KR. Temporary interference in human lateral premotor cortex suggests dominance for the selection of movements. A study using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain. 1998;121(Pt 5):785–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Jäncke L, Specht K, Shah JN, Hugdahl K. Focused attention in a simple dichotic listening task: an fMRI experiment. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003;16(2):257–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Serrien DJ, Cassidy MJ, Brown P. The importance of the dominant hemisphere in the organization of bimanual movements. Hum Brain Mapp. 2003;18(4):296–305. doi:10.1002/hbm.10086.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Weissman DH, Banich MT. The cerebral hemispheres cooperate to perform complex but not simple tasks. Neuropsychology. 2000;14(1):41–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Dawson G, Watling R. Interventions to facilitate auditory, visual, and motor integration in autism: a review of the evidence. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(5):415–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Greenspan SI, Wieder S. Developmental patterns and outcomes in infants and children with disorders in relating and communicating: a chart review of 200 cases of children with autistic spectrum diagnoses. J Dev Learn Disord. 1997;1(1):87–141.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Ghilardi M, Ghez C, Dhawan V, Moeller J, Mentis M, Nakamura T, et al. Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning. Brain Res. 2000;871(1):127–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Wenderoth N, Debaere F, Sunaert S, van Hecke P, Swinnen SP. Parieto-premotor areas mediate directional interference during bimanual movements. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14(10):1153–63. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh075.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Haaland KY, Harrington DL. Hemispheric control of the initial and corrective components of aiming movements. Neuropsychologia. 1989;27(7):961–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Sainburg RL. Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness. Exp Brain Res. 2002;142(2):241–58. doi:10.1007/s00221-001-0913-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Goble DJ, Brown SH. The biological and behavioral basis of upper limb asymmetries in sensorimotor performance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32(3):598–610. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.10.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Goble DJ, Noble BC, Brown SH. Proprioceptive target matching asymmetries in left-handed individuals. Exp Brain Res. 2009;197(4):403–8. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1922-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Haswell CC, Izawa J, Dowell LR, Mostofsky SH, Shadmehr R. Representation of internal models of action in the autistic brain. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(8):970–2. doi:10.1038/nn.2356.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  94. Colby KM, Parkison C. Handedness in autistic children. J Autism Child Schizophr. 1977;7(1):3–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Barry RJ, James AL. Handedness in autistics, retardates, and normals of a wide age range. J Autism Child Schizophr. 1978;8(3):315–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Gillberg C. Autistic children’s hand preferences: results from an epidemiological study of infantile autism. Psychiatry Res. 1983;10(1):21–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Ricks DM, Wing L. Handedness subtypes in autism. Psychiatric Annals. 1985;15(7):447–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Soper HV, Satz P, Orsini DL, Henry RR, Zvi JC, Schulman M. Handedness patterns in autism suggest subtypes. J Autism Dev Disord. 1986;16(2):155–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. McManus IC, Murray B, Doyle K, Baron-Cohen S. Handedness in childhood autism shows a dissociation of skill and preference. Cortex. 1992;28(3):373–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Cornish KM, McManus IC. Hand preference and hand skill in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1996;26(6):597–609.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Preslar J, Kushner HI, Marino L, Pearce B. Autism, lateralisation, and handedness: a review of the literature and meta-analysis. Laterality. 2014;19(1):64–95. doi:10.1080/1357650X.2013.772621.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Kawashima R, Inoue K, Sato K, Fukuda H. Functional asymmetry of cortical motor control in left-handed subjects. Neuroreport. 1997;8(7):1729–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Ghaziuddin M, Butler E. Clumsiness in autism and Asperger syndrome: a further report. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1998;42(Pt 1):43–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. David FJ, Baranek GT, Giuliani CA, Mercer VS, Poe MD, Thorpe DE. A pilot study: coordination of precision grip in children and adolescents with high functioning autism. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2009;21(2):205–11. doi:10.1097/PEP.0b013e3181a3afc2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  105. Iverson JM, Wozniak RH. Variation in vocal-motor development in infant siblings of children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(1):158–70. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0339-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Ming X, Brimacombe M, Wagner GC. Prevalence of motor impairment in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Dev. 2007;29(9):565–70. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2007.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Ament K, Mejia A, Buhlman R, Erklin S, Caffo B, Mostofsky S, et al. Evidence for specificity of motor impairments in catching and balance in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(3):742–51. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2229-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  108. Noterdaeme M, Mildenberger K, Minow F, Amorosa H. Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and language disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;11(5):219–25. doi:10.1007/s00787-002-0285-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Minshew NJ, Sung K, Jones BL, Furman JM. Underdevelopment of the postural control system in autism. Neurology. 2004;63(11):2056–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Rinehart NJ, Tonge BJ, Bradshaw JL, Iansek R, Enticott PG, McGinley J. Gait function in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder : evidence for basal-ganglia and cerebellar involvement? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;15(5):256–64. doi:10.1007/s00787-006-0530-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Nayate A, Bradshaw JL, Rinehart NJ. Autism and Asperger’s disorder: are they movement disorders involving the cerebellum and/or basal ganglia? Brain Res Bull. 2005;67(4):327–34. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.07.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Leary MR, Hill DA. Moving on: autism and movement disturbance. Ment Retard. 1996;34(1):39–53.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Iverson JM, Braddock BA. Gesture and motor skill in relation to language in children with language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2011;54(1):72–86. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2010/08-0197).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Knecht S, Dräger B, Deppe M, Bobe L, Lohmann H, Flöel A, et al. Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain. 2000;123(Pt 12):2512–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Pujol J, Deus J, Losilla JM, Capdevila A. Cerebral lateralization of language in normal left-handed people studied by functional MRI. Neurology. 1999;52(5):1038–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Nebel MB, Eloyan A, Nettles CA, Sweeney KL, Ament K, Ward RE, et al. Intrinsic visual-motor synchrony correlates with social deficits in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79(8):633–41. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.08.029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Shehzad Z, Kelly AM, Reiss PT, Gee DG, Gotimer K, Uddin LQ, et al. The resting brain: unconstrained yet reliable. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(10):2209–29. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn256.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Dosenbach NU, Nardos B, Cohen AL, Fair DA, Power JD, Church JA, et al. Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science. 2010;329(5997):1358–61. doi:10.1126/science.1194144.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  119. Chou YH, Panych LP, Dickey CC, Petrella JR, Chen NK. Investigation of long-term reproducibility of intrinsic connectivity network mapping: a resting-state fMRI study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2012;33(5):833–8. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A2894.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  120. Chang C, Glover GH. Time-frequency dynamics of resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. Neuroimage. 2010;50(1):81–98. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Hutchison RM, Womelsdorf T, Gati JS, Everling S, Menon RS. Resting-state networks show dynamic functional connectivity in awake humans and anesthetized macaques. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34(9):2154–77. doi:10.1002/hbm.22058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  122. Fukunaga M, Horovitz SG, van Gelderen P, de Zwart JA, Jansma JM, Ikonomidou VN, et al. Large-amplitude, spatially correlated fluctuations in BOLD fMRI signals during extended rest and early sleep stages. Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;24(8):979–92. doi:10.1016/j.mri.2006.04.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Greicius MD, Kiviniemi V, Tervonen O, Vainionpää V, Alahuhta S, Reiss AL, et al. Persistent default-mode network connectivity during light sedation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29(7):839–47. doi:10.1002/hbm.20537.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  124. Waites AB, Stanislavsky A, Abbott DF, Jackson GD. Effect of prior cognitive state on resting state networks measured with functional connectivity. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;24(1):59–68. doi:10.1002/hbm.20069.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  125. Harrison BJ, Pujol J, Ortiz H, Fornito A, Pantelis C, Yücel M. Modulation of brain resting-state networks by sad mood induction. PLoS One. 2008;3(3):e1794. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001794.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  126. Zuo XN, Kelly C, Di Martino A, Mennes M, Margulies DS, Bangaru S, et al. Growing together and growing apart: regional and sex differences in the lifespan developmental trajectories of functional homotopy. J Neurosci. 2010;30(45):15034–43. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-10.2010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  127. Reid HM, Norvilitis JM. Evidence for anomalous lateralization across domain in ADHD children as well as adults identified with the Wender Utah rating scale. J Psychiatr Res. 2000;34(4-5):311–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  128. Mahajan R, Dirlikov B, Crocetti D, Mostofsky SH. Motor circuit anatomy in children with autism spectrum disorder with or without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Autism Res. 2015. doi:10.1002/aur.1497.

    Google Scholar 

  129. Bolding MS, White DM, Hadley JA, Weiler M, Holcomb HH, Lahti AC. Antipsychotic drugs alter functional connectivity between the medial frontal cortex, hippocampus, and nucleus accumbens as measured by H215O PET. Front Psychiatry. 2012;3:105. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00105.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  130. Sambataro F, Blasi G, Fazio L, Caforio G, Taurisano P, Romano R, et al. Treatment with olanzapine is associated with modulation of the default mode network in patients with Schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(4):904–12. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.192.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  131. Lui S, Li T, Deng W, Jiang L, Wu Q, Tang H, et al. Short-term effects of antipsychotic treatment on cerebral function in drug-naive first-episode schizophrenia revealed by “resting state” functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(8):783–92. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  132. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S. Autism. Lancet. 2014;383(9920):896–910. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61539-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  133. Esbensen AJ, Greenberg JS, Seltzer MM, Aman MG. A longitudinal investigation of psychotropic and non-psychotropic medication use among adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(9):1339–49. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0750-3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  134. Oswald DP, Sonenklar NA. Medication use among children with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2007;17(3):348–55. doi:10.1089/cap.2006.17303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  135. Floris DL, Chura LR, Holt RJ, Suckling J, Bullmore ET, Baron-Cohen S, et al. Psychological correlates of handedness and corpus callosum asymmetry in autism: the left hemisphere dysfunction theory revisited. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(8):1758–72. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1720-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  136. Tian L, Wang J, Yan C, He Y. Hemisphere- and gender-related differences in small-world brain networks: a resting-state functional MRI study. Neuroimage. 2011;54(1):191–202. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.066.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  137. Jeste SS. The neurology of autism spectrum disorders. Curr Opin Neurol. 2011;24(2):132–9. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e3283446450.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  138. Hilton CL, Zhang Y, Whilte MR, Klohr CL, Constantino J. Motor impairment in sibling pairs concordant and discordant for autism spectrum disorders. Autism. 2012;16(4):430–41. doi:10.1177/1362361311423018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  139. Hardan AY, Minshew NJ, Keshavan MS. Corpus callosum size in autism. Neurology. 2000;55(7):1033–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  140. Frazier TW, Hardan AY. A meta-analysis of the corpus callosum in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(10):935–41. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.022.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  141. Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Kana RK, Minshew NJ. Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(4):951–61. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  142. Nydén A, Carlsson M, Carlsson A, Gillberg C. Interhemispheric transfer in high-functioning children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: a controlled pilot study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2004;46(7):448–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  143. Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Makris N, Filipek PA, Kemper TL, Normandin JJ, et al. Localization of white matter volume increase in autism and developmental language disorder. Ann Neurol. 2004;55(4):530–40. doi:10.1002/ana.20032.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  144. Hahamy A, Behrmann M, Malach R. The idiosyncratic brain: distortion of spontaneous connectivity patterns in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(2):302–9. doi:10.1038/nn.3919.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the families who participated in the study. This research was supported by grants from the Autism Speaks Foundation (#2506, #2384, #1739); the National Institutes of Health (R01 NS048527-08, R21 MH098228, R01 MH078160-06A1, P41 EB015909); the Clinical and Translational Research (ICTR) (UL1 TR 000424-06, UL1 TR 001079-03); and the Johns Hopkins Institute for Clinical and Translational Research, which is funded in part by grant numbers UL1 TR 000424-06 and UL1 TR 001079-03 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS). DLF was funded by Sidney Sussex College, Cambridge; the European Molecular Biology Organization (EMBO); the British Psychological Society; and the Ingelheim Boehringer Foundation. We would like to thank Tibor Auer and Jared Nielsen for the helpful discussion.

Authors’ contributions

DLF conducted the analyses and wrote the manuscript. ADB and MBN assisted in carrying out the analyses and revised the manuscript. MM conducted the psychological and behavioural assessments, acquired the data and revised the manuscript. DC acquired and provided the data and revised the manuscript. MCL, JS, SBC and JJP made conceptual suggestions and critically revised the manuscript. SHM designed and obtained funding for the study and edited the manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Competing interests

JJP serves as manager of the F.M. Kirby Research Center for Functional Brain Imaging, which receives research support from Philips HealthCare, which manufactured the MRI scanners used in this study. All other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study received ethical approval by the Johns Hopkins Medicine Institutional Review Board. Written consent was obtained from parents or a legal guardian, and assent was obtained from the participants.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dorothea L. Floris.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Group differences in LIs of individual DMN connections and group differences in LIs of individual visual network connections. (DOCX 83 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Floris, D.L., Barber, A.D., Nebel, M.B. et al. Atypical lateralization of motor circuit functional connectivity in children with autism is associated with motor deficits. Molecular Autism 7, 35 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-016-0096-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-016-0096-6

Keywords