Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Prevalence of suspected developmental delays in early infancy: results from a regional population-based longitudinal study

Abstract

Background

Prevalence estimates on suspected developmental delays (SDD) in young infants are scarce and a necessary first step for planning an early intervention. We investigated the prevalence of SDD at 4, 6 and 12 months, in addition to associations of SDD with gender, prematurity and maternal education.

Methods

This study is based on a Norwegian longitudinal sample of 1555 infants and their parents attending well-baby clinics for regular health check-ups. Moreover, parents completed the Norwegian translation of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires (ASQ) prior to the check-up, with a corrected gestational age being used to determine the time of administration for preterm infants. Scores ≤ the established cut-offs in one or more of the five development areas: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving and personal-social, which defined SDD for an infant were reported. Chi-square tests were performed for associations between the selected factors and SDD.

Results

According to established Norwegian cut-off points, the overall prevalence of SDD in one or more areas was 7.0 % (10.3 % US cut-off) at 4 months, 5.7 % (12.3 % US cut-off) at 6 months and 6.1 % (10.3 % US cut-off) at 12 months. The highest prevalence of SDD was in the gross motor area at all three time points. A gestational age of < 37 weeks revealed a significant association with the communication SDD at 4 months, and with the fine motor and personal social SDD at 6 months. Gender was significantly associated with the fine motor and problem solving SDD at 4 months and personal- social SDD at 6 months: as more boys than girls were delayed. No significant associations were found between maternal education and the five developmental areas of the ASQ.

Conclusion

Our findings indicate prevalence rates of SDD between 5.7 and 7.0 % in Norwegian infants between 4 and 12 months of age based on the Norwegian ASQ cut-off points (10.3–12.3 %, US cut-off points). During the first year of life, delay is most frequent within the gross motor area. Special attention should be paid to infants born prematurely, as well as to boys. Separate norms for boys and girls should be considered for the ASQ.

Peer Review reports

Background

Many studies have described the negative impacts of developmental delays in children, including emotional, behavioural and health problems later in life [1, 2], difficulties in parental child care and the parent-child relationship [3, 4], educational achievement [4, 5] and economic impacts on the families and societies [610]. Early identification and intervention for developmental delays cause an improvement in the successful functioning of affected children [1114]. Research has demonstrated that intervention programmes are cost-effective and may have lifelong benefits, and also that developmental attainment is maximized when intervention is started early [1115]. A necessary first step in order to plan for early intervention is estimation of prevalence of developmental delay and knowledge about the types of delays.

Estimates from the World Health Organization (WHO) indicate that 5 % of the world’s children under 15 years of age have some type of moderate to severe disability [16]. In the United States developmental disabilities occur in 15 % of children from 3 to 17 years of age [17]. In Norway and Scandinavia, data on the developmental status on children is scarce and the few published studies of children below school age show divergent results, varying from 6.3 % to 33 % [18, 19]. Developmental screening programmes have been shown to improve the identification and referral of children who have possible delays [2022]. One of the validated screening tools recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics is the Ages and Stages Questionnaires (ASQ) [23], which is a parent-completed tool for identifying infants and young children at risk for developmental delays. To date, no such recommendation exists in Norway and the Scandinavian countries, however, a Norwegian translation of the ASQ 2nd edition with a Norwegian reference (N ref.) sample has been available since 2003 [18]. The public health system in Norway provides free medical, mental and dental services for all children and youth from 0–18, and close to 100 % of parents with young infants come regularly to local well-baby clinics from birth and up to 5 years of age for weight control, vaccination and a developmental check-up of their infant [24]. Check-ups and developmental monitoring in the well-baby clinic are primarily done by public health nurses and a general practitioner (GP). Both the monitoring and check-ups are essentially based on clinical judgement and not on the use of standardized screening or assessment tools. No official definition exists regarding who is eligible for early intervention at the primary care level; thus the health providers’ clinical judgement, in combination with parent concerns, are the primary drivers for this decision.

If specialist services are needed, the local GP has to make a formal referral and get written consent from the child’s parents. Even so, there is a growing amount of interest for screening tools for developmental delay by professionals in primary care. Without accurate prevalence data based on standardized instruments, it is difficult for primary health care to adequately plan the necessary assessment and intervention responses. A lack of estimates on developmental delays among infants and children also has provided an unclear picture for policy-makers for a decision to provide early intervention services, as well as for planning and estimating the costs of early social, medical and educational intervention programmes. Hence, there is a pressing need for empirical data on knowledge about the prevalence of children at risk of developmental delay in Norway. This study seeks to contribute to building a more comprehensive picture of young infants’ developmental status.

Child development is influenced by bio-medical and socio-cultural factors that are in a continuous interaction [25]. A number of risk factors associated with an increased risk for developmental delay have been identified, including child gender, gestational age and the mothers’ educational level. Predictors of developmental delays can be useful in estimating the potential for delayed development in the population, in addition to providing an opportunity to create environments that support optimal development. The aim of this study was to estimate prevalence rates of SDD among infants at 4, 6 and 12 months of age based on parent-completed ASQ, and to investigate associations of SDD with gender, gestational age < 37 weeks and maternal education.

Methods

Participants

This study is based on a Norwegian population-based prospective cohort study on children’s early development from birth to two years of age. Recruitment took place between May 2011 and May 2012, and the participants were recruited from all existing well-baby clinics in five municipalities, both in urban and rural areas. Every expectant or new mother who came to these clinics was invited to participate in the study by a mid-wife or a public health nurse at their first consultation, either during pregnancy or soon after birth. The study had no specific exclusion criteria since the well-baby clinics offer services to all families with children below 5 years who live in the municipality. Mothers of 1555 children and their partners consented to participate (88.5 %). In > 95 % of cases, it was the mother who completed the ASQ. Mothers who did not consent to participate in the study differed from participating mothers in terms of having a lower educational level (p < 0.001) and higher proportion of non-Scandinavian speaking mothers (p < 0.001). No significant differences were found in gender, birth weight and gestational age between participating and non-participating children.

The current study reports on infant developmental status at 4, 6 and 12 months. The number of infants with a parent-completed ASQ form for each assessment point varied (4 months: n = 1244, 6 months: n = 1192 and 12 months: n = 832). The background characteristics of the study population from each assessment point are summarized in Table 1. One of the municipalities with four well-baby clinics did not collect ASQ information on the children at the 12-months consultation due to time restrictions at this particular consultation, which is the primary reason for the low number of ASQ data at 12 months.

Table 1 Characteristics of the study population

Procedure

The public health nurse or midwife provided written and oral information about the study to the parents based on procedures approved by the Norwegian Regional Committee for Medical and Health Ethics, and parents who volunteered gave their written consent to participate. On enrolment or at the first check-up after birth, background information data such as educational level, civil status, child’s gender, gestational age, and birth weight were collected and recorded. The ASQ was mailed to the participants’ home address two weeks before the 4, 6 or 12 months well-baby clinic visit. For infants born prematurely, the corrected age was used when completing the questionnaires [23]. The parents brought with them the completed ASQ to the scheduled appointment and the information on the ASQ was included as part of the overall clinical evaluation process that took place together with the parents and their child. All parents with ASQ screen positive infants were offered further evaluations of their child within two weeks, as well as referrals to specialist care in severe cases.

Measures

The infants’ development was assessed by the Norwegian version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire, 2nd edition [18, 23], at 4, 6 and 12 month. The ASQ is a parent-completed, developmental screening instrument, and consists of 21 age-specific questionnaires intended for use from the age of 2 months to 60 months [26]. Each questionnaire in the ASQ consists of 30 items covering five areas: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving, and personal-social. Sum scores for the 6 ASQ areas were computed when all ASQ items were valid. Parents were asked to evaluate whether their child had achieved a milestone (“yes”, 10 points), had partly achieved a milestone (“sometimes”, 5 points) or had not yet achieved a milestone (“not yet”, 0 points). Each area total score is compared to a cut-off score. A child who obtains one or more area scores at or below the established cut-off levels is per definition suspected of developmental delay and should be referred for further evaluation. According to the US manual for ASQ, children who score 2 SD or more below average are considered of a suspected delay [26]. The ASQ may be used in a variety of settings (mail, online, telephone, interview, home visit, office of child care or physician) and both as parent reported and reported by health professionals [26]. The original ASQ has been proven to be a valid and reliable screening test, even in its translated and culturally adapted versions in several studies in different populations of children [2732]. According to the Norwegian manual, the cut-off is primarily based on the 2nd percentile [18]. A construct validation study based on the N ref.sample confirmed the Norwegian ASQ version as an effective diagnostic tool of developmental delay [28]. Because no Norwegian concurrent validation study has been published, we decided to present prevalence data based on both the Norwegian and US cut-off scores.

Data analysis

The summary of the data is presented as frequencies and percentages. The associations of SDD at 4, 6 and 12 months with gender, a gestational age of < 37 weeks and maternal education were investigated by chi-square tests. The level of significance was set at 0.05, and the data were analysed using the Statistical Package for Social Science (SPSS) software package version 22 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).

Results

Complete ASQ scores were available for 1244 of the participants at 4 months, 1192 at 6 months and 832 at 12 months. The characteristics of the participating children and their mothers at 4, 6 and 12 months are presented in Table 1. The mothers’ age at the three time point ranged from 17–44, with a mean age of 30.

Table 2 shows the proportion of infants with SDD according to the Norwegian and US cut-off points in the five developmental ASQ areas at 4, 6 and 12 months.

Table 2 The percentage of infants scoring at or below the Norwegian (N ref.) and US cut-off values at 4, 6 and 12 months

As shown in Table 2, the overall prevalence of infants scoring at or below the cut-off points of at least one developmental area according to the Norwegian cut-off points was 7.0 % at 4 months (10.3 % according to the US cut-off), 5.7 % at 6 months (10.3 % by the US cut-off), and 6.1 % at 12 months (12.3 % by the US cut-off). The percentage of infants with SDD in the communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving and social-personal areas varied between 1.1 and 2.6 % at 4 months, 0.6 and 2.3 % at 6 months and 0.4 and 3.6 % at 12 months by the Norwegian cut-off scores. The highest prevalence was found in the gross motor area in all three age groups, 2.6 % at 4 months, 2.3 % at 6 months and 3.6 % at 12 months. We also found that 1.8 % of infants with complete ASQ scores had a delay in more than one area at 4 months, 1.1 and 0.8 % at 6 and 12 months, respectively.

Table 3 shows the associations of gestational age < 37 weeks, gender, maternal education with developmental delay for each area and age groups. Gender was significantly associated with fine motor area (p = 0.029) and problem solving area (p =0.010) at 4 months and personal-social area at 6 months (p = 0.013), with a higher prevalence of SDD among boys. Gestational age of < 37 weeks was significantly associated with delay in the communication area (p = 0.001) at 4 months and the fine motor (p = 0.049) and personal-social area (p <0.001) at 6 months. Maternal education had no significant associations with the areas of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire in any age group.

Table 3 Association between gender, gestational age, maternal education and the area of ASQ

Discussion

The aim of this study was to estimate the prevalence rates of SDD in a community sample of infants at 4, 6 and 12 months of age based on their ASQ scores in five developmental areas, as well as the associations of SDD with gender, prematurity (a gestational age of < 37 weeks) and maternal education. The results suggest that between 5.7 to 7.0 % of young infants between 4 and 12 months had SDD according to the Norwegian ASQ cut-off points, and between 10.3 to 12.3 % according to the US cut-off points. The majority of these had an indication of delays in one area only, most frequently in the gross motor area. Prematurity was significantly associated with SDD in the communication area at 4 months and fine motor and personal-social areas at 6 months. Significant associations were found between gender and the fine motor and problem solving areas at 4 months and the personal-social area at 6 months.

Previous studies have shown substantial variations in the prevalence of developmental delay. A number of methodological issues make it difficult to compare available prevalence rates, such as differences in case definition and criteria, type of measures used, variations in age and whether the studies report on low or high risk populations. Prevalences of developmental delay based on the National Health Interview Surveys (NHIS-CH), which is a parent completed questionnaire on development disability, reported that 15 % of US children between 3 and 17 years had a developmental disability [17]. The Health Intervention Survey (NHIS-D) on Disabilities reported that 3.4 % of all children had general developmental delays and 3.3 % had functional developmental delays among American children between 4 and 59 months [33]. A nationally representative longitudinal sample in the US showed that almost 13 % of the infants who were objectively measured by the Bayley Short Form-Research Edition at 9 and 24 month had developmental delays [34].

The results from the current study were based on a Norwegian version of the ASQ, and the data was gathered from well-baby clinics where almost the entire population of parents with young infants came regularly with their child for a developmental check-up. ASQ was implemented in all nine well-baby clinics in five municipalities with the intention of standardizing the general developmental monitoring and check-up by public health nurses, engaging parents as active partners and increasing the detection rate of infants at risk for SDD. Parents brought the completed ASQ form along to the 4, 6 and 12- month check-ups. To the best of our knowledge, few prevalence estimates exist of SDD based on parent-completed ASQ data collected in a primary care setting. Two studies yielded ASQ data collected from preventive health care clinics in the Netherlands. Prevalence rates for 4-year-old full-term children in the first study were 7.2 % for children with low sosio-economic status (SES), 4.8 % for intermediate and 2.8 % for high SES children [35]. The second study reported prevalence rates for full-term and moderate preterm children (43–49 months), at 4.2 % and 8.3 %, respectively [36]. The prevalence rates from these two studies are in line with the findings in the present study, which indicates prevalence rates between 5.7 and 7.0 % (Norwegian cut-off points, and between 10.3 and 13.3 % for US cut-off points). Other studies among younger children report higher rates of SDD measured with ASQ than our study [18, 19, 3740]. A prevalence of 27 % was found in a well-child clinic among American children from 9 to 31 months [38], while another study reported a prevalence rate of 28.8 % among 9, 18 and 30 month old children who attended an ambulatory well-baby clinic in Chile [40]. However, the Norwegian ASQ normative sample reported prevalence rates of 10.3 % at 4 months, 11.8 % at 6 months and 11.6 % at 12 months [18], and a more recent Norwegian population-based study of 6 month old infants from the capital of Norway found that approximately every third infant obtained an ASQ score at or below the cut-off scores in at least one area according to the Norwegian and US recommended cut-off scores [19]. The Norwegian ASQ normative sample had a relatively small sample size in each age group, and unlike the present study, the participants in both the previous Norwegian studies received an invitation letter, completed the ASQ at home and returned it by mail to the researchers without any feedback. The prevalence rate found in the study by Alvik and Grøholt is unexpectedly high, especially when taking into consideration that infants with a birth weight below 2.5 kg and mothers with non-Scandinavian ethnicity were excluded from the study. In addition, none of the 14 pictograms in the ASQ 6-month questionnaire were included in the ASQ form that the parents were asked to complete at home. [19]. This might possibly have contributed to misinterpretations of the meaning of items and thus an incorrect response.

Developmental delay in one area is often found to be correlated with delay in other areas [41, 42], but being late in one isolated area only is associated with less risk for the child [43]. In our community sample, we found that only 1.8 % of the infants had SDD in more than one area at 4 months, 1.1 % at 6 months and 0.8 % at 12 months. The highest prevalence rate in the present study was found in the gross motor area during the first year of life, 2.6 % at 4 months, 2.3 % at 6 months and 3.6 % at 12 months. This was also the case in the Norwegian reference sample at 12 months (5.5 %) [18]. Still these gross motor prevalences are considerably lower than studies on young children from the US and other countries [26, 33, 39]. Motor development may differ in rate and sequence among infants and children from various cultural backgrounds [44]. It is also well recognized that the development of gross motor skills during early childhood is of paramount relevance for a child’s overall development [45], and a developmental delay in the ASQ motor area in early life has been found to predict later communication [41] and cognitive skills [42]. Several factors affect motor development among children, such as a child’s characteristics (e.g. gender, age, ethnicity and somatic conditions), child-rearing practices, parental/social expectations and the quality and quantity of stimulation provided in home [46]. For example, the caregivers’ attitude and encouragement toward an infant’s tummy time or floor time might be related to the child’s motor performance. Parents in Norway have 12 months leave and spend most of the day together with their infants, and the public health nurses encourage parents to stimulate their infant’s motor development in the first year of life. This may well have contributed to the relatively low prevalence rates in our sample.

Significant associations were found between gender and developmental delay in the fine motor and problem solving area at 4 months, and at the personal-social area at 6 months, with lower mean scores for boys in all areas. The finding that boys have a significantly higher rate of delay is in accordance with other studies of gender differences in preschoolers [28, 33, 47, 48]. On average, Richter and Janson showed that the developmental stage for girls in a Norwegian population was higher than for boys in all ASQ areas, except for gross motor function, in which no significant differences were detected [28]. The gender differences found in this study correspond with the results from previous research, therefore it seems preferable to develop norms for the Norwegian version of the ASQ separately for boys and girls in order to avoid false-positive classifications of boys in further assessments and interventions.

Premature birth (<37 gestation weeks) was associated with a delay in the communication area at 4 months, and the fine motor and personal-social area at 6 months. Developmental delays are common in preterm children and the risk increases with a decreasing geatation age (GA) [36, 49], which can be explained by the developmental stage of the central nervous system at birth [50]. Evidence from neuroscience shows that microstructural and neural connectivity processes are disturbed because of prematurity, and these disturbances may result in an atypical differentiation of neuronal pathways [51]. Premature birth was reflected by a delay in all five ASQ areas in the Norwegian study by Richter and Janson, although these negative consequences were seemingly more pronounced within the fine motor skills, problem solving skills and personal-social skills than the other areas [28]. Kerstjens and colleagues also found that both moderate and early preterm children measured with ASQ at 4 years of age had more frequent problems with fine motor, communication and personal-social functioning compared to their 4 year old peers born full-term [36].

Maternal education had no significant associations with the areas of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire in this study, in contrast to previous reported findings of the impact of maternal education upon child development [28, 31, 52]. There may be several explanations for why maternal education was not significantly related to infant development in this study. Firstly, this study was based on a Norwegian community sample, with a relatively high education level among the parents. Furthermore, our study was conducted on young infants between 4 and 12 months of age. In this early stage of development, biomedical factors may have a greater impact on development than the parents’ educational level. In addition, the Norwegian society also provides a highly stable and comprehensive social, financial and health care network that protects mother and babies to a high degree.

Prevalence estimates on SDD in young infants are scarce and a necessary first step in order to plan for early intervention. This study contributes to building a more comprehensive picture of young Norwegian infants’ developmental status. The sample is population-based with a relatively large sample size, and the ASQ is performed in a naturalistic setting in accordance with the recommended use of the instrument [26] . There were no exclusion criteria for participation in the study, but the families who did not want to participate differed from the participating parents in terms of having a lower educational level and a higher proportion of non-Scandinavian-speaking parents. This may have biased our results to some extent, but we did not find significant relationships between the mother’s education level and the child’s ASQ scores. There were no significant differences between the participating and non-participating children at the time of inclusion in the study. However, there was a reduction in the proportion of low birth weight infants with a completed ASQ from 4 to 12 months of age, which may have influenced the results and reduced the estimated prevalence of SDD. The Norwegian version of the ASQ was used and the Norwegian ASQ items are well translated and back-translated; thus, there is little probability of translation distortion [18]. It would have been preferable if a concurrent Norwegian validation of ASQ was available, but no such validation yet exists. Hence, the results of SDD among 4–12 month old infants in Norway must be interpreted with some caution.

Conclusion

The current study contributes to a limited knowledge base regarding the prevalence of infants at risk for developmental delay. This large, representative regional population-based sample suggests a prevalence rate of SDD between 5.7 and 7.0 % among infants between 4 and 12 months of age based on the Norwegian cut-off points (10.3–12.3 % according to US cut-off points). During the first year of life, delays are most frequently reported within the motor area. Special attention should be paid to infants born prematurely and to boys, and separate norms for boys and girls should be considered for the ASQ.

Abbreviations

ASQ:

Ages and Stages Questionnaires

N ref:

the Norwegian reference sample

DD:

Developmental delay

GA:

Gestational age

GP:

General practitioner

References

  1. Baker BL, McIntyre L, Blacher J, Crnic K, Edelbrock C, Low C. Pre-school children with and without developmental delay: Behaviour problems and parenting stress over time. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2003;47(4-5):217–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Emerson E, Einfeld S. Emotional and behavioural difficulties in young children with and without developmental delay: a bi-national perspective. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(5):583–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Brown MA, McIntyre LL, Crnic KA, Baker BL, Blacher J. Preschool Children with and without Developmental Delay: Risk, Parenting, and Child Demandingess. J Ment Health Res Intellect Disabil. 2011;4(3):206–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Blanchard LT, Gurka MJ, Blackman JA. Emotional, developmental, and behavioral health of American children and their families: a report from the 2003 National Survey of Children’s Health. Pediatrics. 2006;117(6):e1202–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Boyle CA, Decoufle P, Yeargin-Allsopp M. Prevalence and health impact of developmental disabilities in US children. Pediatrics. 1994;93(3):399–403.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Chan E, Zhan C, Homer CJ. Health care use and costs for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: national estimates from the medical expenditure panel survey. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156(5):504–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Newacheck PW, Kim SE. A national profile of health care utilization and expenditures for children with special health care needs. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159(1):10–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Quach J, Oberklaid F, Gold L, Lucas N, Mensah FK, Wake M. Primary health-care costs associated with special health care needs up to age 7 years: Australian population-based study. J Paediatr Child Health. 2014;50(10):768–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Sciberras E, Westrupp EM, Wake M, Nicholson JM, Lucas N, Mensah F, et al. Healthcare costs associated with language difficulties up to 9 years of age: Australian population-based study. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2015;17(1):41–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Westrupp EM, Lucas N, Mensah FK, Gold L, Wake M, Nicholson JM. Community-based healthcare costs for children born low birthweight, preterm and/or small for gestational age: data from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. Child Care Health Dev. 2014;40(2):259–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Manning M, Homel R, Smith C. A meta-analysis of the effects of early developmental prevention programs in at-risk populations on non-health outcomes in adolescence. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2010;32(4):506–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Anderson LM, Shinn C, Fullilove MT, Scrimshaw SC, Fielding JE, Normand J, et al. The effectiveness of early childhood development programs. A systematic review. Am J Prev Med. 2003;24(3 Suppl):32–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. McCormick MC, Brooks-Gunn J, Buka SL, Goldman J, Yu J, Salganik M, et al. Early intervention in low birth weight premature infants: results at 18 years of age for the Infant Health and Development Program. Pediatrics. 2006;117(3):771–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Spittle AJ, Orton J, Doyle LW, Boyd R. Early developmental intervention programs post hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairments in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2007;2:Cd005495.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Reynolds AJ, Temple JA, White BA, Ou SR, Robertson DL. Age 26 cost-benefit analysis of the child-parent center early education program. Child Dev. 2011;82(1):379–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. World Health Organization. The global burden of disease: 2004 update. Geneva: WHO; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Boyle CA, Boulet S, Schieve LA, Cohen RA, Blumberg SJ, Yeargin-Allsopp M, et al. Trends in the prevalence of developmental disabilities in US children, 1997-2008. Pediatrics. 2011;127(6):1034–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Janson H, Smith L. Norsk manualsupplement til “Ages and stages questionnaires”. Oslo: R.BUP, Regionsenter for barne- og ungdomspsykiatri, Helseregion Øst/Sør; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Alvik A, Grøholt B. Examination of the cut-off scores determined by the Ages and Stages Questionnaire in a population-based sample of 6 month-old Norwegian infants. BMC Pediatr. 2011;11:117.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Hix-Small H, Marks K, Squires J, Nickel R. Impact of implementing developmental screening at 12 and 24 months in a pediatric practice. Pediatrics. 2007;120(2):381–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Schonwald A, Huntington N, Chan E, Risko W, Bridgemohan C. Routine developmental screening implemented in urban primary care settings: more evidence of feasibility and effectiveness. Pediatrics. 2009;123(2):660–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Guevara JP, Gerdes M, Localio R, Huang YV, Pinto-Martin J, Minkovitz CS, et al. Effectiveness of developmental screening in an urban setting. Pediatrics. 2013;131(1):30–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Squires J, Potter L, Bricker D. The ASQ User’s guide. 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Sosial- og helsedirektoratet. Kommunenes helsefremmende og forebyggende arbeid i helsestasjons- og skolehelsetjenesten: veileder til forskrift av 3. April 2003 nr 450. Oslo: Sosial- og helsedirektoratet; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Sameroff A. A unified theory of development: a dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Dev. 2010;81(1):6–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Squires J, Twombly E, Bricker D, Potter L. The ASQ-3 User’s guide. 3rd ed. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Klamer A, Lando A, Pinborg A, Greisen G. Ages and Stages Questionnaire used to measure cognitive deficit in children born extremely preterm. Acta Paediatr. 2005;94(9):1327–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Richter J, Janson H. A validation study of the Norwegian version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires. Acta Paediatr. 2007;96(5):748–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Elbers J, Macnab A, McLeod E, Gagnon F. The Ages and Stages Questionnaires: feasibility of use as a screening tool for children in Canada. Can J Rural Med. 2008;13(1):9–14.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Heo KH, Squires J, Yovanoff P. Cross-cultural adaptation of a pre-school screening instrument: comparison of Korean and US populations. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2008;52(Pt 3):195–206.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Kerstjens JM, Bos AF, ten Vergert EM, de Meer G, Butcher PR, Reijneveld SA. Support for the global feasibility of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire as developmental screener. Early Hum Dev. 2009;85(7):443–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kapci EG, Kucuker S, Uslu RI. How Applicable Are Ages and Stages Questionnaires for Use With Turkish Children? Topics Early Child Spec Educ. 2010;30(3):176–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Simpson GA, Colpe L, Greenspan S. Measuring functional developmental delay in infants and young children: prevalence rates from the NHIS-D. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2003;17(1):68–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Rosenberg SA, Zhang D, Robinson CC. Prevalence of developmental delays and participation in early intervention services for young children. Pediatrics. 2008;121(6):e1503–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Potijk MR, Kerstjens JM, Bos AF, Reijneveld SA, de Winter AF. Developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children with low socioeconomic status: risks multiply. J Pediatr. 2013;163(5):1289–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Kerstjens JM, de Winter AF, Bocca-Tjeertes IF, ten Vergert EM, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children at school entry. J Pediatr. 2011;159(1):92–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Rydz D, Srour M, Oskoui M, Marget N, Shiller M, Birnbaum R, et al. Screening for developmental delay in the setting of a community pediatric clinic: a prospective assessment of parent-report questionnaires. Pediatrics. 2006;118(4):e1178–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Sices L, Stancin T, Kirchner L, Bauchner H. PEDS and ASQ developmental screening tests may not identify the same children. Pediatrics. 2009;124(4):e640–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Wei QW, Zhang JX, Scherpbier RW, Zhao CX, Luo SS, Wang XL, Guo SF. High prevalence of developmental delay among children under three years of age in poverty-stricken areas of China. Public Health In Press.

  40. Schonhaut L, Armijo I, Schonstedt M, Alvarez J, Cordero M. Validity of the ages and stages questionnaires in term and preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2013;131(5):e1468–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Wang MV, Lekhal R, Aaro LE, Schjolberg S. Co-occurring development of early childhood communication and motor skills: results from a population-based longitudinal study. Child Care Health Dev. 2014;40(1):77–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Piek JP, Dawson L, Smith LM, Gasson N. The role of early fine and gross motor development on later motor and cognitive ability. Hum Mov Sci. 2008;27(5):668–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Valtonen R, Ahonen T, Lyytinen P, Tolvanen A. Screening for developmental risks at 4 years of age: Predicting development two years later. Nord Psychol. 2007;59(2):95–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Mayson TA, Harris SR, Bachman CL. Gross motor development of Asian and European children on four motor assessments: a literature review. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2007;19(2):148–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Piek JP, Hands B, Licari MK. Assessment of motor functioning in the preschool period. Neuropsychol Rev. 2012;22(4):402–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Cools W, De Martelaer K, Samaey C, Andries C. Fundamental movement skill performance of preschool children in relation to family context. J Sports Sci. 2011;29(7):649–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Westerlund M. Communicative skills in relation to gender, birth order, childcare and socioeconomic status in 18-month-old children. Scand J Psychol. 2005;46(6):485–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Rydell AM, Diamantopoulou S, Thorell LB, Bohlin G. Hyperactivity, shyness, and sex: development and socio-emotional functioning. Br J Dev Psychol. 2009;27(Pt 3):625–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Woythaler MA, McCormick MC, Smith VC. Late preterm infants have worse 24-month neurodevelopmental outcomes than term infants. Pediatrics. 2011;127(3):e622–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Volpe JJ. Brain injury in premature infants: a complex amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8(1):110–24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Lubsen J, Vohr B, Myers E, Hampson M, Lacadie C, Schneider KC, et al. Microstructural and functional connectivity in the developing preterm brain. Semin Perinatol. 2011;35(1):34–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Seguin L, Xu Q, Gauvin L, Zunzunegui MV, Potvin L, Frohlich KL. Understanding the dimensions of socioeconomic status that influence toddlers’ health: unique impact of lack of money for basic needs in Quebec’s birth cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005;59(1):42–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are indebted to all the participating families, and grateful to the staff at the well-baby clinics in Hamar, Løten, Tønsberg, Nøtterøy and Larvik.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisbeth Valla.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

LV: Responsibility for the study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation, and in the writing of the manuscript. KS: Primary responsibility for the study design, Participated in the analytic framework of the study, with the data interpretation, and in the writing of the manuscript. DH: Has been involved in the statistical analysis, and in critically revising the manuscript for important intellectual content. TWL: Was involved in and supervised the statistical analysis. All the authors have given their final approval of the final version of the manuscript.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Norwegian (N.ref.)and US cut-off values at 4, 6 and 12 months Description of dataset- Shows the recommended cut-off scores in Norway and US at 4, 6 and 12 months. (PDF 20 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Valla, L., Wentzel-Larsen, T., Hofoss, D. et al. Prevalence of suspected developmental delays in early infancy: results from a regional population-based longitudinal study. BMC Pediatr 15, 215 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-015-0528-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-015-0528-z

Keywords