Skip to main content

The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load

Abstract

Background

Research on the neural bases of cognitive deficits in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has shown that working memory (WM) difficulties are associated with abnormalities in the prefrontal cortex. However, cognitive load impacts these findings, and no studies have examined the relation between WM load and neural underpinnings in children with ASD. Thus, the current study determined the effects of cognitive load on WM, using a visuo-spatial WM capacity task in children with and without ASD with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).

Methods

We used fMRI and a 1-back colour matching task (CMT) task with four levels of difficulty to compare the cortical activation patterns associated with WM in children (7–13 years old) with high functioning autism (N = 19) and matched controls (N = 17) across cognitive load.

Results

Performance on CMT was comparable between groups, with the exception of one difficulty level. Using linear trend analyses, the control group showed increasing activation as a function of difficulty level in frontal and parietal lobes, particularly between the highest difficulty levels, and decreasing activation as a function of difficulty level in the posterior cingulate and medial frontal gyri. In contrast, children with ASD showed increasing activation only in posterior brain regions and decreasing activation in the posterior cingulate and medial frontal gyri, as a function of difficulty level. Significant differences were found in the precuneus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and medial premotor cortex, where control children showed greater positive linear relations between cortical activity and task difficulty level, particularly at the highest difficulty levels, but children with ASD did not show these trends.

Conclusions

Children with ASD showed differences in activation in the frontal and parietal lobes—both critical substrates for visuo-spatial WM. Our data suggest that children with ASD rely mainly on posterior brain regions associated with visual and lower level processing, whereas controls showed activity in frontal lobes related to the classic WM network. Findings will help guide future work by localizing areas of vulnerability to developmental disturbances.

Background

Approximately 1 in 88 children have an autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and the proportion of children being diagnosed is rising [1]. ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder classically characterized by social deficits, communicative difficulties and repetitive behaviours [2], with evidence of cognitive and executive function impairment [2–9]. The impaired executive processes may account for many of the profound behavioural manifestations in ASD and contribute to autistic symptomology [6, 7]. Emerging literature on cognitive difficulties in ASD, as well as their neural underpinnings, provides evidence for working memory (WM) deficits that are associated with frontal lobe abnormalities, particularly in prefrontal cortical activity [4, 10–14].

Previous neuroimaging, electrophysiology and neurochemical studies in ASD have identified atypical white and grey matter volumes [15–18], functional connectivity [10, 19], cortical sulcal and gyral anatomy [20], brain lateralization [21], neural perfusion [22] and serotonin synthesis capacity [23] compared to those of typically developing (TD) individuals, with the frontal cortex implicated in a number of these differences [24]. Although there is substantial evidence for developmental anatomical abnormalities of the frontal lobes in ASD, associations with cognitive performance are lacking [25]. It remains to be determined if executive dysfunction in ASD is related to functional as well as anatomical frontal lobe aberrations. Further, protracted maturation of the frontal lobes makes the functions they support, such as WM, susceptible to developmental disturbances [26, 27] but amenable to therapeutic interventions.

WM allows for the temporary storage, rehearsal and maintenance of information. It is important for learning, social ability [28], academic achievement [29] and many complex cognitive operations [30, 31]. Impairments in WM have been reported in ASD, yet our understanding of its development and neural correlates is still very limited in this population. We investigated the neural systems underlying visuo-spatial WM capacity using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to determine the role of cognitive load on possible functional differences between children with and without ASD. Cognitive load is a multidimensional construct referring to the processing resources that performing a particular task imposes on one's cognitive system [32]—in the present study, the level of WM task difficulty was used as a measure of cognitive load.

Extant research on the behavioural characterization of WM function in ASD suggests both intact [33–37] WM performance on simple memory tasks, and impaired [33–36, 38] performance on more complex tasks, relative to TD individuals. Discrepancies in the literature may be due partly to methodological inconsistencies regarding task choice, comparison groups or population age. Basic WM abilities are intact in high-functioning ASD (see [39] for review). The majority of studies that found significant group differences assessed WM using tasks with increased complexity and/or cognitive load and thereby imposed heavier demands on WM and executive functions [9, 34, 38]. Further, there is evidence of primarily visuo-spatial WM impairment, whereas verbal WM appears relatively intact in individuals with ASD [35–37].

Only a few studies have examined the neural correlates of WM function in ASD, and no neuroimaging studies of pre-adolescent children exist. Overall, WM processes are largely subserved by the prefrontal and parietal cortices [4, 40–44]. Converging literature identifies a broad system of prefrontal, premotor, dorsal cingulate and posterior parietal activation in visual WM tasks see [44], and neuroimaging studies in ASD provide evidence for atypical activity in these regions [10, 12, 14]. Using an oculomotor visuo-spatial WM task, Luna and colleagues [12] found behavioural impairments in WM, as well as reduced activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and posterior cingulate cortex in adults with ASD compared to controls. Interestingly, there was no evidence of impaired activation in other areas known to support WM neural circuitry, such as the anterior cingulate cortex. A study by Koshino et al. [10] demonstrated bilateral activation in the DLPFC in adult controls, whereas adults with ASD showed limited activation in the left, and more right hemisphere recruitment of prefrontal regions, despite the absence of differences in performance accuracy on an n-back letter task. The authors suggested that while typical adults processed letter stimuli using verbal codes, those with ASD employed visual strategies [10], supporting the idea that WM deficits in ASD may be attributed to less efficient processing strategies. In the only study of adolescents, Silk et al. [14] used a visuo-spatial WM mental rotation task, where behavioural performance was similar for ASD and control groups, and observed impaired cortical activation in the frontal lobes in the ASD group, including the anterior cingulate, DLPFC and caudate nucleus, but normal activation in the parietal cortices relative to controls. These findings suggest dysfunctional frontostriatal networks in ASD. More work is needed to understand the neural correlates and developmental trajectory of WM in childhood in ASD, as limited research is available reporting on pre-adolescent children.

One of the most common experimental paradigms used to manipulate cognitive load in studying WM is the ‘n-back’ protocol [10, 41, 43–47]. In a typical n-back task, participants view a series of stimuli and indicate whether the currently presented stimulus matches one presented ‘n’ (e.g., 0, 1, 2 or 3) trials prior. As difficulty level increases, the number of interfering stimuli between the target and relevant stimulus increases, requiring the utilization of different mental strategies at each level (e.g. 0-back, recognition; 1-back, maintenance; 2-back, maintenance and monitoring). This manipulation consequently increases both memory load and executive function demand (i.e. strategy needed to solve the task) in a non-linear fashion from one level to the next, making function-specific changes difficult to quantify and link with brain areas. Therefore, we used a 1-back colour matching task (CMT) [48] which systematically manipulated memory load while keeping executive function constant across all difficulty levels, allowing a direct investigation of the influence of cognitive load on WM. Specifically, executive schemes (i.e. procedural strategies for solving the task) are constant across levels of items in CMT; what varies with each level is the number of relevant items (colours) to be remembered. Difficulty level was parametrically graded based on behavioural age-dependent growth patterns observed using this task in previous work [48]. These observations point to a linear pattern of WM development, which may also be evident in the neural processes across increasing load. Our task is novel, and it captures the brain regions associated with this linear pattern of function across cognitive load in pre-adolescent children. In a previous study from our group, typical adults showed positive linear relations between cortical activity and CMT task difficulty level in areas involved in WM function [49]. Negative linear relations were found in areas typically associated with the default mode network (DMN). The DMN, which has been found in a wide range of neuroimaging studies, is a network of brain regions, including the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate and inferior parietal lobules, characterized by decreased activation during goal-oriented or attention-demanding tasks [50]. Neurodevelopmental disorders, including ASD, have been associated with abnormal function [51, 52] and structure [53] of the DMN that may interfere with cognitive function.

The present study identified and compared the neural activity underlying visuo-spatial WM capacity in children with and without ASD using fMRI, with manipulation of cognitive load. Given the increasing difficulty levels of the current task, we hypothesized that children with ASD would show poorer performance than matched TD controls on behavioural measures of WM. Further, frontal and posterior parietal cortical areas related to visuo-spatial WM capacity would be under-recruited in children with ASD relative to TD controls, and this difference would increase with load. We expected activity to be linearly modulated (positively for WM areas, negatively in default-mode areas) by task difficulty in all children (comparable to previous work with adults), but these trends would be less marked in children with ASD, particularly at higher cognitive loads.

With the protracted maturation of the frontal lobes, and their susceptibility to developmental anomalies, understanding development in these regions is crucial, particularly in populations with frontal lobe abnormalities, as seen in ASD. As previous research has focused on studying WM in adults and adolescents with ASD, there is a critical need to study WM in pre-adolescents with ASD, who exhibit both cognitive and neurological differences. Given the links between social function, school success and executive function ability, investigating the neural bases of WM deficits in children with ASD will contribute to our knowledge of the underlying causes of ASD-related behaviour. Previous research highlights the importance of investigating cognitive impairments in ASD that may arise from brain abnormalities and drive behavioural symptoms [54]. Further, exploring WM correlates will supplement behavioural phenotypes of ASD. This work will help identify the nature of atypical brain development, with future expectations of establishing age-appropriate interventions that can effectively target WM function and, in turn, other symptoms of ASD.

Methods

Participants

Seventy-three participants were recruited for this study: 42 children aged 7–13 years with high-functioning ASD, and 31 7–13-year-old TD control children. However, 9 TD children and 23 children with ASD were excluded from the analyses for excessive movement and inadequate task performance or protocol completion. After sex-, IQ- and age-matching, the study sample consisted of 19 children with ASD (3 girls and 16 boys) and 17 controls (4 girls and 13 boys). The groups were matched for age (ASD M = 11.05, SD = 1.43; TD M = 11.12, SD = 2.00; t(34) = 0.11, ns.), sex (χ2(1) = 0.34, ns.) and full-scale IQ as determined by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-II [55] (ASD M = 109.42, SD = 15.72; TD M = 115.35, SD = 9.27; t(34) = 1.36, ns.). We substituted the group average IQ for one TD child whose data were missing.

Exclusion criteria for all participants were the presence of any current significant axis I psychiatric comorbidities [2], neurological disorders, medical illnesses, prematurity, uncorrected vision, colour blindness, IQ < 70 as well as standard MRI contraindicators (e.g. ferromagnetic implants). A history of developmental delay, learning disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) was used to exclude TD children only; however, these factors were also not current primary diagnoses in the ASD group. Six children with ASD were each on one psychotropic medication (Strattera, Biphentin, Fluoxetine, Concerta, Abilify and Atomoxetine). Their fMRI data were examined in comparison to children with ASD who were not taking medication, and the data did not differ between these subgroups (see Additional file 1). Children were recruited through community support centres, parent support groups, email lists, hospital ads and private schools. Informed consent, clinical and cognitive testing and MRI scanning were performed at the Hospital for Sick Children in Toronto. Experimental procedures were approved by the Research Ethics Board at the Hospital for Sick Children. All children gave informed assent, and the parents provided informed written consent.

Clinical diagnoses of ASD were confirmed in all cases with a combination of expert clinical judgement, clinical records and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) [56], which was administered by a trained individual who maintains inter-rater research reliability. All children completed the backwards digit recall, listening recall, digit recall, mazes memory and block recall subscales of the Working Memory Test Battery for Children (WMTB-C) [57] to supplement behavioural data collected during fMRI tasks. See Table 1 for demographic, neuropsychological and clinical test characteristics.

Table 1 Demographic and neuropsychological test characteristics of study sample

The colour matching task

Children were required to attend to coloured figures of a clown presented one at a time in sequence. Children were taught to ignore the clown's face and irrelevant colours (blue and green) and focus on other, relevant colours (yellow, purple, pink, red, orange, brown and grey). Tasks that contain misleading or irrelevant factors that evoke interference have been shown to be a more suitable measure of WM capacity, yielding reliable estimates of developmental growth in adults [48] and children (Powell, Arsalidou, Vogan, and Taylor: Controlled inference and assessments of developmental working memory capacity: Evidence from letter and colour matching tasks, submitted). The number of ‘n’ relevant colours (capacity) in the figures was increased by one for each increase in difficulty level. CMT has two integral functions that require mental attention, in which participants must first actively extract the relevant colours embedded in the clown figure and second check for a possible match with colours of the criterion set. As such, items with n (e.g. 1) relevant colours will have difficulty level of n + 2 (e.g. 3; Figure 1A). Children indicated after each item whether the relevant colours of the current figure matched those from the immediately preceding figure (i.e. 1-back), disregarding colour location and repetition. Using a keypad with the right hand, children responded in the scanner by pushing a button for ‘same’ when the clown figure was wearing the same colours as the previous clown and ‘different’ when the clown figure was wearing different colours. All children successfully completed practice trials on a computer outside the scanner with accuracy of 80% or greater.A total of 24 task blocks (168 task trials) and 24 baseline blocks were presented across four runs. Each run consisted of six 32-s blocks, one for each difficulty level (six levels in total); within each block, there was a constant difficulty level, and all difficulty levels were randomized within each run (Figure 1B). The same four runs were presented to all children in the same order. Task blocks consisted of eight stimuli of the same difficulty and alternated with 20-s baseline blocks (Figure 1C), where clowns were coloured only in blue and green, and children were instructed to look at the figures but not respond. Participants had 3 s to view a stimulus and respond, followed by a 1-s inter-stimulus interval where a fixation cross was presented (Figure 1D). The fMRI task took approximately 22 min of scan time.

Figure 1
figure 1

Protocol description of the colour matching task (CMT). (A) There were six levels of difficulty where the number of relevant colours (yellow, purple, pink, red, orange, brown and grey) increased by one to increase the difficulty level. Difficulty = (# of colours) + 2. Children were taught to ignore the clown's face, colour location, colour repetition and irrelevant colours (blue and green). (B) It was a block design task, where each run consisted of six 32-s task blocks (for each difficulty) followed by 20-s baseline blocks where clowns are presented in only blue and green (ignore). Task blocks were presented pseudo-randomly within each run. (C) Example of part of a sequence in a baseline block. Stimuli were presented for 3 s followed by 1-s inter-stimulus cross. Children were instructed to not respond. (D) Example of part of a sequence in a task block; children indicated if the current clown was wearing the same or different colours as the previous clown. Stimuli were presented for 3 s followed by a 1-s inter-stimulus fixation cross.

Performance data were recorded for both accuracy (proportion correct) and reaction time; items were correct if responded correctly within 3 s of stimulus presentation. To ensure adequate task completion, children were excluded if they did not achieve at least 60% accuracy (averaged across four runs) on the easiest two difficulty levels, and also excluded if they did not have at least two out of four runs where 50% or more of the blocks were acceptable in terms of performance (60% accuracy) and motion. Motion was deemed acceptable if children moved less than 1.5 mm from their median head position in at least 60% of the volumes within a task block. See the fMRI preprocessing section below for a description of displacement calculations. A 60% accuracy criterion was chosen because while we could be sure that children were performing better than chance (50%), it was not too stringent. Motion parameters were also entered into the fMRI preprocessing pipeline.

Image acquisition

All imaging data were acquired using a 3 T Siemens Trio MRI scanner with a 12-channel head coil. Head stabilization and motion restriction were achieved with foam padding. The structural scan was a high-resolution T1-weighted 3D MP-RAGE image (Sagittal; FOV = 192 × 240 × 256 mm; 1 mm isometric voxels; TR/TE/TI/FA = 2,300/2.96/900/9), which was used as an individual anatomical reference for the functional images. During structural image acquisition, children watched a movie of their choice using MR-compatible goggles and earphones. Functional images were acquired with single-shot echo planar imaging sequence (Axial; FOV = 192 × 192; Res = 64 × 64; 30 slices 5 mm thick; 3 × 3 × 5 mm voxels; TR/TE/FA = 2,000/30/70). Visual stimuli for the functional task (CMT) were displayed on MR-compatible goggles. Children responded to trials using a dual button MR-compatible keypad. Stimuli were displayed and performance was recorded using the software Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., Berkeley, CA, USA).

Behavioural data analyses

CMT

Accuracy on difficulty levels 7 and 8 (D7 and D8) was poor for both TD children (D7, M = 0.57, SD = 0.03; D8, M = 0.53, SD = 0.02) and children with ASD (D7, M = 0.53, SD = 0.02; D8, M = 0.54, SD = 0.03); thus analyses of only the first four difficulty levels (D3 to D6) were completed. Accuracy and response times were calculated for each difficulty level by averaging across runs for each group. Data were analyzed using repeated measures factorial ANOVAs, with group (ASD and TD) as a between subject factor and difficulty level (D3, D4, D5 and D6) as a within subject factor.

WMTB-C

Standardized scores on the subscales were compared across group using t tests to determine if there were differences between ASD and TD children on these neuropsychological measures of WM.

fMRI data analyses

Image preprocessing of functional data was performed using a combination of standard AFNI [58] and FMRIB's Software Library (FSL) [59] tools. The first three volumes of each run were discarded for scanner stabilization. After slice timing and motion correction, data were smoothed using a 6-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel, temporally filtered (lower and upper cutoff frequencies of 0.01 and 0.2 Hz, respectively) and converted to percent signal change from the baseline volumes. Before group-level analyses, images were registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 template. The maximum Euclidean displacement (MD) travelled by any voxel within the brain was calculated from the six rigid body transformation parameters for each volume. This MD metric was used to flag volumes with unacceptable motion, as described above. The average MD for each subject was used to explore group differences in head motion. Although more motion was found in children with ASD (M = 0.58 mm, SD = 0.54 mm) than TD children (M = 0.29 mm, SD = 0.22 mm), t(34) = 2.07, p = 0.05, both groups had minimal average motion of under 0.60 mm. To control for motion, MD was also included as a covariate of no interest in the GLM.

Data were analyzed with the FSL fMRI Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT) [60]. Data were fit first to a block-design general linear model convolved with a gamma function to model haemodynamic response, using the task parameters (D3 to D6). To examine areas that linearly modulated as a function of difficulty, linear trend analyses were conducted from D3 to D6 using fixed-effects higher level modelling. Individual results were then averaged across runs for each subject in a second-level analysis. Between-group comparisons were carried out using FMRIB's Local Analysis of Mixed Effects-1 (FLAME 1) [59] to obtain an accurate between-subject variance estimation, which increased our ability to detect real activation [60]. Significant activations were reported using cluster-based thresholding determined by Z > |2.3| and a corrected cluster significance threshold of p < 0.05. Regions of interest (ROIs) were selected from the local maxima of areas showing significant group differences between TD and ASD groups in the linear trend analyses. Average percent signal change and standard error scores were extracted from spherical ROIs (6 mm radius) centred about the local maxima of group difference maps, and the mean peak cluster signal change for each group was plotted as a function of difficulty to further examine activation patterns across cognitive load.

Results

Behavioural data

There was a significant effect of group on accuracy (Figure 2A), F(1, 34) = 5.15, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.13, which was driven by TD children (M = 0.80, SD = 0.12) performing more accurately than ASD children (M = .71, SD = 0.12) only at D5 (t(34) = 2.30, p = 0.03). As such, comparisons of brain activity between control and ASD groups were made under comparable accuracy scores across most levels. There was a significant main effect of difficulty on accuracy, F(3, 34) = 53.87, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.61, with performance accuracy decreasing as a function of difficulty level in both groups. Post hoc pairwise comparisons, adjusted for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni, revealed that accuracy on the most difficult level was significantly different from each other at p < 0.05, the exceptions being between D5 and D6 in the ASD group (Table 2), and between D3 and D4, and between D4 and D5 in TD children (Table 2). We also ran a supplementary analysis, inputting performance as a covariate. There were no areas of activation that correlated with performance in either group, suggesting that findings were not confounded by performance per se. Also, there were no significant differences in performance across runs in the TD (F(3, 48) =1.21, p = 0.32) or ASD (F(3, 45) = 0.50, p = 0.69) group, and therefore patterns of performance did not reflect fatigue across time.

Figure 2
figure 2

CMT behavioural performance. (A) Mean proportion correct for D3 to D6 and standard error bars. TD children were significantly more accurate than children with ASD at D5 only. (B) Mean response times for D3 to D6 and standard error bars. There were no significant differences between TD and ASD groups in response times across difficulty levels.

Table 2 Differences in CMT accuracy (proportion correct) across difficulty

Overall, response times increased with increasing difficulty in TD children but only increased up until D5 in children with ASD (Figure 2B). There was no main effect of group on response times (F(1, 34) = 2.33, p = 0.14), but there was a main effect of difficulty level, F(3, 34) = 54.60, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.62. Post hoc comparisons in the ASD group showed that response times differed between difficulty levels, except between D4 and D6, and between D5 and D6 (Table 3). In TD children, response times significantly differed between difficulty levels, except between D4 and D6 and D5 and D6 (Table 3).

Table 3 Differences in CMT response times (seconds) across difficulty levels

For the WMTB-C, TD children had significantly higher scores on the listening recall (t(34) = 2.48, p < 0.05) and backward digit recall (t(34) = 2.28, p < 0.05) subtests than children with ASD. The groups did not differ on any other subtests (see Table 1 for scores on the WMTB-C).

fMRI Data

Task-related activation within groups

The primary objectives of the fMRI analyses were to investigate the pattern of brain activity exhibited as a function of cognitive load (i.e. difficulty level) and determine if this pattern differed in children with and without ASD. Linear trend analyses (D3 to D6) showed that while some brain areas increased in activity as a function of difficulty level, others decreased. ‘Increasing activation’ refers to an increase in BOLD signal with increasing load (i.e. positive linear relations between cortical activity and task difficulty level) and ‘decreasing activation’ refers to a decrease in BOLD signal with increasing load (i.e. negative linear relations between cortical activity and task difficulty level). As shown in Figure 3, magnitude of the signal change increased with difficulty in TD children, particularly between D5 and D6, in bilateral fusiform (BA37), precuneus (BA7), inferior frontal gyri (BA45), right DLPFC (BA9) and bilateral dorsal cingulate/dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (B32/8) extending to the anterior cingulate (B33/24). In the bilateral middle occipital gyrus (BA19), a positive linear change in activation with increasing task difficulty was seen up to D6 (Table 4). Activation found in the bilateral posterior cingulate (BA 23/31) and anterior medial prefrontal gyrus (BA10) decreased (i.e. showed a negative linear relation) as a function of difficulty level (Table 4).

Figure 3
figure 3

Group activation maps for the linear trend analyses in ASD and TD groups during CMT. Significant activations using cluster-based thresholding determined by Z > |2.3| and a corrected cluster significance threshold of p = 0.05. Areas in red depict regions of increasing activation as a function of difficulty (i.e. positive linear relations between cortical activity and task difficulty level), and areas in blue depict regions of decreasing activation (i.e. negative linear relations between cortical activity and task difficulty level). dmPFC dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, Post Cing posterior cingulate cortex, Prec precuneus, DLPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, ACC anterior cingulate cortex, IFG inferior frontal gyrus, MidOcG middle occipital gyrus, FusG fusiform gyrus, amPFG anterior medial prefrontal gyrus.

Table 4 Linear trend analyses across difficulty levels: TD group

Children with ASD did not show the same trends in activation, particularly in the frontal brain regions. A positive linear relation between cortical activity and task difficulty was seen only in the posterior brain regions, including the bilateral middle occipital (BA19) and fusiform gyri (BA37, Table 5). Similar to TD children, activation in the posterior cingulate (BA23/31) and medial prefrontal gyrus (BA10/32) decreased (i.e., showed a negative linear relation) as a function of difficulty level in children with ASD (Table 5).

Table 5 Linear trend analyses across difficulty levels: ASD group

Between-group differences in task-related activation

Between-group analyses of each difficulty level separately showed no significant differences (see Additional file 2). However, there were significant group effects in linear activation trends as a function of difficulty level. As shown in Figure 4, three regions demonstrated group differences in activation. These areas included the bilateral precuneus (BA7), right DLPFC (BA9) and left dorsal medial premotor cortex (BA8) (Table 6). In these regions, TD children showed significant positive linear relations between cortical activity and difficulty level, particularly between D5 and D6, whereas children with ASD did not show these trends between D5 and D6. The magnitude of the signal change in frontal and parietal areas increased largely with the most difficult level when TD children tried to meet WM demand, but children with ASD failed to show this increase. See Figure 5 for graphs of percent signal change with standard error for ROIs of brain regions that showed significantly different linear patterns between TD and ASD groups.

Figure 4
figure 4

Results from between-group comparisons. Significant activations using cluster-based thresholding determined by Z > 2.3 and a corrected cluster significance threshold of p = 0.05. Areas in red depict regions where the control children showed greater positive linear relations between cortical activity and task difficulty level than the ASD group. There were no areas where the ASD group showed greater linear activation trends across difficulty level in the negative or positive direction than controls. Prec precuneus, DLPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, dmPMC dorsal medial premotor cortex.

Table 6 Regions of significant differences between TD and ASD groups
Figure 5
figure 5

Mean peak cluster percent signal changes and standard error. As a function of difficulty between task difficulty and baseline conditions in areas where children with ASD significantly differed from TD children in the linear trend analyses. DLPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Discussion

This is the first study to examine the neural correlates of visuo-spatial WM in pre-adolescent children with ASD relative to TD children. Whereas children with and without ASD did not differ in brain activation during WM function irrespective of load, between-group differences were observed in the linear activation trends across difficulty level. In other words, children with and without ASD differ in how they modulate WM processes during tasks that increase in difficulty level. Using a task that isolated cognitive load on WM, we observed positive linear relations between cortical activity and task difficulty level in prefrontal and parietal regions, particularly between the highest difficulty levels, in TD children that were significantly different from activation trends seen in children with ASD. These areas included bilateral precuneus, right DLPFC and left medial premotor cortices. Other areas known to underlie WM function, including the anterior cingulate and inferior frontal gyri, followed this same pattern but the linear activation trends did not differ significantly between TD and ASD subjects, likely due to subject variability. Overall, TD children demonstrated an opposing system of cognitive processes where areas related to task difficulty (frontal regions) increased in activity and areas associated with the brain's DMN (posterior cingulate and anterior medial frontal gyrus) decreased in activity with increasing cognitive demand; this pattern of activation was absent in children with ASD. There were significant differences in performance on only two verbal WM subtests of the WMTB-C. Further, TD and ASD groups performed similarly on the CMT (except for a small difference at D5), suggesting that the findings are not confounded by behavioural differences.

TD children showed increased recruitment of both frontal and posterior parietal regions as a function of cognitive load, whereas the ASD group showed increased recruitment of only the posterior visual regions, including the left and right fusiform and middle occipital gyri. Our observed pattern of findings in children with ASD are consistent with findings in adults with ASD who also relied on posterior regions related to low-level cognition during WM tasks, rather than areas of high level-cognition, such as in the prefrontal cortices [10]. More activation in occipital-temporal areas in ASD has been proposed to reflect visually based processing styles and a tendency to rely primarily on visual features and details of objects, rather than on WM [61]. These findings are in line with the enhanced perceptual functioning model [62] that suggests individuals with ASD display superior activation of visuo-perceptual regions in association with a reduced activation in higher order frontal areas. The present study extends this literature in adults by highlighting that this processing style appears in young children with ASD as well. The same pattern of activation has also been found in socially relevant tasks [63, 64]. Although greater posterior activation may be adequate for visuo-spatial processing, it is likely inefficient for more complex cognitive operations, such as language comprehension [19] and WM [10], which would have implications for the social and executive deficits typically observed in ASD. Less extensive use of prefrontal areas may be the result of early abnormal prefrontal development [15, 16] and connectivity [10, 19] reported in individuals with ASD.

The finding of stable activation with increasing cognitive load within the prefrontal cortex may be most significant for ASD due to the crucial role of frontal regions in WM and executive function [10, 43, 44, 65–67]. Similar to adults [10, 12] and adolescents [14] with ASD, we found that pre-adolescent children with ASD demonstrated different activation patterns in the DLPFC (BA 9) during WM function. Specifically, in the DLPFC, TD children showed a graded increase in activity from D3 to D6, whereas activity within this region did not increase at the same magnitude in children with ASD. The DLPFC is believed to play a critical role in holding information ‘online’ [26] and mediating strategic organization and data compression processes [68, 69], hence its sensitivity to increasing cognitive demand in ours and other studies [68–71]. Stable activation across difficulty level in this area suggests that young children with ASD may fail to use appropriate organizational strategies, such as ‘chunking’ methods, which facilitate WM by simplifying cognitive load/demand. Furthermore, normative studies have demonstrated that with increasing age, individuals rely more on DLPFC in WM processes [43, 66, 72], suggesting that specialization of this region for WM coincides with structural maturation across development. It is possible that atypical abnormal growth patterns in ASD in the frontal cortex, the DLPFC in particular [15], adversely impacts its functional integrity; these speculations require further research.

Failure to increase recruitment of the precuneus across difficulty level in ASD subjects is also of interest, given evidence supporting parietal involvement in addition to the prefrontal cortex during spatial WM processing [44, 66, 70, 73]. The precuneus forms part of the proposed occipito-parietal network, or alternatively the visual ‘dorsal pathway’, responsible for spatial visual processing (e.g. object location) [74]. Conversely, the occipito-temporal, or visual ‘ventral pathway’, includes the fusiform gyri and is critical to object identification (e.g. colour and shape) [74]. Given that CMT was a visuo-spatial task with gradual increases in WM that needed to be processed, areas responsive to both spatial search (occipito-parietal regions) and categorization (occipito-temporal regions) should show increased recruitment across cognitive load. Although group differences in fusiform activity were absent, we observed a significant difference in the precuneus; TD children showed positive linear relations between cortical activity and difficulty level, particularly between D5 and D6, whereas the ASD group did not show this linear change. Impaired parietal activity in pre-adolescent children with ASD lends support to the proposed dorsal stream deficits within this population as well as in other developmental disorders [75–78], while ventral processing is relatively intact. Furthermore, normative developmental studies suggest that the dorsal pathway has a more protracted maturational trajectory than the ventral stream [66], and WM fMRI studies demonstrate greater parietal [4, 73] but less fusiform recruitment [66] with age. Thus, spared fusiform/occipital activity and abnormal parietal functioning in our ASD group may reflect immature WM processing typically seen in very young TD children. However, it is not clear whether the recruitment of mature neural substrates underlying WM processes is delayed, persistently weak or arrested. Future longitudinal work will help clarify the developmental path of WM neural circuitry in ASD.

Conclusions

Overall, the current study fills a significant gap in our knowledge of neural substrates implicated in visuo-spatial WM functioning among pre-adolescent children and how they differ in children with ASD. Similar to adults, we found that children with ASD show prefrontal and parietal system abnormalities and tend to rely on posterior brain regions associated with lower level cognitive processing. The present study also converges on the growing body of literature proposing dorsal stream deficits in visuo-spatial processing in this population. This study contributes new information on WM differences in school-aged children with and without ASD, demonstrating that even in pre-adolescents, there are significant differences in brain activation patterns during WM processing with increasing cognitive load that differentiate the groups. In light of similar task performance between groups, it is also important to entertain the idea that higher order processing may not be mandatory in ASD with tasks that can be processed efficiently using a perceptual processing approach because of their proposed enhanced perceptual functioning [62]. Conversely, higher order control may be necessary in TD children who do not have such perceptual expertise. Although data may support this argument, ASD is generally considered a network/brain disorder, and what is interesting is how networks in children with ASD differ from TD children with complex tasks. Group effects were driven by the most difficult level analyzed, which may be due to strategies used by TD children necessary to encode the increased number of colours in the highest task load (i.e. meet high WM demand) that children with ASD are unable to employ. Similarly, previous research reports behavioural differences with tasks that have more complex WM demands e.g. [9, 34, 39].

It is important to consider the limitations of the current study when interpreting results. With comparable CMT behavioural performance between ASD and TD groups, we eliminated performance as a confounding factor. Consequently, our sample was less representative of low-functioning individuals with ASD, and thus, results are generalizable to higher functioning individuals only. Future fMRI research is required to understand WM function across various levels of functioning and a range of symptoms. In addition, given our choice of control subjects (TD children), findings can only provide information about differences from the norm. Comparisons to other atypical populations who share similar cognitive but different clinical profiles (e.g. ADHD) [5] will further our understanding about the neural patterns that are unique to ASD, potentially explaining characteristic behaviour in this complex group. Lastly, results are also limited by the relatively small sample size, due to assessing a complex cognitive ability while scanning, which greatly increased the amount of children's data that was not usable.

The findings from the current study have a number of significant implications despite these limitations. Several researchers have stressed the importance of executive cognitive skills for social function [5, 79], a core deficit of ASD. However, there remains a gap in knowledge regarding the link between neuropsychopathology and clinical symptoms of autism. With respect to our findings of atypical neural activity underlying WM processes in ASD, this may impair the ability to hold information ‘online’ that may affect one's ability to evaluate and select appropriate responses during peer interactions, translating into socially inappropriate behaviour. Social behaviour requires complex cognitive processing, further highlighting the importance of our results of increasing group differences in brain activity with increases in complexity. Future neuroimaging studies could work towards understanding the relation between ASD symptomology and neural activation patterns associated with WM processing. Overall, our findings will help guide future longitudinal work by localizing areas of vulnerability to developmental disturbances and allow health care providers to carefully monitor their development. Developmental information will allow us to identify the nature and timing of atypical development, which is critical in establishing age-appropriate cognitive or pharmacological remediation for WM function and behavioural deficits in ASD.

References

  1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC): Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders–autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, United States, 2008. MMWR Morbidity and Mortal Weekly Report. 2012, 61 (3): 1-44.

    Google Scholar 

  2. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 2013, Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 5

    Google Scholar 

  3. Barnard L, Muldoon K, Hasan R, O’Brien G, Stewart M: Profiling executive dysfunction in adults with autism and comorbid learning disability. Autism. 2008, 12: 125-141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Greene CM, Braet W, Johnson KA, Bellgrove MA: Imaging the genetics of executive function. Biol Psychol. 2008, 79: 30-42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Happe F, Booth R, Charlton R, Hughes C: Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain Cognition. 2006, 61: 25-39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Hill EL: Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends Cogni Sci. 2004, 8: 26-32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Joseph RM: Neuropsychological frameworks for understanding autism. Int Rev Psychiatr. 1999, 11: 309-324.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Luna B, Doll SK, Hegedus SJ, Minshew NJ, Sweeney JA: Maturation of executive function in autism. Biol Soc. 2007, 61: 474-481.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Russo N, Flanagan T, Iarocci G, Berringer D, Zelazo PD, Burack JA: Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain Cognition. 2007, 65: 77-86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Koshino H, Carpenter PA, Minshew NJ, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Just MA: Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism. Neuroimage. 2005, 24: 810-821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Koshino H, Kana RK, Keller TA, Cherkassky VL, Minshew NJ, Just MA: fMRI investigation of working memory for faces in autism: visual coding and underconnectivity with frontal areas. Cereb Cortex. 2007, 18: 289-300.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Luna B, Minshew NJ, Garver KE, Lazar NA, Thulborn KR, Eddy WF, Sweeny JA: Neocortical system abnormalities in autism An fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology. 2002, 59: 834-840.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. O’Hearn K, Asato M, Ordaz S, Luna B: Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Dev Psychopathol. 2008, 20: 1103-1132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Silk TJ, Rinehart N, Bradshaw JL, Tonge B, Egan G, O’Boyle MW, Cunnington R: Visuospatial processing and the function of prefrontal-parietal networks in autism spectrum disorders: a functional MRI study. Am J Psychiat. 2006, 163: 1440-1443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Carper RA, Courchesne E: Localized enlargement of the frontal cortex in early autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2005, 57: 126-133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hazlett HC, Poe MD, Gerig G, Smith RG, Piven J: Cortical gray and white brain tissue volume in adolescents and adults with autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2006, 59: 1-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Deutsch CK, O’Brien LM, Lange N, Bakardjiev A, Hodgson J, Adrien KT, Steele S, Makris N, Kennedy D, Harris GJ, Caviness VS: Dissociations of cerebral cortex, subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes in autistic boys. Brain. 2003, 126: 1182-1192.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Mak-Fan KM, Taylor MJ, Roberts W, Lerch JP: Measures of cortical grey matter structure and development in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012, 42: 419-427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ: Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain. 2004, 127: 1811-1821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Levitt JG, Blanton RE, Smalley S, Thompson PM, Guthrie D, McCracken JT, Sadoun T, Heinichen L, Toga AW: Cortical sulcal maps in autism. Cereb Cortex. 2003, 13: 728-735.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. McPartland J, Dawson G, Webb SJ, Panagiotides H, Carver LJ: Event-related brain potentials reveal anomalies in temporal processing of faces in autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2004, 45: 1235-1245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Zilbovicius M, Garreau B, Samson Y, Remy P, Barthelemy C, Syrota A, Lelord G: Delayed maturation of the frontal cortex in childhood autism. Am J Psychiat. 1995, 152: 248-252.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Chandana SR, Behen ME, Juhász C, Muzik O, Rothermel RD, Mangner TJ, Chakraborty PK, Chungani HT, Chungani DC: Significance of abnormalities in developmental trajectory and asymmetry of cortical serotonin synthesis in autism. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005, 23: 171-182.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Lainhart JE: Advances in autism neuroimaging research for the clinician and geneticist. Am J Med Genet. 2006, 142C: 33-39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Griebling J, Minshew NJ, Bodner K, Libove R, Bansal R, Konasale P, Keshavan MS, Hardan A: Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex magnetic resonance imaging measurements and cognitive performance in autism. J Child Neuro. 2010, 25: 856-863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Powell KB, Voeller KK: Prefrontal executive function syndromes in children. J Child Neuro. 2004, 19: 785-797.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Leonard CM, Welcome SE, Kan E, Toga AW: Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J Neurosci. 2004, 24: 8223-8231.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Dennis M, Agostino A, Roncadin C, Levin H: Theory of mind depends on domain-general executive functions of working memory and cognitive inhibition in children with traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2009, 31: 835-847.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Alloway TP: Working memory, but not IQ, predicts subsequent learning in children with learning difficulties. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2009, 25: 92-98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Baddeley A: Working memory. Science. 1992, 255: 556-559.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Engle RW, Tuholski SW, Laughlin JE, Conway AR: Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1999, 128: 309-

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Paas FG, Van Merriënboer JJ: Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: a cognitive-load approach. J Educ Psychol. 1994, 86: 122-

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Benetto L, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ: Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Dev. 1996, 67: 1816-1835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Minshew NJ, Goldstein G: The pattern of intact and impaired memory functions in autism. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2001, 42: 1095-1101.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Russell J, Jarrold C, Henry L: Working memory in children with autism and with moderate learning difficulties. J Child Psychol Psyc. 1996, 37: 673-686.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Ozonoff S, Strayer DL: Further evidence of intact working memory in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001, 31: 257-263.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Steele SD, Minshew NJ, Luna B, Sweeney JA: Spatial working memory deficits in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007, 37: 605-612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Williams DL, Goldstein G, Carpenter PA, Minshew NJ: Verbal and spatial working memory in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005, 35: 747-756.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Williams DL, Minshew NJ, Goldstein G: Memory within a complex information processing model of autism. Memory in autism. Edited by: Boucher J, Bowler D. 2008, New York: Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  40. Baddeley A: Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003, 4: 829-839.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Carlson S, Martinkappi S, Rama P, Salli E, Korvenoja A, Aronen HJ: Distribution of cortical activation during visuospatial n-back tasks as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex. 1998, 8: 743-752.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Fuster JM: Prefrontal neurons in networks of executive memory. Brain Res Bull. 2000, 52: 331-336.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kwon H, Reiss AL, Menon V: Neural basis of protracted developmental changes in visuo-spatial working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002, 99: 13336-13341.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Owen A, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E: N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Map. 2005, 25: 46-59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Kirchener WK: Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J Exp Psychol. 1958, 55: 352-358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Cohen JD, Perlstein WM, Braver TS, Nystrom LE, Noll DC, Jonides J, Smith EE: Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature. 1997, 386: 604-608.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Nelson CA, Monk CS, Lin J, Carver LJ, Thomas KM, Truwit CL: Functional neuroanatomy of spatial working memory in children. Dev Psychol. 2000, 36: 109-116.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Arsalidou M, Pascual-Leone J, Johnson J: Misleading cues improve developmental assessment of working memory capacity: the color matching tasks. Cognitive Dev. 2010, 25: 262-277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Arsalidou M, Pascual-Leone J, Johnson J, Morris D, Taylor MJ: A balancing act of the brain: activations and deactivations driven by cognitive load. Brain Behav. 2013, 3: 273-285.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Ford JM: Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012, 8: 49-76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Assaf M, Jagannathan K, Calhoun VD, Miller L, Stevens MC, Sahl R, O`Boyle JG, Schultz RT, Pearlson GD: Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism spectrum disorder patients. Neuroimage. 2010, 53: 247-256.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Kennedy DP, Redcay E, Courchesne E: Failing to deactivate: resting functional abnormalities in autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006, 103: 8275-8280.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Uddin LQ, Menon V, Young CB, Ryali S, Chen T, Khouzam A, Minshew NJ, Hardan AY: Multivariate searchlight classification of structural magnetic resonance imaging in children and adolescents with autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2011, 70: 833-841.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Frith U: Mind blindness and the brain in autism. Neuron. 2001, 32: 969-979.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Wechsler D: Wechsler intelligence scale for children. 2003, San Antonia, TX: Psychological Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  56. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M: The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000, 30: 205-223.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Pickering S, Gathercole S: Working memory test battery for children. 2001, London, UK: Pearson Assessment

    Google Scholar 

  58. Cox RW: AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res. 1996, 29: 162-173.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Worsley KJ: Statistical analysis of activation images. Ch 14, in functional MRI: an introduction to methods. Edited by: Jezzard P, Matthews PM, Smith SM. 2001, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  60. Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, Chappell M, Makni S, Behrens T, Beckmann C, Jenkinson M, Smith S: Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage. 2009, 45: S173-S186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Ring HA, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Williams SCR, Brammer M, Andrew C, Bullmore ET: Cerebral correlates of preserved cognitive skills in autism. Brain. 1999, 122: 1305-1315.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulières I, Hubert B, Burack J: Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principled of autistic perception. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006, 36: 27-43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Hubl D, Bolte S, Feineis-Matthews S, Lanfermann H, Federspiel A, Strik W, Poustka F, Dierks T: Functional imbalance of visual pathways indicates alternative face processing strategies in autism. Neurology. 2003, 61: 1232-1237.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Pierce K, Müller RA, Ambrose JB, Allen G, Courchesne E: Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism: evidence from functional MRI. Brain. 2001, 124: 2059-2073.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Duncan J, Owen AM: Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 2000, 23: 475-483.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Scherf KS, Sweeney JA, Beatriz L: Brain basis of developmental change in visuospatial working memory. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2006, 18: 1045-1058.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Wager TD, Smith EE: Neuroimaging studies of working memory. Cognitive Affective Behav Neurosci. 2003, 3: 255-274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Rypma B, Berger JS, D’Esposito M: Performance on prefrontal cortical activity. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2002, 14: 721-731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Bor D, Duncan J, Wiseman RJ, Owen AM: Encoding strategies dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand. Neuron. 2003, 37: 361-367.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Rypma B, Prabhakaran V, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD: Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory. Neuroimage. 1999, 9: 216-226.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Finn AS, Sheridan MA, Kam CLH, Hinshaw S, D’Esposito M: Longitudinal evidence for functional specialization of the neural circuit supporting working memory in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2010, 30: 11062-11067.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H: Increased brain activity in frontal and parietal cortex underlies the development of visuospatial working memory capacity during childhood. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2002, 14: 1-10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Olesen PJ, Nagy Z, Westerberg H, Klingberg T: Combined analysis of DTI and fMRI data reveals a joint maturation of white and grey matter in a fronto-parietal network. Cognitive Brain Res. 2003, 18: 48-57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M: Two cortical visual systems. Analysis of visual behavior. Edited by: Ingle DJ I, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJW. 1982, Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press, 549-586.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Pellicano E, Gibson LY: Investigating the functional integrity of the dorsal visual pathway in autism and dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2008, 46: 2593-2596.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Pellicano E, Gibson L, Maybery M, Durkin K, Badcock DR: Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: a possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence?. Neuropsychologia. 2005, 43: 1044-1053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Spencer J, O’Brien J, Riggs K, Braddick O, Atkinson J, Wattam-Bell J: Motion processing in autism: evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. Cognitive Neurosci Neuropsychol. 2000, 11: 2765-2766.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  78. Grinter EJ, Maybery MT, Badcock DR: Vision in developmental disorders: is there a dorsal stream deficit?. Brain Res Bull. 2010, 8: 147-160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Beauchamp MH, Anderson V: SOCIAL: an integrative framework for the development of social skills. Psychol Bull. 2010, 136: 39-64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all of the families and children for their support and participation. We would also like to thank Rina Goukon for her assistance with participant recruitment and data collection. Many thanks to Rachel Leung and Becky Baatjes for administering the ADOS-R and ADOS-2 and Dr. Jessica Brian for reviewing all assessments. Sincere thanks to our MRI technicians, Ruth Weiss and Tammy Rayner, for all their support in data acquisition. The authors would also like to thank Marie Arsalidou for developing the CMT task and allowing us to use it. Lastly, thanks to Crescent School in Toronto for their support and participation in this project. This research was funded by Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP-106582) and preliminary analyses of these data were presented as a poster at the International Meeting for Autism Research (IMFAR) 2013.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vanessa M Vogan.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

VV is responsible for recruitment, data acquisition, analysis and interpretation as well as drafting the manuscript. BM participated in fMRI analysis and revising manuscript. WL developed the fMRI analysis pipelines and helped with experimental design and statistical analyses. MS advised on patient testing, study design and revising manuscript. TP assisted with recruitment, data acquisition and revising manuscript. MT initiated the study and participated in design, analyses and revising manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Vanessa M Vogan, Benjamin R Morgan, Wayne Lee, Tamara L Powell, Mary Lou Smith and Margot J Taylor contributed equally to this work.

Electronic supplementary material

11689_2014_74_MOESM1_ESM.png

Additional file 1: fMRI data for medicated and non-medicated children with ASD. Percent signal change as a function of difficulty between task difficulty and baseline conditions in children with ASD who were on medication versus those who were not. Areas of the brain shown are from regions where children with ASD significantly differed from TD children in the linear trend analyses. Potential differences between medicated and non-medicated children with ASD were also examined statistically using the FSL FEAT, and no significant differences were found between children with ASD who were and were not on medication. However, due to low N (only six subjects on medication), this statistical test may not be reliable. Therefore scatter plots were created to visually examine the data for significant group differences, and this reaffirmed that medication does not appear to affect the findings. (PNG 93 KB)

11689_2014_74_MOESM2_ESM.png

Additional file 2: Individual group activation maps for children with ASD and typically developing children at all levels (D3 to D6) of CMT. Significant activations using cluster-based thresholding determined by Z > |2.3| and a corrected cluster significance threshold of p = 0.05. Areas in red and blue depict regions with significantly higher and lower BOLD signal than baseline, respectively. Between-group comparisons showed no areas of significant difference between children with and without ASD at any single difficulty level. (PNG 312 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vogan, V.M., Morgan, B.R., Lee, W. et al. The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load. J Neurodevelop Disord 6, 19 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-6-19

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-6-19

Keywords