The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective

Poor threat-safety discrimination reflects prefrontal cortex dysfunction in adult anxiety disorders. While adolescent anxiety disorders are impairing and predict high risk for adult anxiety disorders, the neural correlates of threat-safety discrimination have not been investigated in this population. The authors compared prefrontal cortex function in anxious and healthy adolescents and adults following conditioning and extinction, processes requiring threat-safety learning.

Method

Anxious and healthy adolescents and adults (N=114) completed fear conditioning and extinction in the clinic. The conditioned stimuli (CS+) were neutral faces, paired with an aversive scream. Physiological and subjective data were acquired. Three weeks later, 82 participants viewed the CS+ and morphed images resembling the CS+ in an MRI scanner. During scanning, participants made difficult threat-safety discriminations while appraising threat and explicit memory of the CS+.

Results

During conditioning and extinction, the anxious groups reported more fear than the healthy groups, but the anxious adolescent and adult groups did not differ on physiological measures. During imaging, both anxious adolescents and adults exhibited lower activation in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex than their healthy counterparts, specifically when appraising threat. Compared with their age-matched counterpart groups, anxious adults exhibited reduced activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex when appraising threat, whereas anxious adolescents exhibited a U-shaped pattern of activation, with greater activation in response to the most extreme CS+ and CS–.

Conclusions

Two regions of the prefrontal cortex are involved in anxiety disorders. Reduced subgenual anterior cingulate cortex engagement is a shared feature in adult and adolescent anxiety disorders, but ventromedial prefrontal cortex dysfunction is age-specific. The unique U-shaped pattern of activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in many anxious adolescents may reflect heightened sensitivity to threat and safety conditions. How variations in the pattern relate to later risk for adult illness remains to be determined.

In adult anxiety disorders, poor threat-safety discrimination is thought to arise from prefrontal cortex dysfunction that can be ameliorated by exposure therapy (1). Adolescent anxiety disorders are impairing, predict high risk for adult anxiety disorders, and, like adult anxiety disorders, respond to exposure therapy (2, 3). However, while research has identified the neural correlates of threat-safety discrimination in adult anxiety disorders (4), there are no parallel studies in adolescents. In this study, we used conditioning, extinction, and a threat-safety discrimination paradigm to compare prefrontal cortex function in anxious and healthy adolescents and adults (5).

Conditioning and extinction are often used in animal studies to examine fear and safety learning (6, 7). In fear conditioning, a previously neutral conditioned stimulus (CS+) acquires threat value after it is paired with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (UCS). In extinction learning, the CS+ is presented in the absence of the aversive UCS, typically resulting in a new CS+-safe association. The level of fear experienced in response to subsequent encounters with the CS+ is thought to reflect competition between memory for the original threat and subsequent safety associations.

Cross-species work has mapped circuits supporting memory for an extinguished CS+ in the ventral prefrontal cortex (8). When humans encounter an extinguished CS+, they engage the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, two medial prefrontal cortical subregions. Moreover, levels of activation are lower in anxious adults than in nonanxious adults (4, 9). However, no research has examined prefrontal cortex responses to a CS+ following fear conditioning and extinction in healthy or anxious adolescents. To do so, we adapted previously used paradigms in which neutral faces served as the CS (10, 11) and a scream served as the UCS (12, 13). While previous functional MRI (fMRI) studies of healthy youths used our paradigm to study conditioning (13), we instead compared neural responses to a threat following conditioning and extinction in healthy and anxious adolescents and adults.

First, we studied participants in the clinic to compare responses during fear acquisition and extinction. Based on previous research, we hypothesized that compared with healthy individuals, anxious individuals would condition similarly, measured by relative differences between the CS+ and CS– (the CS not predicting the UCS), but would report greater subjective anxiety in response to both the CS+ and CS–. We further expected both anxious groups to exhibit lower levels of extinction than the two healthy groups (14).

Next, we used fMRI to map brain regions engaged during exposure to an array of CS+ and CS– stimuli 3 weeks after extinction. This array contained images created by morphing the previously extinguished CS+ with the CS– in incremental levels. Participants were asked to make fine-grained distinctions among these stimuli during fMRI scanning. Specifically, the paradigm required participants to appraise the level of threat conveyed by each stimulus, a process highly relevant to clinical anxiety (5). This task was expected to engage the ventral prefrontal cortex, given the role of this region in threat-safety discrimination.

Previous research has suggested two hypotheses: adolescent anxiety disorders predict high risk for adult anxiety disorders, and exposure therapy appears to be similarly beneficial in the two age groups. We therefore hypothesized that anxiety disorders in the two age groups would manifest overlapping areas of prefrontal cortex dysfunction. However, research on the neural circuitry mediating normal types of fear has found age-related differences in prefrontal cortex function (12). Moreover, only a subset of adolescent anxiety disorders persists into adulthood (2). As a result, we also expected to observe age-related differences in prefrontal cortex function in both the healthy and anxious groups.

Method

Participants

Youths and adults participated in this study as paid volunteers. Written informed consent from adult participants and parents and written assent from youth participants were provided. Procedures were approved by the National Institute of Mental Health Institutional Review Board.

Individuals were studied if they were medication free, physically healthy, and had an IQ >70. All participants received a comprehensive psychiatric assessment: the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders (15) for adults and the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Present and Lifetime Version (16) for youths under age 18. Parents and youths were interviewed separately, and discrepancies were reconciled with both. Healthy participants were free from any current axis I disorder. Participants with anxiety disorders met DSM-IV criteria for a primary diagnosis of either generalized anxiety disorder or social phobia. To be consistent with previous research, additional anxiety disorders and major depressive disorder were permitted; however, current or past diagnoses of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or obsessive-compulsive disorder or other comorbid disorders were exclusionary.

Procedures

In this study, we adapted a differential fear conditioning and extinction paradigm used in earlier research (13). During fear acquisition, individuals passively viewed neutral faces of two women, the CS. One woman, the CS+, predicted the UCS, a fearful face coterminating with an aversive scream, while the other woman, the CS–, did not. Participants were instructed that they could learn to predict when the UCS would occur but were not informed of the CS/UCS contingency. During extinction, the CS+ and CS– were presented repeatedly in the absence of the UCS (Figure 1). Skin conductance response, startle response using electromyography, and self-reported anxiety were used to assess fear acquisition and extinction. Responses to the CS+, CS–, UCS, and interstimulus interval were measured (see the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article).

FIGURE 1. Experimental Procedures Among Anxious and Healthy Adolescents and Adultsa

a During fear acquisition, one female face (conditioned stimulus [CS+]) was paired with a fearful face coterminating with a scream (unconditioned stimulus [UCS]). The other female face (CS–) was never paired with the UCS. During extinction, the two female faces were presented without the UCS. Several weeks later, participants viewed morphed images continuously varying in similarity from the CS– to CS+. Participants reported whether they were afraid (threat appraisal), whether the CS screamed in the past (explicit memory), or whether the CS had jet black hair (physical discrimination).

Three weeks after fear conditioning and extinction (mean=19.7 days [SD=9.7]), participants underwent MRI scanning. The MRI sample included 14 anxious youths, 15 anxious adults, 25 healthy youths, and 28 healthy adults (Table 1; see also the online data supplement).

TABLE 1. Fear Conditioning and Extinction Results in Anxious and Healthy Adolescents and Adults
Task and GroupN
Age (Years)
IQa
Diagnosis (N)
Days Between Visits
TotalMaleMeanSDRangeMeanSDGeneral Anxiety DisorderSocial PhobiaSeparation AnxietyPanic DisorderMajor Depressive DisorderMeanSD
Fear acquisition and extinction
Anxious
 Youths231213.33.38–19108.412.61811624
 Adults18632.06.823–48121.310.013920
Healthy
 Youths421914.22.68–17112.411.7
 Adults311428.87.221–45119.411.3
Recall
Anxious
 Youths14714.82.811–19108.010.911921418.56.8
 Adults15632.97.024–48122.310.01171020.96.6
Healthy
 Youths251014.42.69–17111.710.522.112.6
 Adults281229.17.521–45120.410.417.69.1

a There was a significant age-group difference in IQ (p<0.005). In all initial analyses, IQ was included as a covariate of no interest, and results indicated that IQ did not alter the significant findings.

TABLE 1. Fear Conditioning and Extinction Results in Anxious and Healthy Adolescents and Adults
Enlarge table

The fMRI paradigm was explicitly designed to capture precisely the transitional boundary between threat and safety through the use of morphed images (1719) (Figure 1). In blocks of trials, participants viewed the CS+ and CS– and nine morphed images consisting of different blends of the CS– and CS+. This stimulus array was used to measure response gradients (linear or quadratic trends) along a continuum (19, 20). Previous research of recall in clinical anxiety has used passive viewing to mimic animal research; however, passive viewing does not engage psychological processes, which are particularly relevant to clinical anxiety, such as the internal focus on subjective fear levels (i.e., threat appraisal) (5). Threat appraisal can be assessed and compared with other psychological processes by directing participants to focus on a particular question with regard to each stimulus presented. Therefore, for each block, participants used a button press to answer one of three yes or no questions in response to each face: 1) Are you afraid now? (threat appraisal); 2) Did she scream in the past? (explicit memory); and 3) Is her hair jet black? (perceptual discrimination).

Using linear mixed-effects, random-effects analysis in AFNI (Analysis of Functional NeuroImages [http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/afni/]) (21), statistical interactions were tested among between-group factors (diagnosis [anxious, healthy], age group [adult, youth]), within-group factors (cognitive instruction [threat appraisal, explicit memory, and physical discrimination]), and covariates (linear [morph level] and quadratic [morph2-level] trends across morphed images). The linear and quadratic trend regressors consisted of the morphed level for each image and its square, respectively. Scanner and days between fear conditioning and scanning were included as covariates of no interest. Interactions were tested at the whole-brain level and in the ventral region of the medial prefrontal cortex, an a priori region. Because of their relevance to fear conditioning and extinction, the amygdala and the hippocampus were also examined. In our omnibus analysis, we corrected for multiple comparisons using a family-wise-error approach at the cluster level through Monte Carlo simulations. The cluster threshold probability was set at 0.05, with a voxel-wise p value of 0.005. Statistical significance was defined as 90 voxels (1,406 mm3) for whole brain. To restrict the search territory, regional masks of ventral regions of the medial prefrontal cortex, encompassing both the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (11,453 mm3, effectively a 14-mm radius sphere), and the amygdala and hippocampus (1,828 mm3, effectively a 7-mm radius sphere) were used to determine the cluster threshold for a priori regions. Based on calculations, the cluster threshold was 15 voxels (234 mm3) for the medial prefrontal cortex and three voxels (47 mm3) for the amygdala and hippocampus. These thresholds were determined using the AFNI AlphaSim program (the methods are described in the online data supplement).

Post hoc tests were conducted within each significant cluster identified by higher-level interactions (i.e., diagnosis-by-age group-by-instruction-by-morph2 level). Percent-signal-change values, relative to baseline, were averaged across voxels within each significant cluster for each of the 33 effects of interest for all individual participants. Using these extracted values in SPSS (SPSS, Inc., Chicago), linear mixed-model analysis and post hoc tests with Bonferroni correction decomposed statistically significant group differences that emerged in the omnibus analysis. These post hoc tests determined group differences in activation levels and patterns of response (i.e., significant linear and quadratic trends) when participants were directed to different cognitive states (i.e., threat appraisal, explicit memory). Behavioral data were analyzed in a similar manner (see the data supplement).

Results

Several noteworthy outcomes emerged following fear acquisition and extinction. These effects are summarized here, and further details are provided in the online data supplement. First, more anxious than healthy individuals discontinued participation (diagnosis odds ratio: 4.2, p<0.02), as did more youths than adults (odds ratio for a 5-year increment in age: 10.3, p<0.001). Second, both anxiety disorder groups reported higher subjective ratings for both the CS+ and CS– than the two healthy groups (F=19.3, df=1, 109, p<0.001). During fear acquisition, both physiological and subjective measures revealed robust fear conditioning (CS+ > CS–), with similar levels in all groups (skin conductance response: difference between CS+ and CS–, mean=0.1, t=4.8, df=99, p<0.001; startle response: difference between CS+ and CS–, mean=2.0, t=4.3, df=113, p<0.001; subjective fear: difference between CS+ and CS–, mean=1.9, t=7.9, df=113, p<0.001; no significant group effects were found). Finally, during extinction, no diagnostic-group or age-group differences in conditioning levels were demonstrated in skin conductance response. However, anxiety-related differences were detected in subjective response (diagnosis-by-age group-by-phase-by-CS type interaction: F=4.5, df=2, 218, p<0.02) and age-related differences in startle response of conditioned fear (age group-by-phase-by-CS type interaction: F=2.6, df=4, 440, p<0.04).

Behavioral data were collected during scanning several weeks later. Across all groups, a quadratic pattern of online ratings across stimuli was noted for threat appraisal (β=0.003, SE=0.001, t=2.6, df=899, p<0.001) and explicit memory (β=0.008, SE=0.001, t=7.5, df=899, p<0.001), albeit weaker for threat appraisal (instruction-by-morph2-level interaction: F=4.2, df=2, 1931, p<0.01). Although behavior differed based on cognitive instruction, there were no effects of diagnosis or age group on these ratings during scanning (see Figure S2 in the online data supplement). Additional behavioral data are presented in the data supplement.

For the fMRI data, whole-brain random-effects analyses indicated four-way interactions (diagnosis-by-age group-by-instruction-by-morph2 level) in two clusters within the medial prefrontal cortex: the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex/Brodmann’s area [BA] 25/BA 32 ([coordinates: –9, 26, –9], 29 voxels [453 mm3], F=8.5, df=2, 2592, p<0.05, corrected); and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex/BA 32/BA 11 ([coordinates: 4, 49, –6], 43 voxels [672 mm3], F=9.2, df=2, 2592, p<0.05, corrected) (Figure 2). No other regions (i.e., the amygdala or hippocampus) survived a priori cluster-level or whole-brain cluster-level correction.

FIGURE 2. Whole-Brain Analyses Indicate Four-Way Interactions (diagnosis-by-age group-by-cognitive instruction-by-quadratic trend)a

a The subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (coordinates: −9, 26, −9) and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (coordinates: 4, 49, −6) survived a 15-voxel cluster threshold, defined using a voxel-wise probability threshold of p<0.005, which yields a corrected threshold of p<0.05 for medial prefrontal cortical regions. Images are displayed in radiological convention (left is right). Green lines in the axial slice indicate the position of sagittal slices.

To examine higher-level interactions in the significant clusters, the average percent-signal change values, relative to baseline, from voxels within these clusters were extracted for individual participants. Data for each cognitive instruction were examined separately (i.e., threat appraisal, explicit memory) (see the online data supplement).

Threat Appraisal

Patterns in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex differed when participants were instructed to indicate whether or not they were afraid (i.e., threat appraisal). Compared with healthy subjects, anxious patients exhibited subgenual anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivation during threat appraisal (full sample: F=12.1, df=1, 207, p<0.001; adults: F=9.1, df=1, 207, p<0.003; adolescents: F=3.7, df=1, 207, p<0.06), with no diagnosis group-by-age group interaction. The activation in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex also did not vary across morphed images (Figures 3A and 4A).

FIGURE 3. Response Patterns During Threat Appraisal and Explicit Memorya

a The graphs show group differences in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during threat appraisal (panels A and B, respectively) and during the explicit-memory task (panels C and D, respectively). Panel A plots the percent-signal change values relative to baseline. Panels B–D plot the beta coefficients across morphed images for the quadratic response. The significance is defined as an alpha level ≤0.05.

b Group difference in neural activation.

c Significant quadratic trend across morphs.

d Group difference in the quadratic pattern.

FIGURE 4. Response Patterns Across Morphed Imagesa

a To further illustrate effects (as shown in Figure 3), the neural patterns across morphed images are plotted for the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during threat appraisal (panels A and B, respectively) and during the explicit-memory task (panels C and D, respectively). For illustrative purposes, the percent-signal change values, relative to baseline, for every two morphed images other than 0%, 50%, and 100% are averaged together. Only group differences in neural activation are reflected here. Significance is defined as an alpha level ≤0.05.

b Group difference in neural activation.

In contrast, activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during threat appraisal varied with both clinical anxiety and development. First, anxious adults exhibited a unique pattern (diagnosis-by-age group: F=6.1, df=1, 479, p<0.02), revealing hypoactivation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex compared with the other three groups (F=4.9, df=3, 188, p<0.003). Thus, for anxious adults, the pattern of activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during threat appraisal resembled the pattern in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex. Second, anxious youths exhibited a unique pattern across morphed images (diagnosis-by-age group-by-morph2 level: F=4.3, df=1, 540, p<0.04). Unlike the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, the pattern in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in anxious youths differed from that observed in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in anxious adults. Specifically, anxious youths manifested greater activation in response to the morphed images at the extremes of the continuum relative to the blended images compared with both healthy youths and anxious adults (both t values >2.2, df=2536, p<0.03) (Figures 3B and 4B). Thus, anxious youths exhibited a stronger U-shaped response across the morphed images than these groups.

Explicit Memory

The main purpose of the analyses for explicit memory was to demonstrate that findings in threat appraisal, the more clinically relevant state, were task specific. Results suggested that the patterns described above were specific to the threat-appraisal task. When participants were instructed to remember the UCS (i.e., the explicit-memory task), patterns of activation in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex differed from those observed during the threat-appraisal task. Specifically, anxious youths exhibited hyperactivation in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex compared with both healthy groups (diagnosis-by-age group: F=5.2, df=1, 504, p<0.02; F=5.2, df=3, 226, p<0.002). Moreover, unlike during the threat-appraisal task, morph status affected activation in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex during the memory task (diagnosis-by-age group-by-morph2 level: F=6.4, df=1, 542, p<0.02). Specifically, quadratic effects of stimulus class differed between anxious and healthy youths (t=3.1, df=2667, p<0.002) and between healthy adults and healthy youths (t=2.2, df=2667, p<0.03) (Figures 3C and 4C).

Task demands also affected the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, albeit with somewhat more evidence of similarities between the threat-appraisal and explicit-memory tasks than the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex. Thus, both age and diagnosis influenced ventromedial prefrontal cortex response on the explicit-memory task, as they did for the threat-appraisal task; nevertheless, the nature of the findings in the explicit-memory task differed from those in the threat-appraisal task. Specifically, anxious youths manifested signs of overall hyperactivation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex compared with the other groups (F=10.2, df=3, 203, p<0.001). Additionally, healthy youths exhibited significantly less deactivation than anxious adults (p<0.05). Finally, unlike youths, both adult groups exhibited a quadratic pattern of response across the morphed images (both t values >2.4, df=162, p<0.02). This generated a significant difference in patterns across morphed images between healthy youths and both adult groups (both t values >2.3, df=2536, p<0.03) (Figures 3D and 4D).

Discussion

In this study, clinically anxious and healthy adults and adolescents underwent fear conditioning and extinction in the clinic, followed by fMRI scanning while CS+ and similar-appearing stimuli were viewed in different attention states. In the clinic, the healthy and anxious groups differed on overall self-reported fear and on changes in self-reported fear between conditioning and extinction. In the MRI scan, two key findings emerged during events that required participants to make difficult threat-safety discriminations when appraising threat. First, compared with healthy individuals, anxious adults and adolescents both exhibited hypoactivation in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, suggesting the presence of a shared feature in adult and adolescent anxiety disorders. In contrast, there were age-specific findings in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Specifically, compared with age-matched healthy subjects, anxious adults exhibited less activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, whereas anxious adolescents exhibited greater activation in this region in response to the most extreme CS+ and CS– (i.e., greater quadratic response).

Fear Conditioning and Extinction

Data on discontinuation rates and self-reported fear address the ecological validity of the paradigm. We observed high discontinuation rates among anxious youths, demonstrating the difficulty of studying fear in youths. While no adults discontinued, 49% of anxious youths and 14% of healthy youths did. Moreover, youths who discontinued tended to be more anxious than those who did not, suggesting that the paradigm evokes clinically relevant avoidance. Previous research using fear conditioning (22, 23) and similarly aversive procedures in youths also found high discontinuation rates (24, 25). Both anxious adults and youths reported more fear than their respective healthy counterparts in response to both the CS+ and CS–, across both conditioning and extinction. Moreover, compared with the healthy groups, the anxious groups reported less change in fear in response to the CS+ between conditioning and extinction. These findings also address the ecological validity. That is, had the anxious and healthy groups reported similarly low levels of fear, then the clinical relevance of the paradigm would be questioned. Alternatively, had one patient group reported consistently higher fear than the other, then these differences could have affected our fMRI findings.

During both conditioning and extinction, the pattern of results differed between the physiological data and self-reported fear. Such discrepant findings have appeared in previous studies (12, 14). While the anxious and healthy groups differed on self-reported fear in our study, these groups exhibited no such differences on measures of physiology, either during conditioning or extinction. This fear-conditioning study is the first, to our knowledge, to include both anxious youths and anxious adults in a single study, but previous research of each age group alone found results similar to ours (4, 13). That is, previous studies reported both between-group differences in overall levels of self-reported fear and the absence of differences in skin conductance response or startle response (26, 27). Moreover, this discrepancy between self-reported fear and physiology is consistent with a wealth of other research showing that individual differences in reported fear correlate weakly, if at all, with individual differences in skin conductance response or startle response (28, 29).

Shared Features

This study also demonstrated subgenual anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivation during threat appraisal in both adult and adolescent patients with anxiety disorders compared with their healthy counterparts. Such findings are consistent with data implicating the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in emotion regulation and signs of deficient emotion regulation in PTSD. For instance, other studies of healthy adults have linked subgenual anterior cingulate cortex engagement to effective emotion regulation (30, 31), and studies of PTSD have found lower subgenual anterior cingulate cortex function in patients than in healthy adults during exposure to an extinguished CS+ (4).

Observations of subgenual anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivation in this study and in previous studies of anxious adults are notable, given cross-study differences in methodology. For example, shock was used as the UCS in previous studies of adults but not in our study because of concerns about potential harm to adolescents. Moreover, previous studies contrasted activation in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex to extinguished versus nonextinguished CS+ (4, 32). Rather than contrasting such distinct types of CS+, we used stimuli with various degrees of resemblance to a single CS+. Finally, whereas previous studies of adults used passive viewing, we employed an explicit task adapted from previous research, which was designed to demonstrate engagement of a disorder-relevant process. This decision followed best-practice recommendations for fMRI research in difficult-to-study populations, such as adolescents with anxiety disorders (33). In any case, the observation of hypoactivation in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex across methodologically different studies demonstrates the presence of a replicable association in adult anxiety disorders. Our study now extends this observation to adolescents.

By comparing adolescents and adults, this study bridges longitudinal findings on the outcome of adolescent anxiety disorders with fMRI findings on prefrontal cortical dysfunction in adult anxiety disorders. In longitudinal research, most adults with anxiety disorders also met criteria for an anxiety disorder in adolescence. Such findings suggest similar neural correlates in adolescent and adult anxiety disorders. As noted above, fMRI research in adults has found reduced subgenual anterior cingulate cortex function in the context of deficient fear learning, and our study is the first to directly compare fear learning in anxious and healthy adolescents and adults. It is important to do so, since healthy adolescents often differ from healthy adults in fMRI studies of emotional processes. For example, a recent study (12) comparing healthy adults and adolescents using fMRI with the fear conditioning procedures we used here found that functioning differed in healthy adults and adolescents in both the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex.

Our research demonstrating subgenual anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in both adolescents and adults with anxiety disorders is also relevant to research using both animal models and novel therapeutics. For example, rodent studies have found that lesions to the infra-limbic cortex, a possible rodent homologue of the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, interfere with extinction (7, 34). Such findings have generated interest in novel treatments for adult anxiety disorders that may reduce anxiety through increasing subgenual anterior cingulate cortex function; our findings suggest that such treatments may also be effective in adolescent anxiety disorders.

Age-Specific Features

Another result of this study concerns age-specific dysfunction in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Whereas anxious adults exhibited hypoactivation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during threat appraisal across the range of stimuli, anxious adolescents exhibited enhanced ventromedial prefrontal cortex response to the most extreme CS+ and CS–. This pattern could reflect heightened sensitivity in anxious adolescents in response to novelty, predictability, safety (26), or dissimilarity (19), especially when appraising threat.

These data raise questions about other differences between adolescent and adult anxiety disorders. As noted above, adolescent anxiety disorders predict high risk for adult anxiety disorders. Nevertheless, there is also discontinuity in longitudinal outcome. While the overwhelming majority of adults with an anxiety disorder suffered from an anxiety disorder in adolescence, only about one-half of adolescents with an anxiety disorder continue to manifest anxiety as adults (2). This divergence suggests that adolescent anxiety disorders are heterogeneous and include subgroups of individuals who will mature to account for the majority of adult anxiety disorders and other subgroups who will remit. Differences in ventromedial prefrontal cortex function among anxious adolescents and adults could relate to these varying longitudinal trajectories. For example, changes in ventromedial prefrontal cortex function from adolescence through adulthood could influence anxiety symptom trajectories, thus mediating the adult outcome of adolescent anxiety disorders. Alternatively, the level of ventromedial prefrontal cortex dysfunction present in adolescence could define distinct subgroups of anxiety disorders and predict outcome. For example, the subgroup of anxious adolescents with ventromedial prefrontal cortex hypoactivation, a deficit characteristic of adult disorders, may be at particularly high risk for persistence. Other adolescents who exhibit elevated activation may experience a better long-term outcome. As noted in Figure 3B, activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in anxious youths demonstrated a quadratic pattern during the threat-appraisal task, albeit with marked variability. The subgroup of anxious youths with particularly low activation (i.e., those whose activation overlaps with that of anxious adults) may face high risk for adulthood anxiety.

Limitations

Several limitations of our study should be mentioned. First, the broad age range of the participants may obscure developmentally sensitive processes (35). For example, evidence suggests that it may be more difficult to extinguish conditioned fear in early adolescence compared with late adolescence (34). Second, the youngest participants were the least likely to complete the procedures, raising questions about the degree to which comparable findings would occur with more tolerable procedures for younger patients. However, to generate comparable findings, it would be important for future research with alternative procedures to elicit robust group differences in avoidance and subjective fear, as did the paradigm in this study. Third, while group differences emerged in prefrontal cortical function, corresponding differences in task-elicited behavior were not found. Some investigators view the absence of such differences as a limitation, whereas others argue that important experimental confounders are removed (33). Fourth, data on peripheral physiology were acquired only during the first visit. Future studies should acquire physiological and neural data simultaneously, although including physiological data collection would lengthen the fMRI procedures and potentially reduce their tolerability. Finally, while the anxious and healthy groups were matched on key variables, IQ differed between adolescents and adults. Nevertheless, IQ showed no associations with any dependent measure, and group differences in brain function persisted in subgroups matched on both age and IQ.

Conclusions

No previous fMRI study has compared healthy and anxious adolescents and adults in the context of a fear-learning paradigm. This study specifically examined threat appraisal and explicit memory during exposure to CS+ in adolescents and adults following conditioning and extinction. Using a paradigm that evokes elevated fear in adolescents and adults with an anxiety disorder, we found hypoactivation in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in anxious adolescents and adults compared with healthy adolescents and adults. Such hypoactivation occurred in both age groups specifically when appraising the fear response. In contrast, activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex differentiated anxious adults and adolescents both from each other and their age-matched healthy counterparts.

From the Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, Fla.; the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Md.; Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis; School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore; and Department of Psychology, Boston University, Boston.

Presented at the annual meeting of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, Miami Beach, Fla., Dec. 5–9, 2010, and Waikoloa, Hawaii, Dec. 4–8, 2011; and the annual meeting of the Anxiety and Depression Association of America, Baltimore, March 4–7, 2010, and New Orleans, March 24–27, 2011.

Address correspondence to Dr. Britton ().

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supplementary Material

Supported in part by grant K99-MH-091183 from the Intramural Research Program of NIMH/NIH (to Dr. Britton).

The skin conductance results during fear acquisition partially overlap with data published by Lau et al. (see reference 12). The data are presented as an applied example of the methodology described by Chen et al. (see reference 21).

The authors thank Marilla Geraci for assistance with recruitment and David Luckenbaugh for assistance with statistical data.

References

1 Quirk GJ, Garcia R, González-Lima F: Prefrontal mechanisms in extinction of conditioned fear. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60:337–343Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Pine DS, Cohen P, Gurley D, Brook J, Ma Y: The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:56–64Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Walkup JT, Albano AM, Piacentini J, Birmaher B, Compton SN, Sherrill JT, Ginsburg GS, Rynn MA, McCracken J, Waslick B, Iyengar S, March JS, Kendall PC: Cognitive behavioral therapy, sertraline, or a combination in childhood anxiety. N Engl J Med 2008; 359:2753–2766Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Milad MR, Pitman RK, Ellis CB, Gold AL, Shin LM, Lasko NB, Zeidan MA, Handwerger K, Orr SP, Rauch SL: Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 66:1075–1082Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Britton JC, Lissek S, Grillon C, Norcross MA, Pine DS: Development of anxiety: the role of threat appraisal and fear learning. Depress Anxiety 2011; 28:5–17Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Kim JH, Richardson R: The effect of temporary amygdala inactivation on extinction and reextinction of fear in the developing rat: unlearning as a potential mechanism for extinction early in development. J Neurosci 2008; 28:1282–1290Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Milad MR, Quirk GJ: Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature 2002; 420:70–74Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Quirk GJ, Beer JS: Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: convergence of rat and human studies. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2006; 16:723–727Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Indovina I, Robbins TW, Núñez-Elizalde AO, Dunn BD, Bishop SJ: Fear-conditioning mechanisms associated with trait vulnerability to anxiety in humans. Neuron 2011; 69:563–571Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ: Fear conditioning in humans: the influence of awareness and autonomic arousal on functional neuroanatomy. Neuron 2002; 33:653–663Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Kalisch R, Korenfeld E, Stephan KE, Weiskopf N, Seymour B, Dolan RJ: Context-dependent human extinction memory is mediated by a ventromedial prefrontal and hippocampal network. J Neurosci 2006; 26:9503–9511Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Lau JY, Britton JC, Nelson EE, Angold A, Ernst M, Goldwin M, Grillon C, Leibenluft E, Lissek S, Norcross M, Shiffrin N, Pine DS: Distinct neural signatures of threat learning in adolescents and adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011; 108:4500–4505Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Lau JY, Lissek S, Nelson EE, Lee Y, Roberson-Nay R, Poeth K, Jenness J, Ernst M, Grillon C, Pine DS: Fear conditioning in adolescents with anxiety disorders: results from a novel experimental paradigm. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008; 47:94–102Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Lissek S, Powers AS, McClure EB, Phelps EA, Woldehawariat G, Grillon C, Pine DS: Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther 2005; 43:1391–1424Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 2002Google Scholar

16 Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N: Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:980–988Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Lissek S, Rabin S, Heller RE, Lukenbaugh D, Geraci M, Pine DS, Grillon C: Overgeneralization of conditioned fear as a pathogenic marker of panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:47–55LinkGoogle Scholar

18 Lissek S, Rabin SJ, McDowell DJ, Dvir S, Bradford DE, Geraci M, Pine DS, Grillon C: Impaired discriminative fear-conditioning resulting from elevated fear responding to learned safety cues among individuals with panic disorder. Behav Res Ther 2009; 47:111–118Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Greenberg T, Carlson JM, Cha J, Hajcak G, Mujica-Parodi LR: Neural reactivity tracks fear generalization gradients. Biol Psychol 2013; 92:2–8Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Lissek S, Biggs AL, Rabin SJ, Cornwell BR, Alvarez RP, Pine DS, Grillon C: Generalization of conditioned fear-potentiated startle in humans: experimental validation and clinical relevance. Behav Res Ther 2008; 46:678–687Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Chen G, Saad ZS, Britton JC, Pine DS, Cox RW: Linear mixed-effects modeling approach to FMRI group analysis. Neuroimage 2013; 73:176–190Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Glenn CR, Klein DN, Lissek S, Britton JC, Pine DS, Hajcak G: The Development of Fear Learning and Generalization in 8 to 13 year-olds. Dev Psychobiol 2012; 54:675–684Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Neumann DL, Waters AM, Westbury HR, Henry J: The use of an unpleasant sound unconditional stimulus in an aversive conditioning procedure with 8- to 11-year-old children. Biol Psychol 2008; 79:337–342Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Roberson-Nay R, Klein DF, Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Moulton JL, Guardino M, Pine DS: Carbon dioxide hypersensitivity in separation-anxious offspring of parents with panic disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67:1171–1177Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Pine DS, Klein RG, Coplan JD, Papp LA, Hoven CW, Martinez J, Kovalenko P, Mandell DJ, Moreau D, Klein DF, Gorman JM: Differential carbon dioxide sensitivity in childhood anxiety disorders and nonill comparison group. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:960–967Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Milad MR, Wright CI, Orr SP, Pitman RK, Quirk GJ, Rauch SL: Recall of fear extinction in humans activates the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in concert. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:446–454Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Phelps EA, Delgado MR, Nearing KI, LeDoux JE: Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron 2004; 43:897–905Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Blechert J, Michael T, Vriends N, Margraf J, Wilhelm FH. Fear conditioning in posttraumatic stress disorder: evidence for delayed extinction of autonomic, experiential, and behavioural responses. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45:2019–2033Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Craske MG, Waters AM, Lindsey Bergman R, Naliboff B, Lipp OV, Negoro H, Ornitz EM: Is aversive learning a marker of risk for anxiety disorders in children? Behav Res Ther 2008; 46:954–967Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Delgado MR, Nearing KI, Ledoux JE, Phelps EA: Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction. Neuron 2008; 59:829–838Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Dunsmoor JE, Prince SE, Murty VP, Kragel PA, LaBar KS: Neurobehavioral mechanisms of human fear generalization. Neuroimage 2011; 55:1878–1888Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Milad MR, Quirk GJ, Pitman RK, Orr SP, Fischl B, Rauch SL: A role for the human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in fear expression. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:1191–1194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Carter CS, Heckers S, Nichols T, Pine DS, Strother S: Optimizing the design and analysis of clinical functional magnetic resonance imaging research studies. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64:842–849 [Review]Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Kim JH, Richardson R: New findings on extinction of conditioned fear early in development: theoretical and clinical implications. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67:297–303Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Casey BJ, Ruberry EJ, Libby V, Glatt CE, Hare T, Soliman F, Duhoux S, Frielingsdorf H, Tottenham N: Transitional and translational studies of risk for anxiety. Depress Anxiety 2011; 28:18–28Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar