The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a highly heritable but clinically heterogeneous syndrome. The study examined the familiality and heritability of ADHD subtypes as defined by DSM-IV and by latent-class analysis in a population sample of adolescent female twins. METHOD: To determine which elements of ADHD cluster together, latent-class analysis was applied to data obtained from parents on the 18 DSM-IV ADHD symptoms in 4,036 female twins age 13–23 years in a population sample identified from the registry of all births in Missouri for the years 1968–1996. Relative risk and odds ratios were used to assess within-subtype and between-subtype familiality and heritability of both DSM-IV and latent-class ADHD subtypes. RESULTS: Latent-class analysis was most compatible with the existence of three mild and three severe classes of ADHD symptoms in the general population. The three severe classes showed moderate overlap with DSM-IV ADHD subtypes. The primarily inattentive and combined subtypes of DSM-IV ADHD co-clustered within families. The primarily hyperactive/impulsive DSM-IV subtype and the individual latent-class analysis subtypes did not co-cluster. Subtypes defined by both approaches were highly heritable. CONCLUSIONS: Unlike DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD, latent-class ADHD subtypes appear to be independently transmitted in families. These classes may be more appropriate targets for molecular genetic studies of ADHD.

The familial nature of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has been recognized since the turn of the last century (1). Subsequent twin studies from several countries have estimated the heritability of ADHD to be between 0.6 and 0.9 (reviewed in references 2, 3). Despite the overwhelming evidence for genetic influences on ADHD, there is controversy regarding how to conceptualize ADHD symptoms in genetic studies (for example, references 2, 4, 5). Because of high rates of comorbidity and concerns about the validity of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes (2, 3, 6), it is possible that alternative ways of conceptualizing the ADHD phenotype may identify more “pure” phenotypes.

A major step in elucidating the genetic elements contributing to a disorder is determining what aspects of the disorder are heritable. Despite the almost universal reliance of current candidate gene and genetic linkage studies of ADHD on DSM-IV nosology, very little evidence has been presented regarding the relative heritabilities or familial specificities of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes. Recently, three studies have tested for family specificity of DSM-IV ADHD in first-degree relatives of subjects with ADHD identified from clinics or by advertisement (79). All these studies failed to identify significant family clustering of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes and concluded that symptom variability is largely a function of nongenetic, nonfamilial causes. To our knowledge, no similar analyses of community or population-based families have been published.

In previous studies we have attempted to address whether DSM-IV ADHD subtypes are an appropriate nosology for ADHD by applying clustering techniques to population- and family-based data on the prevalence of individual ADHD symptoms (5, 10, 11). One such technique is latent-class analysis, which assumes categorical rather than continuous latent variables (“classes”) (12). In this approach the observed pattern of symptom clustering is assumed to be explained by the existence of discrete groups with different symptom profiles. This approach has three specific advantages for analysis of the nature of ADHD. First, if there are multiple independent types of ADHD, then the patterns of endorsement of the 18 DSM-IV ADHD symptoms associated with each independent ADHD subtype will be grouped together within independent classes. Second, the probability of endorsement of individual symptoms given membership in a latent class is quantified. If the underlying nature of the phenotype is a continuum of problems with inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, or both, then symptom endorsement profiles of the observed classes will reflect differences in severity or frequency of the reporting of symptoms only. Finally, on the basis of the most likely class, individuals can be assigned to individual latent classes to allow the identification of specific groups for further analyses, such as analyses of family clustering. With this approach we have been able to demonstrate that latent class analysis results in a limited number of classes of ADHD symptoms in both general population twin (5) and family-based (10) samples. The observed latent classes to some extent recapitulate the DSM-IV categorization because classes resembling DSM-IV inattentive, hyperactive/impulsive, and combined subtypes of ADHD are identified. However, these classes do not contain all individuals with DSM-IV ADHD and also include many individuals who do not meet DSM-IV criteria for ADHD. More recently, we have been able to demonstrate that the majority of comorbid problems associated with ADHD are found in a single latent class corresponding to, but defined somewhat more broadly than, the DSM-IV combined subtype of ADHD (11). Our initial conclusion about the underlying nature of ADHD on the basis of these analyses was that there were three continua representing domains of problems with inattention, problems of hyperactivity/impulsivity, and combined problems (5). However, the observed concordance for same-class membership between monozygotic and dizygotic twins is also compatible with there being discreet clinically significant subtypes that have high heritability in each of these domains (10).

In the current study we test two hypotheses in a population sample of female twins drawn from birth records. First, DSM-IV ADHD subtypes do not “breed true,” that is to say that there is no tendency for particular subtypes to cluster exclusively in some families. Second, latent-class ADHD subtypes do “breed true.” Both hypotheses are tested by computing the within-class and between-class concordance rates for monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs. Our results are most compatible with the presence of independent, familial forms of ADHD defined by latent-class analysis that are imperfectly operationalized by DSM-IV criteria, giving rise to the familial coaggregation of different DSM-IV subtypes that others have reported (79). The results suggest that the current DSM-IV subtypes may not be optimal for use as phenotypes for molecular genetic studies.

Method

Subjects

The current data represent an expanded sample of our original report (5) consisting of parent reports on 4,036 female twins aged 13–23 years at the time of the interview. Twin births were identified from the registry of all births in the state of Missouri for the years 1968 through 1996 as a part of an ongoing prospective study of alcohol problems and associated behavioral problems in young women (13). After the study was explained and written consent was obtained, diagnostic interview data were collected by telephone from parents about their twins as described by Hudziak et al. (5). The parent interview about their twins was adapted for telephone administration from the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents—Revised (14), the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (15), and the child version of the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (15). The test-retest kappas for individual ADHD symptoms and for DSM-IV ADHD subtype diagnoses were ≥0.9 for this interview (unpublished 2001 paper of A. Heath, et al.). DSM-IV diagnoses were derived by computer algorithm. For the current sample, the biological mother was the reporting parent for 93.7% of interviews, and the biological father for 5.2% of interviews; 1.1% of the interviews were with a close nonparental relative or guardian of the twins. Determinations of zygosity were based on a standard series of questions about zygosity (13).

Latent-Class Analysis

Latent-class models were fitted to the parent’s responses about their offspring for the 18 DSM-IV ADHD symptoms (1012). The latent-class analysis software program LCAP (10), in which maximum likelihood methods were implemented by the EM algorithm, was used to fit solutions with one to 10 classes to these data. The parameters of the latent-class model are fully described elsewhere (10, 11) but for the reader’s convenience are presented briefly here. As in previous analyses, the final choice of latent-class model was based on several criteria, including improvement in likelihood ratios, the stability of class membership, and the prevalence of new classes when solutions that estimate higher numbers of classes are used. Latent-class analysis estimates two types of parameters: 1) the prevalence of each class (that is, the prior probability that randomly chosen individuals will be in each latent class) and 2) the conditional probabilities describing the distribution of the responses to each symptom within a class. Latent-class analysis as applied here does not attempt to model twin pair resemblance and thus does not impose constraints on whether or not sibling concordance that is greater than chance assignment of sibling pairs to the same class occurs. When the best model was determined, each twin was allocated to the most probable latent class given the twin’s symptom profile. Owing to space constraints, only the summary characteristics of the best latent-class solution are reported here. The estimated endorsement probabilities for each symptom by latent class are shown in Table 1 and are not substantively different from those reported by Hudziak et al. (5).

Family and Genetic Modeling

The importance of family/genetic contributions to DSM-IV ADHD subtype and latent-class membership was estimated in several ways. First, whether ADHD, per se, was familial and whether DSM-IV or latent-class ADHD subtypes represented distinct familial entities were estimated by testing whether both monozygotic and dizygotic co-twins were assigned to the same ADHD subtype or latent class more frequently than expected by chance. Fisher’s exact test was used for these analyses. Second, whether the DSM-IV or latent-class ADHD subtypes represent independent genetic entities was tested by computing the odds ratios for having a given subtype for monozygotic versus dizygotic pairs (with 95% confidence intervals). If one or more genetically independent classes are found, the odds ratio for being assigned to the same latent class conditioned on zygosity should be significantly greater than 1 and the odds ratio for cross-class membership should be nonsignificant or less than 1. Finally, the recurrence risk ratio (λ; the probability of a twin having a given ADHD subtype conditional on the co-twin having that subtype relative to the sample probability of any twin having that subtype [16]) was computed and 95% confidence intervals [CIs] were determined. Separate recurrence risk ratios were calculated for monozygotic (λMZ) and dizygotic (λDZ) pairs. Because of the familial nature of our data, a bootstrap approach was used to compute confidence intervals for the odds ratios by using 1,000 bootstrapped samples. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals were derived by using the bias-corrected intervals described by Efron and Tibshirani (reference 17, pp. 184–188), with the bias correction parameter determined by the proportion of bootstrap replicates that were less than the original estimate of the odds ratio (17, p. 186) and the acceleration parameter equal to 0.

Results

The data consist of parent reports on 4,036 adolescent female twins in 1,127 monozygotic pairs and 891 dizygotic pairs identified through birth records of the state of Missouri. African Americans constituted 13.3% of the sample. The majority of the sample was European American; less than 1% were Hispanic, Asian American, Native American, or of other self-identified ancestries. The average age of the twins at time of the parental interview was 14.9 years (SD=2.7).

Table 1 presents the frequencies of the DSM-IV and latent-class ADHD subtypes as well as the frequencies with which parents endorsed the DSM-IV inattention and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms for their children. The prevalences of DSM-IV inattentive, combined, and hyperactive/impulsive ADHD subtypes were 3.8%, 2.0%, and 0.7%, respectively. Latent-class models of one to 10 classes were fitted, with significant improvement in likelihood ratios as additional classes were added (data available on request). Estimated prevalences of latent classes became stable while progressing from an eight- to a nine-class solution, with additional classes representing less than 1% of the study population. Similarly, few twins were reassigned to different classes when the number of classes increased from an eight- to a nine-class solution. Consequently, the eight-class solution was selected as the best-fitting model for further analyses. The class prevalences and the endorsement frequencies of inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms are remarkably similar to those in our previous latent-class analysis, which included a smaller sample (5). There are classes that have mild or marked endorsement frequencies of inattentive symptoms (mild inattentive and severe inattentive classes), hyperactive/impulsive symptoms (mild hyperactive-impulsive and severe hyperactive-impulsive classes), or both types of symptoms (mild combined and severe combined classes). The talkative/impulsive class was characterized by frequent endorsement of the symptom of “talks excessively” (67.2%) but relatively low levels of endorsement of inattentive and hyperactive ADHD symptoms. The “few symptoms” class was characterized by infrequent (<5%) endorsement of all symptoms.

Table 2 displays the cross-tabulation of DSM-IV and latent-class ADHD subtypes. As in our previous latent-class analysis with a smaller sample (5), there was almost complete assignment of the DSM-IV combined subtype to the severe combined latent class (79 of 80 subjects with the DSM-IV ADHD combined subtype). However, approximately 21% (N=32 of 152) of those diagnosed with the DSM-IV inattentive subtype were also assigned to this latent class as were 32 individuals with no DSM-IV ADHD diagnosis (0.8% of 3,775). About 63% of DSM-IV inattention subtype diagnoses were assigned to either mild or severe inattention classes, and about 37% were assigned to either mild or severe combined classes. About 86% of the subjects with the DSM-IV hyperactive/impulsive subtype were assigned to the severe hyperactive/impulsive latent class, and 14% were assigned to other latent classes. In contrast to the severe combined latent class, which included subjects with both the DSM-IV inattentive and the DSM-IV combined ADHD subtypes, the severe inattentive latent class contained only subjects with the DSM-IV inattentive subtype and the severe hyperactive/impulsive latent class contained only subjects with the DSM-IV hyperactive/impulsive subtype. The three severe latent classes also contained many individuals with no DSM-IV ADHD diagnosis. Individuals with no DSM-IV ADHD diagnosis constituted 46% of the subjects in the severe inattentive latent class, 22% in the severe combined class, and 72% in the severe hyperactive/impulsive class. For the severe inattentive and the severe hyperactive/impulsive latent classes, there were no significant differences in the average number of endorsed inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms or in probability of class assignment for class members with and without a DSM-IV ADHD diagnosis. Compared with the severe combined latent class members without an ADHD diagnosis, class members with a DSM-IV ADHD diagnosis had somewhat more inattentive symptoms (a mean of 8.3 [SD=0.9] versus 7.4 [SD=1.1]) (Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney χ2=16.9, df=1, p<0.001) and a slightly higher probability of class assignment (0.95 versus 0.90) (Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney χ2=15.6, df=1, p<0.001).

In its most general form, the familiality of DSM-IV ADHD can be estimated by comparing the concordance of sibling pairs for the presence or absence of any ADHD subtype. Since this study included a population sample, each subject with ADHD was treated as a proband in this analysis. Sixty-eight percent of monozygotic twin pairs and 22% of dizygotic twin pairs were concordant for DSM-IV ADHD of any subtype (Table 3). These proportions represent an odds ratio of having a second sibling with ADHD of 127.5 (95% CI=75.6–215.0, p<0.001, Fisher’s exact test) and 3.7 (95% CI=2.4–5.6, p<0.001, Fisher’s exact test) for monozygotic and dizygotic siblings, respectively. Familiality was also significant when DSM-IV subtypes were individually analyzed. The odds ratios of having a sibling with any type of DSM-IV ADHD were 73.9 (95% CI=41.6–131.1, p<0.001, Fisher’s exact test) and 3.1 (95% CI=1.8–5.4, p<0.001, Fisher’s exact test), for monozygotic and dizygotic siblings, respectively, for the inattentive subtype; 111.7 (95% CI=44.8–278.6, p<0.001, Fisher’s exact test) and 3.4 (95% CI=1.7–6.7, p<0.001, Fisher’s exact test), respectively, for the combined subtype; and 5.0 (95% CI=1.1–23.8, p<0.02, Fisher’s exact test) and 3.9 (95% CI=1.4–11.1, p<0.005, Fisher’s exact test), respectively, for the hyperactive/impulsive subtype.

The hypothesis of a lack of familial specificity of DSM-IV subtypes can be tested by comparing the concordance of sibling pairs for ADHD subtype. In more than 75% of the instances when an affected monozygotic twin had an affected co-twin, the twins had the same DSM-IV ADHD subtype. Co-twins of a twin with the inattentive subtype were significantly more likely to have the inattentive and the combined subtypes (p<0.0001, Fisher’s exact test, for both comparisons) but not the hyperactive/impulsive subtype (p=0.99, Fisher’s exact test). Similarly, co-twins of a twin with the combined subtype were significantly more likely to have the combined subtype (p<0.0001, Fisher’s exact test) and the inattentive subtype (p<0.001, Fisher’s exact test) but not the hyperactive/impulsive subtype (p<0.99, Fisher’s exact test). Monozygotic twins with the hyperactive/impulsive subtype were more likely to have a co-twin with the hyperactive/impulsive subtype (p<0.0001, Fisher’s exact test) but not the inattentive or the combined subtype (p=0.14 and p=0.54, respectively, Fisher’s exact test). For dizygotic pairs, concordance for same subtype was significantly more likely for the inattentive DSM-IV subtype (p<0.005, Fisher’s exact test), but there was no significant concordance for the combined or the hyperactive/impulsive subtypes (p=0.11 and p=0.14, respectively, Fisher’s exact test). These findings are compatible with a lack of family specificity for the DSM-IV combined and inattentive subtypes and suggest that the hyperactive/impulsive subtype may be independent of the other two subtypes.

The hypothesis that latent-class ADHD subtypes are family specific can also be tested by comparing the concordance of sibling pairs for the same or different latent-class ADHD subtypes. The probability that co-twins are of the same latent-class ADHD subtype ranged from 63% to 90% for monozygotic pairs (range of odds ratios=28.7–488.4) and from 12% to 72% for dizygotic pairs (range of odds ratios=3.2–14.6) (Table 4). The concordance rates were highly significant (p≤0.001, Fisher’s exact test) for all monozygotic and dizygotic same-class comparisons (analyses not shown but easily computable from the data in Table 4). There were no significant positive associations for cross-class pairs for either monozygotic or dizygotic twins (p>0.1, Fisher’s exact test). These findings are compatible with a high level of family specificity for all of the latent-class ADHD subtypes.

To test whether the phenotypically distinct DSM-IV and latent-class ADHD subtypes represent independent genetic forms of ADHD phenotypes, we compared the frequencies of the same subtype in monozygotic versus dizygotic twins (Table 3, Table 4, and Table 5). For all eight latent classes and for the inattentive and combined DSM-IV ADHD subtypes there were significantly more monozygotic twins in the same class than dizygotic twins in the same class (Table 3 and Table 4). As shown in Table 5, the odds ratios for monozygotic twins in the same class versus dizygotic twins in the same class ranged from 3.6 to 17.4. These results are consistent with a genetic basis for membership in both DSM-IV and latent-class ADHD subtypes. We similarly compared frequencies and odds ratios for monozygotic versus dizygotic twins for all six possible DSM-IV ADHD cross-subtype combinations and all 28 possible cross-subtype combinations of latent-class subtype. For DSM-IV subtypes, there was a significant cross-subtype odds ratio greater than 1 for the inattentive and combined subtypes (odds ratio=6.9, 95% CI=1.6–29.1). All other cross-subtype comparisons were nonsignificant or had odds ratios less than 1. Of the 28 possible latent-class combinations, only the combination of the mild inattentive subtype with the few symptoms subtype had an odds ratio above 1 (odds ratio=2.2, 95% CI=1.6–3.2). This later finding is of questionable significance owing to the number of comparisons across latent classes. These findings suggest that ADHD latent classes approximate pure genetic categories and that the inattentive and combined DSM-IV ADHD subtypes do not.

To allow comparison of our findings with those of other studies, we calculated the sibling recurrence risk ratio (λS) (16) for the three DSM-IV ADHD subtypes and the eight latent-class subtypes (Table 5). This ratio is an estimate of familiality adjusted for the population prevalence of a given disorder. For both DSM-IV and latent-class analysis approaches, the values of λS are greater for monozygotic than for dizygotic twins, consistent with a genetic contribution to liability. The magnitudes of the observed values for λDZ for the DSM-IV ADHD subtypes are similar to those reported for nontwin siblings (18). It should be noted, however, that because of the low base rate of ADHD in girls, even in a population sample of this size, the 95% confidence intervals for several DSM-IV subtypes and latent classes in dizygotic twins are large and include 1.

Discussion

The current study of a large sample of female twins selected from birth records replicates and expands on our previous findings (5, 10, 11). The inclusion of a larger sample allowed us to estimate the familiality and heritability of latent-class and DSM-IV ADHD subtype membership. In contrast to our previous findings (5), the current data are most compatible with the presence of distinct and heritable ADHD phenotypes that differ in the relative contributions of individual inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms.

The analyses of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes in this population-based sample replicate the findings of the nonspecificity of DSM-IV subtypes in families from clinically referred or volunteer samples (79). In a study of families recruited through clinics, Faraone et al. (7, 8) refuted the prediction of specificity of the DSM-IV inattentive and combined subtypes. In contrast, for the hyperactive/impulsive subtype of DSM-IV ADHD in boys they found almost complete exclusivity of hyperactive/impulsive subjects among relatives of hyperactive/impulsive probands (8). Smalley et al. (9) also found no specificity of the inattentive and combined subtypes but could not test for specificity of the hyperactive/impulsive subtype since they pooled together data from the families of probands with the combined and hyperactive/impulsive subtypes. In the current study we support the lack of family specificity of the DSM-IV inattentive and combined subtypes and also support the specificity of the DSM-IV hyperactive/impulsive subtype. When twin families were subtyped with latent-class criteria, however, the nonspecificity of the DSM-IV subtypes was resolved into specific familial conditions. These results are most compatible with the presence of independent, familial forms of ADHD that are approximated by latent-class analysis and are imperfectly operationalized by DSM-IV criteria.

A variety of previous studies have also attempted to define more heritable forms of ADHD by examining comorbid psychiatric disorders or other medical conditions (reviewed in references 2, 3, 6). In particular, Faraone et al. (18) suggested that restriction of cases to persistent ADHD (the full diagnosis persists into young adulthood) or restricting cases to ADHD with comorbid conduct disorder or bipolar affective disorder leads to findings of a higher prevalence of recurrence of the illness among family members, relative to the general population. Although conduct disorder was rare and mania was not assessed in the current population sample of female twins, the majority of subjects with mood disorders and other disruptive behavior disorders that were comorbid with DSM-IV ADHD were included in the combined latent-class subtype (11).

These results have implications for the design of molecular genetic studies to identify specific DNA sequences involved in the etiology of ADHD. Most ADHD candidate gene and genetic linkage studies use probands identified through clinical sampling or advertisement who meet DSM-IV criteria for ADHD. If the results of the current study are correct, then such recruitment strategies combine several genetically independent forms of ADHD while excluding many individuals with heritable ADHD subtypes who do not meet DSM-IV criteria. Such sampling strategies greatly reduce the power of both association and linkage studies to detect causative genes. Such mixing of heritable types of ADHD may explain the equivocal reports of the association of ADHD with candidate genes. It is interesting to note that two studies have reported that restricting findings to inattentive symptoms or subtypes increases the association of ADHD with polymorphisms of the DRD4 locus (19, 20). Such family samples could be divided into more homogenous groups by application of the current types of analyses.

Although the ADHD classes described here have been demonstrated in both family (10) and community twin samples (5), the current study has several limitations, including the use of a twin sample, the restriction of data to parents’ reports about their children, and the restriction of the sample to adolescent females. The observed distribution of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes and the risk ratios for female dizygotic twins, as previously reported for male and female siblings in nontwin families (79, 18), suggests that any biases introduced by including only female twins are minor. In addition, the frequencies of dizygotic twins being in the same latent class are similar to those found for male and female nontwin siblings (10). Most studies of the heritability of ADHD have used parents’ reports about male children. It is of obvious interest to expand the current findings by including twins’ self-reports and teachers’ reports. Critical tests of the familiality of these classes in male subjects, as well as the prevalence of individual classes in a nontwin community sample, await the application of these analytic approaches to other data sets.

TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3
TABLE 4
TABLE 5

Presented in part at the 1999 World Congress on Psychiatric Genetics, Monterey, Calif., Oct. 14–18, 1999. Received Dec. 4, 2000; revision received May 4, 2001; accepted June 1, 2001. From the Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine; and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, Burlington. Address reprint requests to Dr. Todd, Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, Box 8134, 660 S. Euclid, St. Louis, MO 63110; (e-mail). Supported by grants MH-52813 and MH-31302 from NIMH; grants AA-10111, AA-11998, AA-12239, and AA-07728 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; and grants DA-00272 and DA-12540 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. The Missouri Adolescent Female Twin Study (MOAFTS) is supported by grant AA-09022 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.

References

1. Still GF: Some abnormal psychical conditions in children. Lancet 1902; 1:1008-1012, 1077-1082, 1163-1168Google Scholar

2. Todd RD: Genetics of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: are we ready for molecular genetic studies? Am J Med Genet Neuropsychiatry Genet 2000; 96:241-243CrossrefGoogle Scholar

3. Faraone SV, Biederman J: Neurobiology of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44:951-958Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Levy F, Hay D, McStephen M, Wood C, Waldman I: Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. a category or a continuum? genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:737-744Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Hudziak JJ, Heath AC, Madden PF, Reich W, Bucholz KK, Slutske W, Bierut LJ, Neuman RJ, Todd RD: Latent class and factor analysis of DSM-IV ADHD: a twin study of female adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998; 37:848-857Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Hudziak JJ, Todd RD: Familial subtyping of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry 1993; 6:489-493CrossrefGoogle Scholar

7. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Mick E, Williamson S, Wilens T, Spencer T, Weber W, Jetton J, Kraus I, Pert J, Zallen B: Family study of girls with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1077-1083LinkGoogle Scholar

8. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Friedman D: Validity of DSM-IV subtypes of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a family perspective. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:300-307Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Smalley SL, McGough JJ, Del’Homme M, NewDelman J, Gordon E, Kim T, Liu A, McCracken JT: Familial clustering of symptoms and disruptive behaviors in multiplex families with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:1135-1143Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Neuman RJ, Todd RD, Heath AC, Reich W, Hudziak JJ, Bucholz KK, Madden PAF, Begleiter H, Porjesz B, Kuperman S, Hesselbrock V, Reich T: The evaluation of ADHD typology in three contrasting samples: a latent class approach. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38:25-33Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Neuman RJ, Heath AC, Hudziak JJ, Reich W, Bucholz KK, Madden PAF, Todd RD: Latent class analysis of ADHD and comorbid symptoms in a population sample of adolescent female twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (in press)Google Scholar

12. McCutcheon AL: Latent Class Analysis. Newbury Park, Calif, Sage Publications, 1987Google Scholar

13. Heath AC, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK: Ascertainment of a twin sample by computerized record matching with assessment of possible sampling biases. Behav Genet 2000; 29:209-219CrossrefGoogle Scholar

14. Reich W: Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (DICA). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:59-66Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, Dinwiddie SH, Hesselbrock VM, Nurnberger JI Jr, Reich T, Schmidt I, Schuckit MA: A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: a report of the reliability of the SSAGA. J Stud Alcohol 1994; 55:149-158Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Risch N: Linkage strategies for genetically complex traits, I: multilocus models. Am J Hum Genet 1990; 46:222-228MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Efron B, Tibshirani RJ: An Introduction to the Bootstrap. Boston, Chapman & Hall, 1993Google Scholar

18. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Monuteaux M: Toward guidelines for pedigree selection in genetic studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Genet Epidemiol 2000; 18:1-16Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Rowe DC, Stever C, Giedinghagen LN, Gard JM, Cleveland HH, Terris ST, Mohr JH, Sherman S, Abramowitz A, Waldman ID: Dopamine DRD4 receptor polymorphism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry 1998; 3:419-426Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. McCracken JT, Smalley SL, McGough JJ, Crawford L, Del’Homme M, Cantor RM, Liu A, Nelson SF: Evidence for linkage of a tandem duplication polymorphism upstream of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Mol Psychiatry 2000; 5:531-536Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar