The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.157.1.26

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Abnormal activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and a related cortical network during working memory tasks has been demonstrated in patients with schizophrenia, but the responsible mechanism has not been identified. The present study was performed to determine whether neuronal pathology of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is linked to the activation of the working memory cortical network in patients with schizophrenia. METHOD: The brains of 13 patients with schizophrenia and 13 comparison subjects were studied with proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic (1H-MRS) imaging (to measure N-acetylaspartate as a marker of neuronal pathology) and with [15O]water positron emission tomography (PET) during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (to measure activation of the working memory cortical network). An independent cohort of patients (N=7) was also studied in a post hoc experiment with 1H-MRS imaging and with the same PET technique during performance of another working memory task (the “N-back” task). RESULTS: Measures of N-acetylaspartate in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex strongly correlated with activation of the distributed working memory network, including the dorsolateral prefrontal, temporal, and inferior parietal cortices, during both working memory tasks in the two independent groups of patients with schizophrenia. In contrast, N-acetylaspartate in other cortical regions and in comparison subjects did not show these relationships. CONCLUSIONS: These findings directly implicate a population of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex neurons as selectively accounting for the activity of the distributed working memory cortical network in schizophrenia and complement other evidence that dorsolateral prefrontal cortex connectivity is fundamental to the pathophysiology of the disorder.

Working memory is a cognitive construct describing the ability to hold information transiently in mind in the service of comprehension, thinking, and planning (1, 2). Complex cognitive processes such as working memory are thought to be subserved by the functional integration of interconnected regions forming large-scale cortical networks (15). Data on human and nonhuman primates show that a key cortical region for the execution of working memory tasks is the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (612), which has reciprocal anatomical connections with the parietal, temporal, and cingulate cortices, which also participate in the cortical network related to working memory (15).

Deficits in working memory have been reported to be a cardinal feature of the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (13, 14). Attempts to anatomically localize these deficits with functional neuroimaging studies in patients performing working memory tasks have often shown subnormal activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and, to a lesser extent, abnormalities of other regions in the working memory network (1522). There has been considerable debate on issues involving the mechanism of this pattern of hypofunction, including whether it reflects distributed neuronal pathology, is referable to focal cortical pathology, or is, perhaps, an artifact of the test procedure (1522). Postmortem studies of the brains of patients with schizophrenia have shown evidence of abnormalities in a number of cortical areas within the working memory network, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, cingulate, and temporal cortices (2326), although the most extensive data have implicated the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (2732). Since the overall function of a cortical network presumably relies on the competence of both local information processing within specific local circuits and axonal connections between local circuits and distant cortical areas, a deficit of a single region, for example the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, could conceivably have functional reverberations throughout the working memory network. The purpose of the present study was to address directly the question of whether the integrity of a population of neurons within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, as studied with proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic (1H-MRS) imaging, preferentially accounts for the distributed pattern of cortical function associated with working memory in schizophrenia, as studied with [15O]water positron emission tomography (PET) during performance of working memory tasks.

1H-MRS imaging detects signals in multiple brain regions arising from N-acetyl-containing moieties (mainly N-acetylaspartate, NAA), choline-containing compounds (CHO), and creatine plus phosphocreatine (CRE) (33). NAA is an intraneuronal amino acid, the highest concentrations of which occur in pyramidal neurons (34). Its biological role has yet to be clearly defined. However, it acts through the glutamatergic N-methyl-d-aspartic acid (NMDA) receptor to elevate intracellular calcium (35), and its concentrations are reduced by pharmacological inhibition of mitochondrial energy metabolism (36) and by a number of pathological processes affecting the integrity of neurons (37, 38). It is interesting that a recent study (39) has also shown increased NAA measures in rats during experimental status epilepticus, suggesting that NAA correlates with the functional status of neurons. Relative concentrations of NAA have been previously shown to be lower than normal in the prefrontal cortex of patients with schizophrenia (4044).

[15O]Water PET identifies changes in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) associated with neuronal activity. In the first of two experiments we measured rCBF during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, an abstract reasoning task involving the use of previously learned information to formulate a strategy for present and future actions. To the extent that recent memory is essential for achieving the correct action, the test has been considered to involve working memory and to be sensitive to prefrontal pathology (10, 15, 16, 45, 46). Several earlier studies (1519) have shown reduced rCBF in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and other related cortical areas in patients with schizophrenia during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and other tasks involving working memory. We also performed a second, post hoc experiment to address the issue of whether the correlations found in the patients during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test are task specific or related to generic working memory function. In this second experiment, a separate group of patients with schizophrenia performed a less complex working memory task, a version of the “N-back” task (47). This task has been previously shown to produce activation in a cortical network including the same regions involved in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and to reveal similar pathophysiological characteristics in patients with schizophrenia (22, 48). A “2-back” condition, in which subjects respond according to a number seen two stimuli before, requires continuous updating of the mental set and the use of working memory (22).

METHOD

Subjects

For the Wisconsin Card Sort study, there were 26 subjects: 13 patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia according to DSM-IV criteria (11 men; mean age=35.0 years, SD=8.6) and 13 comparison subjects (eight men; mean age=34.6 years, SD=8.0). Each subject underwent 1H-MRS imaging and [15O]water PET on two different days. For the 1H-MRS imaging scan, five of the patients had been without drugs for at least 3 weeks, while the others were receiving neuroleptics. Neuroleptics have been previously shown not to affect NAA findings (43, 49). On the day of the PET scan, five of the patients who had been receiving drugs when studied with 1H-MRS imaging were being treated with clozapine, while the others had been drug free for at least 3 weeks. The patients and comparison subjects had similar performances on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, as expressed by the average percentage of correct responses (patients, 71%; comparison subjects, 74%). The N-back study involved a different group of seven patients (five men; mean age=31.8 years, SD=8.7) with a diagnosis of schizophrenia according to DSM-IV. They had all been without neuroleptics for at least 2 weeks (range=15–30 days) before both the 1H-MRS imaging and [15O]water PET scans. The average performance of the patients on the 2-back version of the task was 50%, which is well below that reported for normal subjects (82%) (22).

After complete description of all studies to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained from each and every subject.

1H-MRS Imaging Procedure

The 1H-MRS imaging studies were performed as previously described (33, 41, 43) on a conventional GE Signa 1.5-T nuclear magnetic resonance imaging system. The 1H-MRS imaging pulse sequence acquires four spectroscopic slices (TR=2200 msec, TE=272 msec) involving 32 × 32 phase-encoding steps over a 240-mm field of view for each slice. Each volume element (voxel) has nominal dimensions of 7.5 mm × 7.5 mm × 15 mm (0.84 ml). Actual volume, based on full width at half maximum after filtering of k-space, is 1.4 ml. The 1H-MRS imaging data processing involved locating NAA, CHO, and CRE in spectra from each voxel and then displaying the four 32 × 32 arrays showing spatial variation of the magnitude of each of the signals in each of the slices. Regions of interest were drawn on coplanar magnetic resonance imaging scans as previously described (41). The metabolites were studied as ratios of the area under each peak: NAA/CRE, NAA/CHO, CHO/CRE.

[15O]Water PET Procedure

For the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test study, each subject underwent two PET scans during a single session: one scan while performing the card sorting test and the other one while performing a sensorimotor control task. The PET data were acquired as described by Berman et al. (10) on a Scanditronix PC2048-15B PET scanner that simultaneously produces 15 contiguous slices in 16 frames over 4 minutes. An intravenous bolus of approximately 42 mCi of [15O]water was administered before each scan. Arterial input functions were measured with automated arterial blood sampling, and absolute rCBF (milliliters of blood per minute per 100 g of tissue) was calculated for each voxel. For the N-back task study, 60-second PET data were acquired nonquantitatively on a GE Advance PET camera in three-dimensional mode after a bolus injection of 10 mCi of [15O]water per scan. Images for each subject were registered by using the Automated Image Registration (AIR) program and then normalized to the atlas of Talairach and Tournoux (50) and smoothed with a 15 × 15 × 5 filter by using the SPM95 package. The PET data were normalized by expressing each value as a ratio to the global mean. To determine activation during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, we subtracted the rCBF during the control condition from that during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. For the N-back study, activation data were derived by subtracting the average rCBF for seven scans acquired during the 2-back condition from the average for two scans acquired during rest. Pearson correlations between the 1H-MRS imaging regional values and rCBF PET data during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test were determined on a voxel-by-voxel basis. The statistical threshold used was p<0.01, corresponding to a Pearson r of 0.683. A cluster threshold of 10 contiguous voxels was applied as well. Because of the smaller group in the N-back study, we used a Spearman correlation analysis to avoid a potential outlier effect.

RESULTS

1H-MRS Imaging Measures and Brain Activation During Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

In the patients, NAA/CRE in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was strongly and positively correlated with activation in the prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s areas 9, 10, 44, 45, 46), parietal cortex (Brodmann’s area 39/40), and temporal association cortex (figure 1 and table 1). NAA/CRE in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex also exhibited negative correlations, mostly with subcortical structures, including the cerebellum and basal ganglia. On the other hand, NAA/CRE in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the comparison subjects showed a different pattern of relationships with rCBF activation, correlating only in a few scattered voxels in the left inferolateral prefrontal cortex and not with any other region activated by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

Because it is impossible to perform absolute measurements of metabolites with our 1H-MRS imaging technique and because we wished to test whether the relationships in patients with schizophrenia were specifically attributable to NAA signals, we also examined the correlations of two other ratio measures, NAA/CHO and CHO/CRE, in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to rCBF activation. While NAA/CHO correlated with activation in exactly the same Brodmann’s areas as seen with NAA/CRE (figure 1), CHO/CRE showed only sporadic correlations and none in the areas associated with working memory. Even though absolute concentrations of the metabolites were not studied, this consistent pattern of correlations indicates that they arise from abnormalities in NAA. These results suggest that the integrity of a population of neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and their connections predict the activation of the whole working memory cortical network in patients with schizophrenia.

We also tested whether the correlations between NAA measures and activation were specific to NAA measures in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. We examined correlations between NAA/CRE in the hippocampal area (also shown to have low NAA measures in schizophrenia [40, 42, 46, 47, 51, 52]), superior temporal gyrus, anterior cingulate, and occipital cortex (regions that are activated during working memory tasks) and the rCBF activation data. Few sporadic correlations emerged, and none of these involved areas of rCBF activation in the working memory network. Therefore, these additional data suggest that the correlation between NAA relative measures in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and activation in the cortical working memory network is regionally specific.

We also examined whether the correlations between NAA measures in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and rCBF activation (obtained by subtraction of blood flow during the control task from blood flow during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) were related to blood flow changes during working memory per se, rather than to blood flow during any volitional task. We performed separate correlations between NAA/CRE in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and rCBF during the two components of the rCBF activation signal, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and the control task. While the results of the correlations with rCBF during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test showed the same pattern of correlations as with the activation data (figure 2), there were few correlations during the control task and these involved brain areas not activated by the working memory task. Therefore, the correlations between NAA measures and activation are likely due to rCBF changes during working memory.

1H-MRS Imaging Measures and Brain Activation During N-Back Task

In this post hoc experiment, we addressed several issues, including the reproducibility of the data in another cohort of patients, whether the correlations are task specific or related to generic working memory function, and the possible impact of antipsychotic medication. NAA/CRE in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of these patients was also positively correlated with activation in the same brain regions (as identified by the local maxima of the activation) found during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test study, including the prefrontal cortex (rs=0.86, N=7, p<0.01) and the temporal-parietal cortex (rs=0.84, N=7, p<0.01). These data suggest that the correlations are reproducible in another group of patients with schizophrenia and that they are typical of tasks engaging the working memory circuitry, regardless of the specific test used. These additional results also indicate that the correlations are not due to active treatment with antipsychotic drugs, as this entire cohort was drug free, and not dependent on task performance per se, as the same relationships were found during this working memory task, on which the patients’ performance was abnormal, and during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, on which a different cohort of patients and comparison subjects did not differ.

DISCUSSION

Our results show that in schizophrenia the functional integrity of neurons within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (as represented by NAA measures) has predictable physiological reverberations throughout the entire working memory cortical network. NAA measures in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex predict activation of cortical regions involved in the execution of working memory tasks, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex itself, the parietal cortex, and the temporal association cortex. Moreover, these relationships are regionally specific, involving only the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as a predictor of network activation. The lack of such relationships in healthy subjects suggests that they emerge in patients because of disease-associated neuronal pathology in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In the present subjects, as in our previous study groups (41, 43), NAA measures in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of patients (averaged bilateral NAA/CRE: mean=2.6, SD=0.4) were significantly lower than those of normal comparison subjects (mean=2.9, SD=0.3) (two way ANOVA: F=4.5, df=1, 24, p<0.04; no effect of side or side-by-group interaction). To the extent that low NAA measures are a reflection of impaired functional integrity of neurons, this putative impairment constrains in a predictable way the functional capacity of the distributed working memory network, as if these dorsolateral prefrontal cortex neurons by virtue of their projections constitute a rate-limiting factor for the degree of network recruitment (48). These results are consistent with the anatomical and physiological centrality of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with respect to working memory function (612) and, perhaps, with respect to the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.

A traditional criticism of functional neuroimaging studies assessing differences in activation by working memory tasks between patients with schizophrenia and healthy subjects has been that patients usually perform worse on these tests, thus making the comparison unfair. Critics of this approach argue that it is impossible to say whether the abnormal neurobiology causes deficits in performance or vice versa. To address this criticism, we selected patients who could perform the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test well enough to be matched with comparison subjects. Indeed, previous studies (5355) have shown that there is a certain percentage of patients with schizophrenia who perform well on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Moreover, to further address the issue of performance and the related neurobiology, we also selected another group of patients who were not capable of performing a working memory task as well as the comparison subjects. The two cohorts of patients allowed us to assess possible correlations between NAA in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and activation of the working memory network in the presence or absence of impaired performance. It was interesting that the same pattern of relationships emerged during both working memory tasks, irrespective of whether the patients’ performance was normal. This suggests that the relationships reflect the capacity of neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to recruit the working memory network and that they are not an epiphenomenon of test score. The fact that task performance was normal in one group of patients during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test but not in another group during the 2-back test suggests that network capacity, although constrained by the neuronal integrity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, was adequate for the demands of the former condition (the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) but not for the latter (the N-back task).

Our findings are consistent with and amplify an emerging database implicating an abnormality of prefrontal cortical connectivity in schizophrenia. While we have demonstrated this possibility at the level of functional connectivity, others have reported in vivo and postmortem changes consistent with it. Functional neuroimaging studies have suggested that dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction and connectivity may be responsible for some of the neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia (10, 21, 22, 48, 56, 57). Postmortem studies of the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia have shown diminished neuropil (27), a low number of dendritic spines on layer III pyramidal neurons (30), small layer III neurons (32), abnormal levels of developmental and synaptic proteins such as synaptophysin and growth-associated protein 43 (28), and selective abnormalities in gene expression for glutamate NMDA receptor subunits (58). The evidence that neuronal connections of layer III neurons may be especially affected (32, 33) is particularly relevant to our results as these neurons project to other cortical areas, including those recruited during working memory (1). Consistent with our findings and with this body of literature suggesting abnormal connectivity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, we have recently reported that the same measure of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex neuronal integrity, i.e., NAA-related signals, predicts both steady-state (59) and amphetamine-induced (60) subcortical dopamine activity in patients with schizophrenia. Thus, a population of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex neurons identified by low NAA signals may be critical effectors of both the cortical pathophysiology implicated in the cognitive deficits of schizophrenia and the dopamine-related phenomena implicated in treatment with antipsychotic drugs. We have also shown (59) that monkeys with developmental prefrontal pathology induced by neonatal lesions of mesial temporal-limbic structures evince analogous relationships between prefrontal NAA measures and subcortical steady-state and stimulus-induced release of dopamine, further indicating that development of prefrontal neurons and of their connections is a potential mechanism for the determination of these relationships.

It is obvious, however, that since our results were obtained with statistical correlations, they do not intrinsically express a relationship of causality. Therefore, even though the evidence supporting our interpretations is robust, the preceding discussion has to be viewed as conjectural. In fact, another possible interpretation of the present findings is that the NAA measures in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex reflect a low abundance of axon terminals from other regions, e.g., the thalamus (as NAA is also found in neuronal processes). Indeed, in a previous study of rhesus monkeys (61) we showed that neonatal mesial-temporal limbic lesions can induce NAA deficits in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, perhaps reflecting a loss of inputs from the lesioned areas. However, by either scenario, i.e., low afferent input to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or low efferent activity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, it is the net effect on the connectivity of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex neurons and other cortical areas that the correlations implicate.

Some further caution in the interpretation of the results of the present study should be considered. The presence of a statistical correlation in one group but not in another could be caused by greater variance in the former than in the latter. However, this was not the case in our two groups of subjects in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test experiment, who did not have significant variance differences in either the activation or NAA data (analyzed with Hartley F-max, Cochran C, and Bartlett chi-square tests). Moreover, it is conceivable that the correlation between NAA in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and activation in the distributed working memory cortical network in the patients could simply be an epiphenomenon of the fact that activation in all the other regions of the network has a high degree of covariance with activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. However, if this was the case, the same correlations between NAA in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and activation in the cortical network would have been evident also in the comparison group, where we found a similar degree of high covariance between activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the other regions of the network (data not shown). Since this was not the case, we can assume that the correlations in the patients are not such an epiphenomenon. Another line of evidence against the correlations being an epiphenomenon of the high degree of covariance of the activation of all regions in the working memory cortical network is the specificity of correlations to NAA measures in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In fact, if the correlations were an epiphenomenon of intracortical rCBF relationships, it would be expected that NAA in other cortical regions of the network would show similar relationships with activation in the entire working memory cortical network. However, this also was not the case, since NAA measures in the superior temporal gyrus and anterior cingulate did not show correlations with activation of the working memory cortical network at all.

In conclusion, the data of the present study show potentially unique relationships between pathology of dorsolateral prefrontal cortical neurons and physiological activation of the whole working memory network in patients with schizophrenia. These data are consistent with current speculation focusing on the role played by development of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and its connections in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (31, 62).

Received Jan. 8, 1999; revision received June 16, 1999; accepted July 8, 1999. From the Clinical Brain Disorders Branch, Intramural Research Programs, NIMH; and the Laboratory of Diagnostic Radiology Research, Office of the Director, NIH. Address reprint requests to Dr. Weinberger, Clinical Brain Disorders Branch, Intramural Research Programs, NIMH, NIH, Rm. 4S235, MSC 1379, 10 Center Dr., Bethesda, MD 20892; (e-mail). The authors thank Jozef Duyn, Ph.D., and Chrit Moonen, Ph.D., for making the proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic (1H-MRS) imaging pulse sequence available and Alan Barnett, Ph.D., for help with processing of the1H-MRS imaging data.

TABLE 1
FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Voxel-by-Voxel Correlations Between Metabolite Ratios for the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex of 13 Schizophrenic Patients and Blood Flow Activation During the Wisconsin Card Sorting Testa

aThe slices are identified by position on the z axis (Talairach coordinate). The first row shows, in red, voxels with significant positive Pearson correlations (r>0.68, p<0.01) between the ratio of N-acetylaspartate to creatine plus phosphocreatine (NAA/CRE) and activation of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (test condition minus control condition). The second row shows areas of significant correlations between the ratio of N-acetylaspartate to choline-containing compounds (NAA/CHO) and rCBF activation during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. The third row shows significant correlations between NAA/CRE and rCBF during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test alone.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Correlations Between the Ratio of N-Acetylaspartate to Creatine Plus Phosphocreatine (NAA/CRE) and Regional Cerebral Blood Flow (rCBF) in Areas of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex of 13 Patients With Schizophrenia During the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and a Control Task

arCBF during the test minus rCBF during the control task.

References

1. Goldman-Rakic PS: Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by representational knowledge, in Handbook of Physiology, Section 1: The Nervous System, vol V. Edited by Plum F. Bethesda, Md, American Physiological Society, 1987, pp 373–417Google Scholar

2. Baddeley A: Human Memory: Theory and Practice. Needham Heights, Mass, Allyn & Bacon, 1990Google Scholar

3. Damasio AR, Tranel D, Damasio H: Face agnosia and the neural substrates of memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 1990; 13:89–109Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Mesulam MM: Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Ann Neurol 1990; 5:597–613CrossrefGoogle Scholar

5. Fuster JM: Network memory. Trends Neurosci 1997; 20:451–459Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Fuster JM, Alexander GE: Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science 1971; 173:652–654Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Kubota K, Niki H: Prefrontal cortical unit activity and delayed alternation performance in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 1971; 34:337–347Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Meyer E, Evans AC: Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal memory tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1993; 90:878–882Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Friedman HR, Goldman-Rakic PS: Coactivation of prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex in working memory tasks revealed by 2DG functional mapping in the rhesus monkey. J Neurosci 1994; 14:2775–2788Google Scholar

10. Berman KF, Ostrem JL, Randolph C, Gold J, Goldberg TE, Coppola RC, Carson RE, Herscovitch P, Weinberger DR: Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychologia 1995; 33:1027–1046Google Scholar

11. D’Esposito M, Detre JA, Alsop DC, Shin RK, Atlas C, Grossman M: The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature 1995; 378:279–281Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Cohen JD, Perlstein WM, Braver TS, Nystrom LE, Noll DC, Jonides J, Smith EE: Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature 1997; 386:604–608Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Goldman-Rakic PS: Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 6:348–357Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Goldberg TE, Gold JM: Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia, in Schizophrenia. Edited by Hirsch SR, Weinberger DR. Oxford, UK, Blackwell Science, 1995, pp 146–162Google Scholar

15. Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Zec RF: Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, I: regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:114–125Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Illowsky BP: Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, III: a new cohort and evidence for a monoaminergic mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:609–615Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Rubin P, Holm S, Friberg L, Videbech P, Andersen HS, Bendsen BB, Stromso N, Larsen JK, Hemmingsen R: Altered modulation of prefrontal and subcortical brain activity in newly diagnosed schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: a regional cerebral blood flow study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:987–995Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Andreasen NC, Rezai K, Alliger R, Swayze VW II, Flaum M, Kirchner P, Cohen G, O’Leary DS: Hypofrontality in neuroleptic-naive patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia: assessment with Xenon 133 single-photon emission computed tomography and the Tower of London. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:943–958Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Catafau AM, Parellada E, Lomena FJ, Bernardo M, Pavia J, Ros D, Setoain J, Gonzalez-Monclus E: Prefrontal and temporal blood flow in schizophrenia: resting and activation technetium-99m-HMPAO SPECT patterns in young neuroleptic-naive patients with acute disease. J Nucl Med 1994; 35:935–941MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Ganguli R, Carter C, Mintun M, Brar J, Becker J, Sarma R, Nichols T, Bennington E: PET brain mapping study of auditory verbal supraspan memory versus visual fixation in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41:33–42Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Andreasen NC, Paradiso S, O’Leary DS: “Cognitive dysmetria” as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry? Schizophr Bull 1998; 24:203–218Google Scholar

22. Callicott JH, Ramsey N, Tallent K, Bertolino A, Knable M, Coppola R, Goldberg TE, Mattay VS, van Gelderen P, Frank JA, Moonen CTW, Weinberger DR: Functional magnetic resonance imaging brain mapping in psychiatry: methodological issues illustrated in a study of working memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 1998; 18:186–196Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Benes FM, Davidson B, Bird ED: Quantitative cytoarchitectural studies of the cerebral cortex of schizophrenics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:31–35Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Falkai P, Bogerts B: Cell loss in the hippocampus of schizophrenics. Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci 1986; 236:154–161Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Jakob H, Beckman H: Prenatal developmental disturbances in the limbic allocortex in schizophrenics. J Neural Transm 1989; 65:303–326CrossrefGoogle Scholar

26. Benes FM, McSparren J, Bird ED, SanGiovanni JP, Vincent SL: Deficits in small interneurons in prefrontal and cingulate cortices of schizophrenic and schizoaffective patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:996–1001Google Scholar

27. Selemon LD, Rajkowska G, Goldman-Rakic PS: Abnormally high neuronal density in the schizophrenic cortex: a morphometric analysis of prefrontal area 9 and occipital area 17. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:805–818Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Perrone-Bizzozzero NI, Sower AC, Bird ED, Benowitz LI, Ivins KJ, Neve RL: Levels of the growth-associated protein GAP-43 are selectively increased in association cortices in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996; 93:14182–14187Google Scholar

29. Glantz LA, Lewis DA: Reduction of synaptophysin immunoreactivity in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:660–669Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Hirsch SR, Das I, Garey LJ, de Belleroche J: A pivotal role for glutamate in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia, and its cognitive dysfunction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1997; 56:797–802Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Lewis DA: Development of prefrontal cortex during adolescence: insights into vulnerable neural circuits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 1997; 16:385–398Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Rajkowska G, Selemon LD, Goldman-Rakic PS: Neuronal and glial somal size in the prefrontal cortex. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:215–224Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Duyn JH, Gillen J, Sobering G, van Zijl PC, Moonen CTW: Multisection proton MR spectroscopic imaging of the brain. Radiology 1993; 188:277–282Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Moffett JR, Namboodiri MA: Differential distribution of N-acetylaspartylglutamate and N-acetylaspartate immunoreactivities in rat forebrain. J Neurocytol 1995; 24:409–433Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. Rubin Y, LaPlaca MC, Smith DH, Thibault LE, Lenkinski RE: The effect of N-acetyl-aspartate on the intracellular calcium concentration in NTera2-neurons. Neurosci Lett 1995; 198:209–212Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Bates TE, Strangward M, Keelan J, Davey GP, Munro PMG, Clark JB: Inhibition of N-acetylaspartate production: implications for 1H MRS studies. Neuroreport 1997; 7:1397–1400Google Scholar

37. Vion-Dury J, Salvan AM, Confort-Gouny S, Dhiver C, Cozzone P: Reversal of brain metabolic alterations with zidovudine detected by proton localised magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Lancet 1995; 345:60–61Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Hugg JW, Kuzniecky RI, Gilliam FG, Morawetz RB, Faught RE, Hetherington HP: Normalization of contralateral metabolic function following temporal lobectomy demonstrated by 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging. Ann Neurol 1996; 40:236–239Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Najim IM, Wang Y, Shedid D, Luders HO, Ng TC, Comair YG: MRS metabolic markers of seizures and seizure-induced neuronal damage. Epilepsia 1998; 39:244–250Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. Buckley PF, Moore C, Long H, Larkin C, Thompson P, Mulvany F, Redmond O, Stack JP, Ennis JT, Waddington JL:1H Magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the left temporal and frontal lobes in schizophrenia: clinical neurodevelopmental and cognitive correlates. Biol Psychiatry 1994; 36:792–800Google Scholar

41. Bertolino A, Nawroz S, Mattay VS, Barnett AS, Duyn JH, Moonen CTW, Frank JA, Tedeschi G, Weinberger DR: Regionally specific pattern of neurochemical pathology in schizophrenia as assessed by multislice proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:1554–1563Google Scholar

42. Deicken RF, Zhou L, Corwin F, Vinogradov S, Weiner MW: Decreased left frontal lobe N-acetylaspartate in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:688–690LinkGoogle Scholar

43. Bertolino A, Callicott JH, Elman I, Mattay VS, Tedeschi G, Frank JA, Breier A, Weinberger DR: Regionally specific neuronal pathology in untreated patients with schizophrenia: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 18:1–9Google Scholar

44. Cecil KM, Lenkinski RE, Gur RE, Gur RC: Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in the frontal and temporal lobes of neuroleptic naive patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 1998; 20:131–140CrossrefGoogle Scholar

45. Milner B: Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Arch Neurol 1963; 9:100–110CrossrefGoogle Scholar

46. Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Kameyama M, Nakashara K, Sekihara K, Miyashita Y: Transient activation of inferior prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting. Nat Neurosci 1998; 1:80–84Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47. Gevins A, Smith ME, McEvoy L, Yu D: High resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: effects of task difficulty, type of processing and practice. Cereb Cortex 1997; 7:374–385Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48. Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, Santha AKS, Finn K, Coppola RC, Goldberg TE, Frank JA, Weinberger DR: Capacity constraints in working memory: dissociating cortical activation from task performance. Cereb Cortex 1999; 9:20–26Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49. Renshaw PF, Yurgelun-Todd DA, Tohen M, Gruber S, Cohen BM: Temporal lobe proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of patients with first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:444–446LinkGoogle Scholar

50. Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. New York, Thieme Medical, 1988Google Scholar

51. Maier M, Ron MA, Barker GJ, Tofts PS: Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy: an in vivo method of estimating hippocampal neuronal depletion in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 1995; 25:1201–1209Google Scholar

52. Nasrallah HA, Skinner TE, Schmalbrock P, Robitaille PM: Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia: a pilot study. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 165:481–485Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53. Goldberg TE, Kelsoe JR, Weinberger DR, Pliskin NH, Kirwin PD, Berman KF: Performance of schizophrenic patients on putative neuropsychological tests of frontal lobe function. Int J Neurosci 1988; 42:51–58Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

54. Braff DL, Heaton R, Kuck J, Cullum M, Moranville J, Grant I, Zisook S: The generalized pattern of neuropsychological deficits in outpatients with chronic schizophrenia with heterogeneous Wisconsin Card Sorting Test results. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:891–898Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55. Goldstein G, Beers SR, Shernansky WY: Neuropsychological differences between schizophrenic patients with heterogeneous Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance. Schizophr Res 1996; 21:13–18Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56. Frith CD, Friston KJ, Herold S, Silbersweig D, Fletcher P, Cahill C, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RS, Liddle PF: Regional brain activity in chronic schizophrenic patients during the performance of a verbal fluency task. Br J Psychiatry 1995; 167:343–349Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57. Wiser AK, Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Watkins GL, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD: Dysfunctional cortico-cerebellar circuits cause “cognitive dysmetria” in schizophrenia. Neuroreport 1998; 9:1895–1899Google Scholar

58. Akbarian S, Sucher NJ, Bradley D, Tafazzoli A, Trinh D, Hetrick WP, Potkin SG, Sandman CA, Bunney WE Jr, Jones EG: Selective alterations in gene expression for NMDA receptor subunits in prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics. J Neurosci 1996; 16:19–30Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

59. Bertolino A, Knable M, Saunders R, Callicott JH, Kolachana B, Mattay VS, Frank JA, Egan M, Weinberger DR: Prefrontal cortical regulation of subcortical dopamine in patients with schizophrenia: a 1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging and IBZM-SPECT study. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:660–667Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

60. Bertolino A, Breier A, Callicott JH, Adler C, Mattay VS, Shapiro M, Frank JA, Pickar D, Weinberger DR: The relationship between dorsolateral prefrontal neuronal N-acetyl aspartate and evoked release of striatal dopamine in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology (in press)Google Scholar

61. Bertolino A, Saunders RC, Mattay VS, Bachevalier J, Frank JA, Weinberger DR: Altered development of prefrontal neurons in rhesus monkeys with neonatal mesial temporo-limbic lesions: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging study. Cereb Cortex 1997; 7:740–748Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

62. Weinberger DR, Lipska BK: Cortical maldevelopment, anti-psychotic drugs, and schizophrenia: a search for common ground. Schizophr Res 1995; 16:87–110Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar