Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Trust and health: testing the reverse causality hypothesis
  1. Giuseppe Nicola Giordano1,2,
  2. Martin Lindström1,2
  1. 1Department of Clinical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden
  2. 2Centre for Economic Demography (CED), Lund University, Malmö, Sweden
  1. Correspondence to Dr Giuseppe Nicola Giordano, Department of Clinical Sciences, Malmö University Hospital, Clinical Research Centre (CRC), Entrance 72, House 60, Floor 12, Lund University, Malmö 204 02, Sweden; Giuseppe_nicola.giordano{at}med.lu.se

Abstract

Background Social capital research has consistently shown positive associations between generalised trust and health outcomes over 2 decades. Longitudinal studies attempting to test causal relationships further support the theory that trust is an independent predictor of health. However, as the reverse causality hypothesis has yet to be empirically tested, a knowledge gap remains. The aim of this study, therefore, was to investigate if health status predicts trust.

Methods Data employed in this study came from 4 waves of the British Household Panel Survey between years 2000 and 2007 (N=8114). The sample was stratified by baseline trust to investigate temporal relationships between prior self-rated health (SRH) and changes in trust. We used logistic regression models with random effects, as trust was expected to be more similar within the same individuals over time.

Results From the ‘Can trust at baseline’ cohort, poor SRH at time (t−1) predicted low trust at time (t) (OR=1.38). Likewise, good health predicted high trust within the ‘Cannot’ trust cohort (OR=1.30). These patterns of positive association remained after robustness checks, which adjusted for misclassification of outcome (trust) status and the existence of other temporal pathways.

Conclusions This study offers empirical evidence to support the circular nature of trust/health relationship. The stability of association between prior health status and changes in trust over time differed between cohorts, hinting at the existence of complex pathways rather than a simple positive feedback loop.

  • SOCIAL EPIDEMIOLOGY
  • SOCIAL CAPITAL
  • SELF-RATED HEALTH
  • TIME-SERIES
  • LONGITUDINAL STUDIES

This is an Open Access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Introduction

One hundred years after Durkheim1 suggested links between individual health and social cohesion, social capital (considered a subset of social cohesion2) entered the field of public health.3 ,4 Numerous studies have since reported positive associations between this phenomenon and health outcomes.5 ,6

Defined as ‘social networks and norms of reciprocity’,7 social capital has been conceptualised at the collective level and individual level,2 ,8–11 being measured by proxies such as generalised trust and social participation.12 Interestingly, multilevel studies show that the greatest effects of social capital on health are at the individual level,5 ,6 ,13–17 that is, only 0–4% of total variation in individual health may be attributable to collective social capital.18–21 Of the individual-level social capital proxies, generalised trust has provided the most consistent association with health outcomes22 and is, therefore, the outcome of interest in this temporality study of individual-level social capital and health. Hypotheses as to how individual-level social capital may influence health include psychological/psychosocial mechanisms and norms regarding health-related behaviours (eg, smoking).3

Numerous cross-sectional studies have reported positive associations between social capital (trust) and health outcomes.5 Possible hypotheses behind reported associations include:

  1. Trust independently predicts health (by the mechanisms proposed previously3);

  2. Associations are non-causal, that is, past associations are confounded by unmeasured factors;23 ,24

  3. Health status affects trust (reverse causality), for example, uncertainty/vulnerability associated with poor health lowers trust;25

  4. A reciprocal/circular relationship exists.26

However, scarcity of suitable (longitudinal) social capital data means that such hypotheses remain largely empirically untested.6

Regarding hypothesis (I), a PUBMED search identified six longitudinal studies incorporating three or more time-points required to correctly test temporal (causal) relationships,27 while investigating trust and health.26 ,28–32 All six reported that generalised trust positively influenced health.

Regarding the ‘non-causal’ hypothesis (II), Fujiwara and Kawachi23 adjusted for shared genetic/environmental factors, utilising twin-pair data to confirm associations between generalised trust, participation and health. Likewise, a longitudinal, multilevel study by Giordano et al24 concluded that associations between generalised trust and health remained after adjusting for shared environmental factors (the household).

Regarding (III), no studies were identified explicitly investigating reverse causality. This is in stark contrast to the field of crime and social capital research, where mutual pathways have been extensively researched.33

Regarding (IV), one paper demonstrated the potential for a ‘mutually reinforcing’ feedback loop between health and trust.26 However, the study also reported that individuals with poor health predicted increased trust levels (ie, negative association), with no further discussion of this apparent paradox.26

An important knowledge gap, therefore, remains. Current evidence suggests that trust independently influences health and, as such, decision-makers have applied this knowledge at policy level to positively improve population health.34 However, without rigorous testing of the reverse causality hypothesis, the direction of the trust/health relationship remains an assumption. The aim of this longitudinal individual-level study, therefore, is to investigate how later levels of trust are affected by prior health status.

Methods

Data collection

The British Household Panel Survey (BHPS) is a longitudinal survey of randomly selected private households, conducted by the UK's Economic and Social Research Centre. Since 1991, individuals within selected households have been annually interviewed with a view to identifying social and economic changes within the British population. Full details of the selection process, weighting and participation rates, can be found online.35

The raw data used for this panel study came from the BHPS individual-level responses in years 2000, 2003, 2005 and 2007. The same individuals (N=8114) were followed across this 7-year time frame; participation rate for year 2000 (as compared with year 1999) was 93.6%, and, compared with the original 1991 cohort, was 62.0%.

The research centre fully adopted the Ethical Guidelines of the Social Research Association; informed consent was obtained from all participants and strict confidentiality protocols were adhered to throughout data collection and processing procedures.

Dependent variable

Generalised trust was assessed by asking people: ‘Would you say that most people can be trusted, or that you can't be too careful?’. Possible answers were ‘Most people can be trusted’, ‘You can't be too careful’ and ‘It depends’. This variable was dichotomised (as standard), with only those respondents stating that most people could be trusted being labelled ‘Can trust’; all negative responses (including ‘it depends’) were labelled ‘Can't trust’.36

Explanatory variables

Self-rated health

Self-rated health (SRH) is considered a valid predictor of morbidity and future mortality.37 ,38 The same individuals were asked: ‘Compared to people your own age, would you say that your health has on the whole been: excellent, good, fair, poor or very poor?’. As is standard, this five-point scale was recoded into the dichotomous variable ‘good’ (excellent, good) and ‘poor’ (fair, poor, very poor) health.39

Social participation/social support

Social isolation and lack of social support have been associated with lower trust;40 therefore, marital status, cohabiting and social participation were considered as potential confounders. Social participation was measured by asking respondents questions about being active members of listed voluntary community groups or any sports, hobby or leisure group activity found locally (see online supplementary appendix). Only those who answered positively to any of these were judged to participate, with all others being labelled ‘No participation’.

Respondents were asked if they were ‘married, separated, divorced, widowed or never married’. These five options were recoded into the dichotomous variable ‘married’ and ‘not married’ (separated, divorced, widowed or never married41). A further variable ‘Lives alone’ (‘yes’ or ‘no’) was used to capture individuals who cohabited.

Socioeconomic status variables

As low trust has been associated with individual-level disadvantage,42 socioeconomic resources were included in these analyses. Social class was determined by respondents’ most recent occupation, derived from the Registrar General's Social Classification of occupations. The usual six categories (see online supplementary appendix) were dichotomised into ‘higher’ (1–3a) and ‘lower’ (3b–6) social class.

Highest achieved education level was categorised as ‘Undergraduate or higher’, ‘Year 13’ and ‘Year 11’ or ‘No formal qualifications’.

Household income was weighted according to size by summing the income of all household members and dividing this sum by the square root of the household size.43 This item was maintained as a continuous variable (per £1000 increase) and was an expression of total income, net of taxation.

Confounders

Age, gender, smoking status and time were considered confounders in this study, age being stratified into quintiles (tables 14). Smoking status was categorised as ‘smoker’ and ‘non-smoker’ according to respondents’ answer to the question ‘Do you smoke cigarettes?’.

Table 1

Baseline (year 2000) frequencies of all considered variables expressed as integers and percentages (%) of NT (8114) stratified by trust

Table 2

Transitions of trust status between 2000 and 2007 expressed as integers and percentages (%) of NT (8114), stratified by trust at baseline

Table 3

ORs with 95% CIs of trust levels at time (t) according to logistic regression analysis of all lagged (t−1) explanatory variables between years 2000 and 2007, results stratified by baseline trust status (NT=8114)

Table 4

Double coding of trust (2000–2003): ORs with 95% CIs of changes in trust status over time (2003–2007) according to multivariate logistic regression analysis of all lagged (t−1) explanatory variables (NT=6036)

All explanatory variables (except gender) were lagged at time (t−1) in reference to trust at time (t).

Statistical analyses

All data were stratified by baseline (year 2000) trust to create two distinct cohorts: ‘Can trust’ and ‘Cannot trust’ at baseline. After initial disaggregation, the two ‘trust’ cohorts were modelled as separate entities. Models 1a-3a dealt solely with individuals from the ‘Can trust at baseline’ cohort (0), who now no longer trusted (1) (N=3125); models 1b-3b dealt with individuals from the ‘Cannot trust at baseline’ cohort (0), who now could trust (1) (N=4989); the outcome of interest in both sets of models was change from baseline trust status over time. When ‘trust 2003’ was the outcome, only explanatory variables from year 2000 were considered; when ‘trust 2005’ was the outcome, explanatory variables from 2003 were considered; and when ‘trust 2007’ was the outcome, explanatory variables from 2005 were considered.

To assess robustness, we performed two sensitivity tests. The first specified that individuals had to have two registrations of the same trust level in 2000 and 2003 before being included in their respective trust cohort, to reduce any misclassification bias of reported trust.

The second tested for other temporal pathways by running all explanatory variables from time (t) alongside their respective lagged (t−1) counterparts, the outcome being trust at time (t). If association between SRH at time (t−1) and trust at (t) held if the model also contained SRH at time (t), this would confirm the robustness of the main results.

For all analyses, we used logistic regression models with random effects, as trust was expected to be more similar within the same individual over time than between different individuals. The model allowed a random intercept for each individual and we obtained SEs that were adjusted for the temporal correlation of trust within the same individual across the timeframe of our study. The equations for logistic regression models with random effects are as follows:Embedded Image

Where i=time, j=individual, µ0j=the random intercepts (assumed to be independently normally distributed with a common variance), Xi−1j is a vector of lagged explanatory variables, β0 is the fixed overall intercept and β, the corresponding vector of coefficients.

All considered explanatory variables were utilised for all analyses, which were conducted using GLLAMM V.2.3.20,44 within the statistical software package STATA V.11.2.45

Results

Table 1 shows frequencies and total percentages of all considered explanatory variables, stratified by baseline trust status (year 2000).

Table 2 further describes transitions in individual trust status over time.

Model 1a: ‘Can trust’ cohort

The outcome of interest in model 1a was change from ‘Can trust at Baseline’ (0) to ‘Now cannot trust’ (1), between 2000 and 2007. As shown in table 3, poor SRH at time (t−1) was associated with lack of trust at time (t) (OR=1.38). Of the socioeconomic status (SES) variables, those with low social class or those who had completed Year 13 of high school at time (t−1) predicted a lack of trust at time (t), (OR=1.95 and 1.54, respectively).

Of the social support variables, not being married at time (t−1) predicted low trust at time (t) (OR=1.32), as did being female and smoking at (t−1) (OR=1.23 and 1.45, respectively).

Model 1b: ‘Cannot trust’ cohort

The outcome of interest in model 1b was change from ‘Cannot trust at Baseline’ (0) to ‘Now can trust’ (1), between 2000 and 2007. As shown in table 3, good SRH and active participation at time (t−1) predicted high levels of trust at time (t) (OR=1.30 and 1.21, respectively). Of the SES variables, high social class at (t−1) predicted high trust at (t) (OR=1.51), as did non-smoking status and being male at (t−1) (OR=1.39 and 1.41, respectively).

Sensitivity tests

Double coding

Table 4 shows results after specifying that individuals had to have two consecutive registrations of the same trust level (‘Can trust’ or ‘Cannot trust’) in years 2000 and 2003 before being included in their respective trust cohort (NT=6036). In model 2a (Can trust cohort), though similar patterns were seen, associations between poor SRH, not being married, being female and smoking at time (t−1), and lack of trust at time (t), were no longer significant at p<0.05. Being of low social class and completing Year 13 education at (t−1) remained associated with a lack of trust at time (t).

In model 2b (Cannot trust cohort), poor SRH at (t−1) and being female predicted high trust at time (t) (OR=1.24 and 1.41, respectively). Smoking status, active participation, high social class and being retired at (t−1) were no longer associated with high trust in the sensitivity test for this cohort.

Temporal pathways testing

Table 5 shows the results after running all explanatory variables from time (t), alongside their respective lagged (t−1) counterparts, the outcome being trust at time (t). Note that although all explanatory variables at time (t) and (t−1) were included in each model, only the results for SRH are shown. From model 3a, SRH at times (t−1) and (t) had positive association with trust, with the effect of SRH at time-point (t) being the stronger (OR=1.26 and 1.66, respectively). Conversely, in model 3b, the strength of association between good health and high trust was identical for time (t−1) and at (t) (OR=1.24).

Table 5

Temporal pathway testing: ORs with 95% CIs of changes in trust status over time (2000–2007) according to multiple variable logistic regression analysis of all lagged (t−1) and unlagged (t) explanatory variables (NT=8114)

Discussion

This longitudinal study explicitly tested the reverse causality hypothesis. We investigated temporal relationships between lagged values in SRH at time-point (t−1) and generalised trust at (t). Results, in conjunction with past temporality research,28 provided a more detailed overview of the health/trust relationship, with empirical evidence now suggesting not a simple ‘cause–effect’ relationship but one which appeared circular in nature.

As a robustness check, our first sensitivity test specified that individuals had to have consecutive registrations of the same trust level in years 2000 and 2003 before cohort definition (table 4). This was considered prudent, as approximately 45% of individuals from our sample changed trust status over the 7-year timeframe (table 2), which could have introduced misclassification bias. Despite some loss of significance, results revealed similar patterns of association between SRH and trust seen in the main analyses (table 3), which, in part, added strength to the notion of a circular trust/health relationship. Lack of statistical significance in table 4 may be the result of the reduced sample size (≈25%) or that after double-coding only two points in time (2005 and 2007) remained to measure changes in trust.

Different temporal pathways may coexist or confound each other, leading to further bias. Results of our second sensitivity test are shown in table 5. From model 3a, SRH at times (t−1) and (t) both had positive associations with trust at (t). However, the association between poor health and lack of trust was stronger when the two events were reported at time (t), offering some empirical support for the ‘mutually reinforcing’ feedback loop hypothesis.26 In model 3b, the positive association between health and trust was mirrored; however, the strength of association was identical at time (t−1) and (t), that is, unlike model 3a, the influence of good health on high trust remained stable over time.

From the above points, it appears that the health/trust relationship is more complex than the direct cause–effect response previously postulated.26 ,28–32 Empirical evidence from this and other temporality research28 suggests that the trust/health relationship is circular in nature. However, as patterns of association between SRH and trust seem cohort dependent, our results do not fully support the existence of a mutually reinforcing feedback loop.26 This is clearly seen in table 5 (model 3a) where the (larger) impact of poor health on low trust at (t) could be due to feelings of uncertainty or vulnerability.25 Those individuals reporting poor health for longer periods (ie, at (t) and (t−1) due to, say, chronic illness) would retain the propensity not to trust for longer and may be behind weaker associations between poor health at (t−1) and low trust at (t).

Alternatively, patterns of association in table 5 could reflect levels of healthcare utilisation (UK residents have universal access to healthcare). It has been theorised that healthcare institutions are ‘purveyors of wider societal norms’, such as generalised trust.46 Therefore, it is plausible that in welfare states such as the UK, the positive association between prior health and later trust could be mediated, in part, by healthcare utilisation.47 As poor health and low trust have both been associated with low healthcare use,47 such behaviour may deny individuals the appropriate medical treatment and also limit exposure to institutions that help perpetuate the societal norm of trust.46

Strengths and limitations

A major strength is the longitudinal design of this study, tracking the same individuals (N=8114) at four time-points over 7 years. The study captures associations between lagged (t−1) explanatory variables and changes from baseline trust, allowing us to build on past temporal research in this field.28 To the best of our knowledge, this is the first time the reverse causality hypothesis of the SRH/trust relationship has been explicitly investigated. The large sample size meant that disaggregation by baseline trust status still allowed for two large independent cohorts, which enabled us to investigate changes from baseline trust. The fact that data were obtained via interview rather than relying on postal questionnaires contributed to the very high participation rate of around 90%, year on year.35

A major limitation of this study is that the BHPS sample was originally selected to reflect the UK population as a whole and avoided oversampling of smaller-sized communities. Furthermore, our longitudinal data were unsuitable to perform any meaningful contextual analysis. The outcome ‘generalised trust’ was dichotomised (see methods). Although handled in the standard fashion,36 there is always some loss of information on dichotomisation. Further, as trust and other variables used in this study were self-reported, they were also subject to misreporting bias. The ‘double-coded’ sensitivity test was employed to reduce this risk. Although temporal relationships are considered ‘essential’ in establishing causality,48 it is a gross oversimplification to assume that all pathways have been investigated in this study. By year 2000, only 62.0% of the original cohort members were able to answer the questions posed,35 introducing selection bias into this study (this is assumed to be small, however, as strength and direction of associations are both as expected).

Conclusion

The circular relationship between trust and health, as shown in this study, suggests that pathways other than direct positive (causal) effects are present.28 Nor did we find evidence to fully support the existence of a positive (mutually reinforcing) feedback loop between health and trust.26 We noted that strength and stability of the association between SRH and trust was cohort dependent. Our results, therefore, offered some empirical support to other theories postulated to describe the complex mechanisms behind the trust/health relationship.25 ,46 Further longitudinal research is required to capture event-timings more precisely, in order to disentangle the ways that trust and health appear to affect each other over time.

What is already known on this subject?

  • Past social capital research suggests that generalised trust may be an independent predictor of health. Despite emerging longitudinal data within this field adding weight to this argument, the reverse causality hypothesis (ie that health predicts trust) has yet to be empirically investigated.

What this study adds?

  • This longitudinal individual-level study attempted to fill a knowledge gap by investigating temporal relationships between health and trust. Our results showed that prior health status consistently predicted changes from baseline trust levels, which suggests that pathways behind positive associations are more than the simple ‘cause’ and ‘effect’ mechanisms previously hypothesised.

Acknowledgments

The data used in this study were made available through the UK Data Archive. The data were originally collected by the ESRC Research Centre on Micro-social Change at the University of Essex (now incorporated within the Institute for Social and Economic Research). Neither the original collectors of the data nor the Archive bear any responsibility for the analyses or interpretations presented here.

References

Supplementary materials

  • Supplementary Data

    This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.

Footnotes

  • Contributors GNG and ML conceived the study design. GNG built the datasets, performed all analysis and drafted the first version of the manuscript. ML helped finalise the draft. GNG and ML read and approved the submitted version of the manuscript.

  • Funding This study was supported by Swedish Research Council Linnaeus Centre for Economic Demography (VR 79), the Swedish Research Council (K2014–69X-22427-01-4), Swedish ALF Government Grant (Dnr M M 2014/354), and the Research Funds of the University Hospital in southern Sweden.

  • Competing interests None declared.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.