Intended for healthcare professionals

Research

Personality, lifestyle, and risk of cardiovascular disease and cancer: follow-up of population based cohort

BMJ 2006; 332 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.38833.479560.80 (Published 08 June 2006) Cite this as: BMJ 2006;332:1359
  1. Til Stürmer, visiting associate professor of medicine (til.sturmer{at}post.harvard.edu)1,
  2. Petra Hasselbach, research fellow2,
  3. Manfred Amelang, professor of psychology2
  1. 1 Divisions of Pharmacoepidemiology and Pharmacoeconomics, and Preventive Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02120, USA,
  2. 2 Department of Psychology, University of Heidelberg, Germany
  1. Correspondence to: T Stürmer
  • Accepted 13 April 2006

Abstract

Objective To study the relation between measures of personality and risk of cardiovascular disease and cancer in a large cohort.

Design Follow-up of population based cohort.

Setting Heidelberg, Germany.

Participants 5114 women and men aged 40-65 in 1992-5.

Main outcome measures Psychological traits assessed by several standardised personality questionnaires in 1992-5, related to cause of death (to 2002-3) or reported incidence of cardiovascular diseases and cancer (validated by treating doctors). Relative risks (and 95% confidence intervals) for combined morbidity and mortality according to five important personality traits were estimated using multivariable Cox proportional hazards models.

Results During median follow-up of 8.5 years, 257 participants died and 72 were diagnosed with a heart attack, 62 with stroke, and 240 with cancer (morbidity and mortality combined). A high internal locus of control over disease was associated with a decreased risk of myocardial infarction (adjusted relative risk for an increase of 1 SD = 0.75; 95% confidence interval 0.58 to 0.96). An increase of 1 SD in time urgency was associated with a decreased risk of cancer (adjusted relative risk 0.83; 0.73 to 0.95). Other major personality traits—anger control, psychoticism, and symptoms of depression—were not consistently associated with myocardial infarction, stroke, or cancer.

Conclusion Internal locus of control over disease and time urgency seem to be associated with reduced risk for common chronic diseases, probably by affecting unmeasured health related behaviour. The other personality traits assessed had no major impact on cardiovascular disease and cancer.

Introduction

Medicine and psychology have long been separate disciplines. Despite the lack of interdisciplinary studies and cooperation, doctors are aware that psychological traits influence the incidence and the course of chronic diseases.

A recent large multinational case-control study linked permanent stress at work or home during the past year to increased incidence of myocardial infarction.1 The size of the effect of psychosocial factors was similar to that seen for abdominal obesity, diabetes, and hypertension.2 The proposed pathophysiological links between psychological stress and cardiovascular disease include clustering of “traditional” risk factors, endothelial dysfunction, myocardial ischaemia, plaque rupture, thrombosis, and malignant arrhythmias.3 4

Evidence for a link between psychological factors and cancer is weak. A recent review concluded that “there is not any psychological factor for which an influence on cancer development has been convincingly demonstrated in a series of studies.”5 A metaanalysis of stressful life events and risk of breast cancer did not support an overall association between such events and breast cancer, except for a modestly increased risk with the death of a spouse.6 In a large population based cohort study from Denmark, the incidence of cancer was 18% higher in mothers 7-18 years after the death of a child.7

We hypothesised that personality differences influence the incidence of and mortality from cardiovascular disease and cancer, independent of “traditional” risk factors. We empirically derived five personality scales that measure different independent personality traits8 and present results on all five scales without selection according to the strength of association with the disease outcomes.

Methods

Recruitment and follow-up of the cohort have been described.8 Briefly, we identified a representative sample of women and men aged 40-65 from the population registry of Heidelberg, Germany. Between 1992 and 1995, 5114 of these people completed an extensive baseline questionnaire on psychological traits, lifestyle factors, and comorbidity. All participants provided written informed consent to assess their health status 10 years later, including assessment of causes of death.

In 2002, we mailed a follow-up questionnaire to all participants and asked for information about chronic diseases that had been diagnosed since baseline. All participants who did not reply and could not be reached by telephone were followed up through population registries.

Personality variables

The personality scales used at baseline were selected for several reasons.8 Some scales were markers of broad aspects of personality (extraversion, neuroticism, psychoticism). Other scales had been used in research on health outcomes (scales to measure depression, time urgency, hostility, anger out, low sense of coherence, and irritability), some were hypothesised to be relevant (scales to measure optimism, anger in, anger control, social support, exaggerated social control, internal and external locus of control over disease, jealousy), and others were important with respect to response style (scales to measure social desirability).

Since many of these personality scales overlap and are highly correlated, we aggregated the information contained in these scales (by using factor analysis) to obtain five broad independent dimensions of personality.8 These dimensions are unique to the population from which they were derived. To make it possible to compare our results with reports in the literature, we used the personality scales that were most strongly correlated with these five broad dimensions, rather than the dimensions themselves. Both the derivation of the dimensions and the selection of scales relied solely on cross sectional baseline data (that is, were independent of the disease outcomes), and results are presented for all scales assessed.

Morbidity and mortality

We validated all diagnoses of myocardial infarction, stroke, and cancer that had occurred since baseline by contacting the treating doctors (with the written informed consent of participants). Mortality follow-up was 99.6% (22 participants moved to an unknown address or left the country); we assessed the cause of death from death certificates for 98.8% of participants who had died. Two doctors coded the cause of death according to ICD-10 (international classification of diseases, 10th revision), and inconsistencies were resolved by discussion.

Covariates

We assessed the risk factors for chronic diseases that might be associated with personality variables at baseline. Besides age and sex, these were body mass index; smoking status; alcohol consumption (amount of beer, wine, and spirits drunk each week); exercise (hours each week); comorbidity (history of myocardial infarction, stroke, cancer, hypertension, hyperlipidaemia, and diabetes); family history of myocardial infarction, stroke, and cancer; and education (years of schooling).

Statistical analyses

For each participant, we calculated the time to death, diagnosis of disease, or end of follow-up, whichever came first. We used Cox proportional hazards models to assess the association between each personality scale (in thirds or as a continuous variable) and the incidence of myocardial infarction, stroke, or cancer. We excluded participants with the disease at baseline or in whom the incidence of disease could not be assessed because of missing information (247 for myocardial infarction, 274 for stroke, and 256 for cancer).

Results

table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of the 4267 participants who replied to the follow-up questionnaire or died during follow-up (83.4% of the original cohort). Mean age was 53.4 years; 51.5% were women. Risk factors for chronic diseases, such as overweight or obesity, smoking, no regular physical exercise, hypertension, hyperlipidaemia, and a family history of myocardial infarction, stroke, or cancer were common.

Table 1

Baseline characteristics of 4267 participants from the Heidelberg cohort. Values are number (percentage) unless otherwise indicated

View this table:

table 2 shows the distribution of the five personality scales according to baseline characteristics (actual patient numbers are shown in table A on bmj.com). All scales were divided into low, medium, or high according to the observed distribution (with one third of participants in each category, except for psychoticism, which was split into 22%, 53%, and 26%). Older age was strongly associated with more symptoms of depression and higher anger control. Women were more likely than men to have more symptoms of depression, and men were more likely to have higher anger control, higher time urgency, and higher internal locus of control over disease (the patient's belief that the onset and process of an illness is the result of their behaviour). Higher frequency of exercise was strongly associated with fewer symptoms of depression and higher locus of control. Patients with any comorbidity—especially those with a history of myocardial infarction, stroke, diabetes, or cancer—had more symptoms of depression.

Table 2

Distribution of observed personality scales in 4267 participants from the Heidelberg cohort according to baseline characteristics. Values are percentages (table A on bmj.com gives the actual numbers)*

View this table:

During a median follow-up of 8.5 years, 257 participants died, 72 participants had a heart attack, 62 participants had a stroke, and 240 participants were diagnosed with cancer (morbidity and mortality combined).

Tables 3, 4 and 5 show the five personality scales (representing the five broad personality dimensions) at baseline as predictors of incident myocardial infarction, stroke, and cancer during follow-up. For each personality scale, we performed two separate analyses; one compared the low and the high category to the medium category and one used the z transformed scores from the original personality scale as a continuous variable, so that the relative risk corresponds to an increase of 1 SD (standard deviation) in the personality scale. The first set of analyses assesses non-monotonic trends and the second assumes a monotonic log-linear trend.

Table 3

Personality variables and incident myocardial infarction (morbidity and mortality) in the Heidelberg cohort (4267 participants)*

View this table:
Table 4

Personality variables and incident stroke (morbidity and mortality) in the Heidelberg cohort (4267 participants)*

View this table:
Table 5

Personality variables and incident cancer (morbidity and mortality) in the Heidelberg cohort (4267 participants)*

View this table:

Anger control, time urgency, and symptoms of depression were not associated with the incidence of myocardial infarction (table 3). Participants with a low internal locus of control over disease had an adjusted relative risk of 1.84 (95% confidence interval 1.01 to 3.32) compared with those with a medium internal locus of control, and participants with a high internal locus of control over disease had a relative risk of 0.77 (0.41 to 1.46). An increase of 1 SD in internal locus of control over disease was associated with a relative risk of 0.75 (0.58 to 0.96). The relative risks of myocardial infarction were 1.59 (0.88 to 2.87) and 1.28 (0.71 to 2.30) for participants with low and high psychoticism compared with those with a medium degree of psychoticism. Although this indicates a U-shaped association, an increase of 1 SD was associated with a relative risk of 1.21 (1.01 to 1.45).

None of the personality scales was strongly associated with the incidence of stroke (table 4). Higher values of symptoms of depression, anger control, and time urgency showed a monotonic association with increased risk of incident stroke. In the fully adjusted models, these trends were less pronounced. Higher degrees of psychoticism were associated with a decreased risk of stroke, and this trend was more pronounced after we controlled for confounding.

table 5 shows that anger control, internal locus of control over disease, psychoticism, and symptoms of depression were not associated with cancer. Time urgency was inversely associated with the risk of cancer; participants with high time urgency had a relative risk for cancer of 0.68 (0.49 to 0.95) compared with those with medium time urgency, and an increase in time urgency of 1 SD was associated with an adjusted relative risk for cancer of 0.83 (0.73 to 0.95).

Discussion

In a large, population based cohort study with a median follow-up of 8.5 years (during which 257 participants died, 72 had a heart attack, 62 had a stroke, and 240 developed cancer) some personality traits were risk factors for morbidity and mortality, independent of lifestyle factors. A low internal locus of control over disease and high and low psychoticism were risk factors for myocardial infarction. A high time urgency was associated with reduced risk of developing cancer. Overall, however, the personality traits had no major impact on the incidence of and mortality from cardiovascular disease and cancer.

Comparison with other studies

Research on personality variables as risk factors for cardiovascular disease has focused on stress, social support, low sense of coherence, and hardiness.9 Findings from the Western Collaborative Group study and the Framingham heart study seemed to establish type A behaviour (the triad of competitive ambition, time urgency, and hostility) as a risk factor for cardiovascular disease.10 11 But at least one large study (MRFIT; multiple risk factor intervention trial) found no association, and a later analysis of the Western Collaborative Group study found that the association between type A behaviour and all cause mortality is more complex.12 13 Because type A behaviour has now been deconstructed, we analysed anger control (inversely associated with hostility) and time urgency separately.9 Neither personality trait was associated with cardiovascular disease, so we cannot confirm the finding of an increased risk of non-fatal myocardial infarction with increasing time urgency seen in a recent case-control study.14

A high internal locus of control over disease has consistently been linked with reduced risk of cardiovascular disease.9 Our results on myocardial infarction agree with those on control over life circumstances (not disease) seen in a large case-control study.1 Residual confounding by healthy behaviours apart from those that we (and others) could control for is a likely explanation for at least part of the reduced risk.

Our finding of increased risk for myocardial infarction with increasing psychoticism needs to be interpreted with caution because of the inconsistency of the results. The analysis based on dividing participants into thirds does not indicate a monotonic association, so that the analysis assuming a monotonic (log linear) trend is probably driven by influential observations at the high end of the highly skewed scale. High values of psychoticism have been associated with less health conscious behaviour in at least one study, but not with myocardial infarction.15

Our finding of no association between personality traits and stroke is especially interesting in the light of emerging research on the individual components of composite end points and the greater contribution of stroke than myocardial infarction to overall risk of cardiovascular disease in women.16

A recent review of the association between psychological factors and cancer concluded that life events (other than loss of a spouse or child), negative emotional states, fighting spirit, stoic acceptance or fatalism, active coping, personality factors, and locus of control have little influence on whether people develop cancer or not.5 We found that a high time urgency was associated with a lower risk of cancer, but the strength of the association was moderate, and risk was not increased in the category of low time urgency. High time urgency has been associated with a decreased risk for all cause mortality in women after myocardial infarction but not with a decreased risk for any type of cancer.17 High time urgency might be associated with delayed diagnosis and thus lower incidence of cancer. Secondary analyses restricted to 83 deaths caused by cancer showed similar results, but with an adjusted hazards ratio of 0.58 (0.32 to 1.05) for high versus medium time urgency and of 0.80 (0.64 to 1.00) for an increase of 1 SD. By looking at cancer overall, we may have missed associations with individual types of cancer. A meta-analysis found a modest increase in risk of breast cancer after the death of a spouse but did not support an overall association between stressful life events and risk of breast cancer.6 A previous review reached similar conclusions.18

Limitations

We lack information on important risk factors for cardiovascular disease and cancer, including blood lipids, markers of subclinical systemic inflammation, diet, and use of drugs (for example, low dose aspirin, statins) during follow-up. Whereas an association between personality variables and biological markers is unlikely, personality variables could be associated with long term use of preventive drugs, screening practices, and reporting of symptoms.19 It might be better not to control for such covariates that are consequences rather than confounders.

Despite the population based recruitment, participants probably do not represent the whole range of personalities in the population. We might have missed associations between extreme patterns of these traits and disease.

Implications

Psychological traits such as the personality variables considered in our analysis are probably stable over time and are likely to have a genetic component.20 21 The more these traits are predetermined, the more they should be seen as part of a risk prediction rather than be used for “victim blaming”. Life events seem to influence the stability of these traits, and some events (such as becoming unemployed) might be avoidable at the level of the individual or society.20 22 We assessed personality variables at baseline with an extensive list of validated scales under standardised conditions, and we used factor analysis to find five personality scales as indicators for five independent personality dimensions without information on outcomes.8 An analysis that adjusted for multiple comparisons found no statistically significant association. We did not adjust the results for multiple comparisons because it is unlikely that all associations between personality traits and disease are random, because we chose all scales without knowledge of the outcomes, and because we present all the associations that we evaluated.23

Mortality and morbidity follow-up were almost complete and the incidence of the chronic diseases was validated, so the estimates of relative risks should be valid, even if we missed some events.

We adjusted our analyses for various lifestyle factors at baseline (smoking, alcohol consumption, body mass index, physical exercise, and education). Since the personality variables and these lifestyle factors were assessed at the same time, we cannot tell how personality variables influenced these lifestyle factors. Psychological traits probably influence the risk of disease only after long latency periods, but they might have more immediate effects through variables such as lifestyle factors (smoking, alcohol consumption, and weight).24 We therefore adjusted for age and sex only as well as for these two factors plus lifestyle factors. Repeated measures and more sophisticated models are needed to determine the causal effect of personality differences on the risk of disease.

What is already known on this topic

Evidence of an effect of personality on disease is best for stress and cardiovascular disease, but little evidence exists for cancer

What this study adds

Overall, the personality traits assessed had no major impact on incidence of and mortality from cardiovascular disease and cancer

Higher internal locus of control over disease (a patient's belief that the onset and progress of disease is a result of their behaviour) may be associated with a reduced risk of myocardial infarction but not of stroke or cancer

Higher time urgency may be associated with a reduced risk of cancer but not of cardiovascular disease (myocardial infarction and stroke)

Footnotes

  • Embedded Image An additional table is on bmj.com

  • Contributors MA and TS designed the study and obtained funding; PH collected the data; TS and PH analysed the data; TS drafted the paper; TS, PH, and MA helped to write the paper. MA is guarantor.

  • Funding German Research Foundation (research grant AM 37/19-1).

  • Competing interests None declared.

  • Ethical approval Ethics committee-I of the medical faculty of Heidelberg, Ruprecht-Karls University of Heidelberg, Germany.

References

  1. 1.
  2. 2.
  3. 3.
  4. 4.
  5. 5.
  6. 6.
  7. 7.
  8. 8.
  9. 9.
  10. 10.
  11. 11.
  12. 12.
  13. 13.
  14. 14.
  15. 15.
  16. 16.
  17. 17.
  18. 18.
  19. 19.
  20. 20.
  21. 21.
  22. 22.
  23. 23.
  24. 24.
View Abstract