Case Study
The Etiology of Autism: Pre-, Peri- and Neonatal Factors

https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199311000-00021Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective

To examine pre-, peri-, and neonatal factors in autism using composite optimality scores.

Method

Pre-, peri-, and neonatal composite optimality scores were examined in 39 autistic subjects and 39 randomly matched sibling controls using a modification of the Gillberg Optimality Scale (Modified-GOS). Scores were based on best-estimate ratings of maternal interviews and medical records. Rules for best-estimate ratings were derived from a study of agreement between these two sources.

Results

Significant differences in optimality between autistic probands and their siblings were not present after adjustment for “maternal parity.‘’ Examination of specific variables revealed that only maternal parity differed significantly between autistic subjects and randomly matched sibling controls, reflecting an excess of first and fourth born among the autistic subjects.

Conclusion

The findings of this study suggest that previous reports of an association between optimality and autism are aresult of failure to adjust for birth order. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 1993, 32,6:1256–1263.

REFERENCES (37)

  • E.Y. Deykin et al.

    Pregnancy, delivery, and neonatal complications among autistic children

    Am. J. Dis. Child.

    (1980)
  • J-A. Finegan et al.

    Pre-, peri- and neonatal factors and infantile autism

    J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry

    (1979)
  • S.E. Folstein et al.

    Etiology of autism: genetic influences

    Pediatrics

    (1991)
  • C. Gillberg et al.

    Infantile autism: a total population study of reduced optimality in the pre-, peri-, and neonatal period

    J. Autism Dev. Disord.

    (1983)
  • D. Hewson et al.

    Childbirth research data: medical records or women's reports?

    Am. J. Epidemiol.

    (1987)
  • M.B. Jones et al.

    Stoppage rules and genetic studies of autism

    J. Autism Dev. Disord.

    (1988)
  • H. Knobloch et al.

    Some etiologic and prognostic factors in early infantile autism and psychosis

    Pediatrics

    (1975)
  • A. LeCouteur et al.

    Autism diagnostic interview: a standardized investigator-based instrument

    J. Autism Dev. Disord.

    (1989)
  • Cited by (102)

    • Developmental neurotoxicity and autism: A potential link between indoor neuroactive pollutants and the curious birth order risk factor

      2017, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition to body and environmental heat, the primary factor affecting volatile exposure is the quantity of new synthetic material encountered by the infant; families are most likely to acquire new products for first-borns and to reuse them for subsequent children,1 who would benefit from prolonged depletion of emitted neuroactive agents. Interestingly, an oft-found secondary spike in autism rates for fourth-born (and later) children (Deykin and MacMahon, 1980; Tsai and Stewart, 1983; Lord et al., 1991; Piven et al., 1993; Bolton et al., 1997) might correspond to the point when passed-down (and presumably out-gassed) products are sufficiently worn out to induce replacement with fresh (volatile-rich) products. Affluence presumably also affects the quantity of new synthetic material encountered by the infant, not just in infant products, but in household furnishings, flooring and automobiles.

    • Perinatal and background risk factors for childhood autism in central China

      2014, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Many studies over the past decades have attempted to identify genetic, organic, maternal, neurochemical, immunological, and microbial factors that may increase the risk of autism in order to develop preventative strategies (Nelson, 1991; Piven et al., 1993; Croen et al., 2007; Kolevzon et al., 2007; Wallace et al., 2008; Longo et al., 2009; Matsuo et al., 2011). Earlier studies indicated that children with autism were exposed to specific gestational and perinatal risks such as elderly gravida, exposure to certain medications during pregnancy, and maternal infection, and were more likely to have low birth weight, premature delivery, various kinds of injury during delivery, and respiratory distress syndrome (Nelson, 1991; Piven et al., 1993). Both Croen et al. (2007) and Kolevzon et al. (2007) found that the risk of autism increases when parents are older, when the maternal relatives have emotional and anxiety disorders, and when paternal relatives have learning and attention deficits.

    • The prenatal, perinatal and neonatal risk factors for children's developmental coordination disorder: A population study in mainland China

      2014, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      Moreover, threatened abortion at <20 weeks were significantly more common in the mothers of DCD children when it is compared with control subjects and attained the highest OR value among the prenatal risks. Threatened abortion or bleeding during pregnancy has previously been reported in mothers whose children having developmental diseases such as autism and Pervasive Developmental (Glasson et al., 2004; Juul-Dam, Townsend, & Courchesne, 2001; Piven et al., 1993). However, few studies have been conducted in the relationship between threatened abortion and motor impairment.

    • Prenatal, perinatal and neonatal risk factors of Autism Spectrum Disorder: A comprehensive epidemiological assessment from India

      2013, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      Hence, it is imperative to focus of prenatal, perinatal events as risk factors for ASD. There has been a huge focus on pre- and perinatal events as risk factors in various studies across the globe, wherein disruptions and disorders of pregnancy, labor complications, fetal distress, low birth weight and premature birth have been studied and implicated in ASD (Bilder, Pinborough-Zimmerman, Miller, & McMahon, 2009; Bolton et al., 1997; Bryson, Smith, & Eastwood, 1988; Buchmayer et al., 2009; Burd, Severud, Kerbeshian, & Klug, 1999; Burstyn, Sithole, & Zwaigenbaum, 2010; Cryan, Byrne, O‘Donovan, & O‘Callaghan, 1996; Deb et al., 1997; Deykin & MacMahon, 1980; Dodds et al., 2011; Dubovický, 2010; El-Baz et al., 2011; Finegan & Quarrington, 1979; Gardener et al., 2011; Ghaziuddin, Shakal, & Tsai, 1995; Gillberg & Gillberg, 1983; Glasson et al., 2004; Guinchat et al., 2012; Hultman, Sparén, & Cnattingius, 2002; Johnson et al., 2010; Juul-Dam, Townsend, & Courchesne, 2001; Kinney, Munir, Crowley, & Miller, 2008; Kolevzon, Gross, & Reichenberg, 2007; Kröger et al., 2011; Kuban et al., 2009; Larsson et al., 2005; Lord, Mulloy, Wendelboe, & Schopler, 1991; Losh, Esserman, Anckarsäter, Sullivan, & Lichtenstein, 2012; Maimburg & Vaeth, 2006; Mason-brothers et al., 1987; Nelson, 1991; Ornitz, 1985; Piven et al., 1993; Schendel & Bhasin, 2008; Stein, Weizman, Ring, & Barak, 2006; Wallace, Anderson, & Dubrow, 2008; Zambrino, Balottin, Bettaglio, Gerardo, & Lanzi, 1995; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2002). Despite several studies being conducted worldwide to analyze the risk factors of ASD, the results are not conclusive.

    • The Environment in Autism Spectrum Disorders

      2013, The Neuroscience of Autism Spectrum Disorders
    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This research was supported by NIMH Grant RO1 MH39936-04 (Dr. Folstein) and The John Merck Fund (Dr. Piven). The authors acknowledge the help of Pat Palmer, Ph.D., Stephan Arndt, Ph.D., and Janiece Thein.

    View full text