Abstract

Social cognition provides humans with the necessary skills to understand and interact with one another. One aspect of social cognition, mentalizing, is associated with a network of brain regions often referred to as the ‘social brain.’ These consist of medial prefrontal cortex [medial Brodmann Area 10 (mBA10)], temporoparietal junction (TPJ), posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) and anterior temporal cortex (ATC). How these specific regions develop structurally across late childhood and adolescence is not well established. This study examined the structural developmental trajectories of social brain regions in the longest ongoing longitudinal neuroimaging study of human brain maturation. Structural trajectories of grey matter volume, cortical thickness and surface area were analyzed using surface-based cortical reconstruction software and mixed modeling in a longitudinal sample of 288 participants (ages 7–30 years, 857 total scans). Grey matter volume and cortical thickness in mBA10, TPJ and pSTS decreased from childhood into the early twenties. The ATC increased in grey matter volume until adolescence and in cortical thickness until early adulthood. Surface area for each region followed a cubic trajectory, peaking in early or pre-adolescence before decreasing into the early twenties. These results are discussed in the context of developmental changes in social cognition across adolescence.

INTRODUCTION

Mentalizing, the ability to infer the intentions, beliefs and desires of others to predict their behavior, is fundamental to human development. In a large number of functional neuroimaging studies using a wide variety of mentalizing tasks, the process of mental state attribution has been associated with a network of brain regions that include the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), temporoparietal junction (TPJ), posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) and anterior temporal cortex (ATC; Frith and Frith, 2003; Blakemore, 2012). Developmental functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shown changes in recruitment within this ‘social brain network’ across adolescence (see Burnett et al., 2011 for review). However, little is known about the way in which these regions of the social brain network develop structurally during this period of life. In this study, we investigated structural development in regions of the social brain network from ages 7 to 30 years using data from the longest ongoing longitudinal pediatric neuroimaging sample (Giedd et al., 1996).

From infancy, humans display signs of social cognition (Striano and Reid, 2006; Kovács et al., 2010). For many years it was assumed that social cognitive development was mostly complete in childhood (Wimmer and Perner, 1983). However, recent behavioral research demonstrates that online social cognitive skills improve across adolescence (Dumontheil et al., 2010a). Several fMRI studies have shown differences in functional recruitment of the social brain network between adolescence and adulthood during social cognitive tasks (Blakemore, 2008, 2010). Despite the variety of paradigms used, from understanding irony (Wang et al., 2006) to thinking about social emotions like guilt (Burnett et al., 2009), many developmental fMRI studies of social cognition to date have reported decreased recruitment of dorsal mPFC in adolescents as compared with adults (Wang et al., 2006; Blakemore et al., 2007; Pfeifer et al., 2007, 2009; Burnett et al., 2009; Sebastian et al., 2011; Goddings et al., 2012). In some studies, higher activity in more posterior regions, such as the pSTS/TPJ (Blakemore et al., 2007), and in the ATC (Burnett et al., 2009), was observed in adults compared to adolescents. These changes in functional recruitment have been hypothesized to reflect changes in neurocognitive strategy and/or neuroanatomy (Blakemore, 2008). As decreases in functional activity can co-occur with reductions in grey matter volume (Lu et al., 2009; Cohen Kadosh et al., 2012), it is informative to describe the typical structural developmental trajectories of social brain regions.

The human brain undergoes profound changes in anatomy across the first decades of life (Webb et al., 2001; Petanjek et al., 2011). Neuroimaging methods, such as MRI, have enabled the investigation of these anatomical changes in large, longitudinal samples (Giedd et al., 1996). These studies have consistently shown continuing neuroanatomical development in grey and white matter (Giedd et al., 1999; Sowell et al., 2003; Brain Development Cooperative Group, 2012), with association cortices reducing in grey matter volume across adolescence. Whereas previous studies have investigated changes in structure across the entire brain, we have limited our analysis to four a priori regions of interest (ROIs) within the social brain network. Additionally, we have utilized surface-based cortical reconstruction software to distinguish grey matter volume, cortical thickness and surface area trajectories in our ROIs. As previous studies using vertex-wise analysis, lobar-level analysis or large numbers of ROIs encompassing the whole brain have typically shown inverted u-shaped trajectories, or a general decrease, in grey matter volume across the frontal, temporal and parietal cortices, we predicted similar trajectories in all regions examined in our analysis. Previous studies investigating cortical thickness across late childhood have shown diverse patterns, including increasing cortical thickness in medial prefrontal and anterior temporal cortices (Sowell et al., 2004), decreasing cortical thickness in the frontal pole (van Soelen et al., 2012) and decreasing cortical thickness in lateral parietal lobes (Sowell et al., 2004; van Soelen et al., 2012). Investigations of cortical thickness across the second decade and into adulthood have shown increasing cortical thickness in the temporal lobe until mid-adolescence (Shaw et al., 2008), and decreasing cortical thickness in medial frontal and lateral parietal lobes (Shaw et al., 2008; Tamnes et al., 2010). Based on these findings, we predicted either an inverted u-shaped pattern or general decrease in cortical thickness for all the regions in this study. However, changes in surface area across development have been less well characterized, although investigators are beginning to include analyses of surface area in studies (Ostby et al., 2009; Raznahan et al., 2011; Shaw et al., 2012). We predicted that the surface area would show an inverted u-shaped trajectory reaching a peak in early/pre-adolescence, similar to what was found by Raznahan et al. (2011) for the entire cortex. Although we did not predict sex differences in these regions based on differential social cognitive development between females and males, we did predict that females would reach a peak in grey matter volume and surface area, but not in cortical thickness, earlier than males (Raznahan et al., 2011).

METHODS

Participants

The sample consisted of 288 unrelated individuals (124 females, 164 males), each of whom had undergone two or more high-quality MRI scans ∼2 years apart over the age range of 7–30 years. The average age that participants displayed the earliest signs of pubertal onset (Tanner Stage 2), as measured by a self-report (parents or participants) Tanner stage diagrams (Taylor et al., 2001), in a subset of individuals (41 females, 51 males), was 10.01 years (9.94 for females, 10.06 for males). This subset was chosen based on how many individuals were participants in the project while in Tanner Stage 2, and the availability of data. As adolescence is defined as the time period between puberty and relative self-sufficiency, when we refer to the period of adolescence in the context of this study, we are referring to roughly 10–18 years of age. Of the 288 individuals, 126 were members of twin pairs (55 females, 71 males); only one member per twin pair was included in the analysis. There were no significant differences between males and females in socioeconomic status (SES), handedness, race, intelligence quotient (IQ) or number of scans (Table 1). The absence of neurological or psychiatric illness was established through a telephone screening interview and completion of a parent-report screening questionnaire (Childhood Behavior Checklist). Handedness was established using Physical and Neurological Examination of Soft Signs inventory (Denckla, 1985). All participants had a full-scale IQ >80 (IQ was estimated using age-appropriate Wechsler Intelligence Scales). SES was quantified using Hollingshead Scales (Hollingshead, 1975). Sample characteristics are detailed in Table 1.

Table 1

Participant characteristics

Group
Sex difference
CharacteristicAllFemaleMale
Number of individuals288124164n.s.
 Singleton1626993
 Member of twin pair1265571
Handedness, no.n.s.
 Right255107148
 Mixed211011
 Left1174
Race, no.n.s.
 Caucasian250107143
 African-American211011
 Asian211
 Hispanic844
 Other725
IQn.s.
 Mean (s.d.)114 (12.0)113 (12.5)115 (11.5)
SESn.s.
 Mean (s.d.)41 (18.2)42 (17.5)40 (18.7)
Number of scans, no.n.s.
 2 scans1386078
 3 scans733637
 4 scans401129
 5 scans231211
 6 scans1156
 7 scans303
Total857362495
Age distribution  of scans (years)
 Mean (s.d.)14.9 (4.9)14.4 (4.9)15.2 (4.8)
 Range7.0–30.67.1–29.57.0–30.6
Group
Sex difference
CharacteristicAllFemaleMale
Number of individuals288124164n.s.
 Singleton1626993
 Member of twin pair1265571
Handedness, no.n.s.
 Right255107148
 Mixed211011
 Left1174
Race, no.n.s.
 Caucasian250107143
 African-American211011
 Asian211
 Hispanic844
 Other725
IQn.s.
 Mean (s.d.)114 (12.0)113 (12.5)115 (11.5)
SESn.s.
 Mean (s.d.)41 (18.2)42 (17.5)40 (18.7)
Number of scans, no.n.s.
 2 scans1386078
 3 scans733637
 4 scans401129
 5 scans231211
 6 scans1156
 7 scans303
Total857362495
Age distribution  of scans (years)
 Mean (s.d.)14.9 (4.9)14.4 (4.9)15.2 (4.8)
 Range7.0–30.67.1–29.57.0–30.6

n.s., not statistically significant at P < 0.05.

Demographic characteristics for the sample. There were no significant differences in IQ, handedness, SES, race or number of scans between female and male participants.

Table 1

Participant characteristics

Group
Sex difference
CharacteristicAllFemaleMale
Number of individuals288124164n.s.
 Singleton1626993
 Member of twin pair1265571
Handedness, no.n.s.
 Right255107148
 Mixed211011
 Left1174
Race, no.n.s.
 Caucasian250107143
 African-American211011
 Asian211
 Hispanic844
 Other725
IQn.s.
 Mean (s.d.)114 (12.0)113 (12.5)115 (11.5)
SESn.s.
 Mean (s.d.)41 (18.2)42 (17.5)40 (18.7)
Number of scans, no.n.s.
 2 scans1386078
 3 scans733637
 4 scans401129
 5 scans231211
 6 scans1156
 7 scans303
Total857362495
Age distribution  of scans (years)
 Mean (s.d.)14.9 (4.9)14.4 (4.9)15.2 (4.8)
 Range7.0–30.67.1–29.57.0–30.6
Group
Sex difference
CharacteristicAllFemaleMale
Number of individuals288124164n.s.
 Singleton1626993
 Member of twin pair1265571
Handedness, no.n.s.
 Right255107148
 Mixed211011
 Left1174
Race, no.n.s.
 Caucasian250107143
 African-American211011
 Asian211
 Hispanic844
 Other725
IQn.s.
 Mean (s.d.)114 (12.0)113 (12.5)115 (11.5)
SESn.s.
 Mean (s.d.)41 (18.2)42 (17.5)40 (18.7)
Number of scans, no.n.s.
 2 scans1386078
 3 scans733637
 4 scans401129
 5 scans231211
 6 scans1156
 7 scans303
Total857362495
Age distribution  of scans (years)
 Mean (s.d.)14.9 (4.9)14.4 (4.9)15.2 (4.8)
 Range7.0–30.67.1–29.57.0–30.6

n.s., not statistically significant at P < 0.05.

Demographic characteristics for the sample. There were no significant differences in IQ, handedness, SES, race or number of scans between female and male participants.

Participants were recruited from the community through local advertisement and paid for their participation in the study (Giedd et al., 1996). The institutional review board of the National Institutes of Health approved the research protocol employed in this study and written informed consent and assent to participate in the study were obtained from parents/adult participants and children, respectively.

Image acquisition

All MRI scans were T1-weighted images with contiguous 1.5 mm axial slices and 2.0 mm coronal slices, obtained on the same 1.5-T General Electric Signa scanner (Milwaukee, WI, USA) using a three-dimensional spoiled gradient-recalled echo sequence with the following parameters: echo time, 5 ms; repetition time, 24 ms; flip angle, 45°; acquisition matrix, 256 × 192; number of excitations, 1; and field of view, 24 cm. A clinical neuroradiologist evaluated all scans for gross abnormalities.

Image processing

Cortical reconstruction was performed with the Freesurfer5.1 image analysis suite, which is documented and freely available for download online (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). The technical details of these procedures are described in prior publications (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999a). The processing stream for structural images includes motion correction (Reuter et al., 2010), removal of non-brain tissue using a hybrid watershed/surface deformation procedure (Segonne et al., 2004), automated Talairach transformation, intensity normalization (Sled et al., 1998), tessellation of the grey matter white matter boundary, automated topology correction (Fischl et al., 2001; Segonne et al., 2007) and surface deformation following intensity gradients to optimally place the grey/white and grey/cerebrospinal fluid borders at the location where the greatest shift in intensity defines the transition to the other tissue class (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl and Dale, 2000). Only one anatomical scan was processed per scanning session (timepoint). For the purposes of this analysis, we have averaged together these two surface area measurements to obtain a measurement of the middle grey surface area. Each cortical reconstruction was visually inspected (by author K.L.M.), and unsuccessful cortical reconstructions were identified and excluded from the present analyses. This left a sample of 288 individuals (857 scans) for the analyses of mBA10, TPJ and pSTS, and a sample of 221 individuals (447 scans) for the analysis of the ATC. Each cortical model was registered to a spherical atlas using individual cortical folding patterns to match cortical geometry across subjects (Fischl et al., 1999b). Measurements of mean cortical thickness (mm), middle grey surface area (mm2) and grey matter volume (mm3) were extracted for each region of interest.

Regions of interests

Four regions of interest (ROIs) were created for each hemisphere using an averaged template in Freesurfer. These ROIs, illustrated in Figure 1, include medial Brodmann Area 10 (mBA10), TPJ, pSTS and ATC. As this analysis necessitated well-defined borders for all ROIs, we chose mBA10 as our proxy for dorsal mPFC. The mBA10 itself has been characterized in meta-analyses as a neural correlate of mentalizing (Amodio and Frith, 2006; Gilbert et al., 2006). Structural measurements for each hemisphere were combined to produce one value for each ROI. Posthoc analyses for the left and right hemispheres were performed to examine hemispheric differences. We describe how we defined each ROI below.

Fig. 1

The social brain. Social brain regions of interest include medial Brodmann Area 10 (mBA10; green), temporoparietal junction (TPJ; orange), posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS; pink) and anterior temporal cortex (ATC; blue).

mBA10

The medial portion of BA10 was defined using the PALS B12 Brodmann atlas projected onto an averaged Freesurfer template (Van Essen, 2005). From this atlas, the BA10 label was selected, and any area of this label located outside of the medial cortex was eliminated.

TPJ

The TPJ was defined using the border coordinates of a functional subdivision generated in a previous study (Mars et al., 2012). This study defined three consistent functional subregions within a larger TPJ area in the right hemisphere, each with unique functional and structural connectivity profiles. Of the three subregions, we chose to use the posterior TPJ subregion, as it was functionally connected to other areas of the social brain, such as mBA10 and ATC (Mars et al., 2012).

pSTS

The pSTS region was created by extending the Desikan-Killiany Atlas defined bank STS (Fischl et al., 2004; Desikan et al., 2006) to the border of the TPJ.

ATC

The ATC was defined by extending the temporal pole, defined as Brodmann Area 38 in the PALS B12 Brodmann atlas, back to include areas of the ATC recruited during mentalizing tasks in previous fMRI studies (see Olson et al., 2007 for a review).

Statistical analysis

We used mixed models (Pinheiro and Bates, 2000) to estimate the fixed effects of age on each measure, with nested random effects terms modeled for within person dependence of observations. For example, an equation for a model using cubic grey matter volume (GM) growth for ith individual’s jth timepoint is: GMij = Intercept + di + β1(age) + β2(age2) + β3(age3) + εij. Age terms were centered to the mean age (i.e. the mean age, 14.89 years, was subtracted from each age) to reduce the correlation between the age, age-squared and age-cubed terms. For each measure, an F-test was used to determine whether a cubic, quadratic or linear growth model best fit the entire sample.

After determining the best growth model for the entire sample, we conducted likelihood-ratio (LR) tests to determine if either the growth curve height or shape were statistically different between sexes. First, to establish if growth curve height was significantly different between males and females, we conducted a LR test to determine whether the best fitted model including age and a main effect of sex predicted significantly more variance in the measure of interest compared with the model including age terms alone. Second, to establish if the growth curve shape for the measure of interest was significantly different between males and females, we conducted a LR test to determine whether the best fitted model including interactions between age terms and sex predicted significantly more variance in the measure of interest compared with a simpler model including only age terms and a main effect of sex. For measures of interest following non-linear growth curves, age-at-peak was calculated by solving the first-order derivative of the growth trajectory equation that had been defined for that measure using mixed modeling.

RESULTS

Medial BA10

Grey matter volume for mBA10 followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 9.51, P < 0.003) decreasing steadily from age 9 to 22 years. Cortical thickness for mBA10 decreased linearly from childhood into the early twenties (F(1,568) = 329.34, P < 0.001), whereas the surface area followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 5.67, P < 0.018), peaking at 8.0 years and decreasing into the early twenties (Figure 2). Grey matter volume for the left mBA10 followed a linear trajectory (F(1,568) = 219.46, P < 0.0001), and right mBA10 followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 12.72, P < 0.0005). Cortical thickness for the left mBA10 followed a quadratic trajectory (F(1,567) = 7.93, P < 0.006), and right mBA10 followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 7.18, P < 0.008). Surface area for the left mBA10 followed a linear trajectory (F(1,568) = 32.97, P < 0.0001), and right mBA10 followed a linear trajectory (F(1,568) = 89.11, P < 0.0001) (Supplementary Figures S1 and Supplementary Data).

Fig. 2

Fitted models of structural development across adolescence. The best fitting models for all participants are shown for each region of interest (combined hemispheres). Models are fitted to the middle 80% of the sample (ages 9–22 years for mBA10, TPJ and pSTS; ages 11–24 years for ATC). The lighter lines show the fitted models applied to females only, and the darker lines show the fitted models applied to males only. Solid lines indicate the fitted model was significant P < 0.05, whereas dashed lines indicate the fitted model was not significant (P ≥ 0.05).

TPJ

Grey matter volume for the TPJ followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 44.06, P < 0.0001) peaking at 9.3 years and then decreasing into the early twenties. Cortical thickness for the TPJ decreased linearly from childhood into the early twenties (F(1,568) = 475.81, P < 0.0001), whereas the surface area followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 14.73, P < 0.0002) peaking at 11.5 years and decreasing into the early twenties. Grey matter volume for the left TPJ followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 20.00, P < 0.0001), and right TPJ followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 37.46, P < 0.0001). Cortical thickness for the left TPJ followed a linear trajectory (F(1,568) = 341.44, P < 0.0001), and right TPJ followed a linear trajectory (F(1,568) = 354.93, P < 0.0001). Surface area for the left TPJ followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 10.73, P < 0.002), and right TPJ followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 6.306, P < 0.02).

pSTS

Grey matter volume for the pSTS followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 25.90, P < 0.0001) peaking at 8.7 years and then decreasing into the early twenties. Cortical thickness for the pSTS decreased linearly from childhood into the early twenties (F(1,568) = 574.07, P < 0.0001), whereas the surface area followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,568) = 6.98, P < 0.009) peaking at 12.9 years and decreasing into the early twenties. Grey matter volume for the left pSTS followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,568) = 16.39, P < 0.0002), and right pSTS followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 14.98, P < 0.0002). Cortical thickness for the left pSTS followed a linear trajectory (F(1,568) = 444.21, P < 0.0001), and right pSTS followed a linear trajectory (F(1,568) = 335.75, P < 0.0001). Surface area for the left pSTS followed a linear trajectory (F(1,568) = 12.17, P < 0.0006), and right pSTS followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,566) = 4.82, P < 0.03).

ATC

Grey matter volume for the ATC followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,223) = 6.31, P < 0.02) peaking at age 12.6 years and then decreasing into the mid-twenties. Cortical thickness increased from childhood following a quadratic trajectory (F(1,224) = 9.56, P < 0.002) peaking at 19.4 years, whereas surface area followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,224) = 3.91, P < 0.05) peaking at 8.3 years and decreasing into the mid-twenties. Grey matter volume for the left ATC followed a linear trajectory (F(1,225) = 13.96, P < 0.0003), and right ATC followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,223) = 7.12, P < 0.009). Cortical thickness for the left ATC followed a quadratic trajectory (F(1,224) = 5.10, P < 0.03), and right ATC followed a linear trajectory (F(1,225) = 3.96, P < 0.05). Surface area for the left ATC followed a linear trajectory (F(1,225) = 32.22, P < 0.0001), and right ATC followed a cubic trajectory (F(1,223) = 4.60, P < 0.04).

Sex differences

Sex differences were observed in grey matter volume and surface area, but not in cortical thickness, for all ROIs. Grey matter volume trajectory heights were significantly different between females and males in mBA10 (LR = 34.68, P < 0.0001), TPJ (LR = 33.41, P < 0.0001), pSTS (LR = 38.10, P < 0.0001) and ATC (LR = 67.43, P < 0.0001), with females displaying smaller volumes than males across ages 7–30 years. Surface area trajectory heights were significantly different between females and males in mBA10 (LR = 53.83, P < 0.0001), TPJ (LR = 42.26, P < 0.0001), pSTS (LR = 41.30, P < 0.0001) and ATC (LR = 54.43, P < 0.0001), with females displaying smaller surface areas than males across ages 7–30 years. Additionally, a significant difference in trajectory shape was observed in TPJ surface area (LR = 9.89, P < 0.02), with females reaching a peak in surface area earlier than males (10.2 years vs. 12.6 years, respectively). No differences in cortical thickness were observed between the sexes in the combined hemispheres and in the individual hemispheres. Left and right hemispheres showed the same effects for all measurements as the combined hemispheres for mBA10 and pSTS. The interactive effect for surface area in the TPJ did not hold in each individual hemisphere, but grey matter volume and surface area trajectory heights were significantly different between females and males. Grey matter volume for the ATC in the left hemisphere showed a significant interactive effect with sex (LR = 4.60, P < 0.04), whereas grey matter volume for the ATC in the right hemisphere, as well as surface area for both hemispheres, displayed a main effect of sex, as was found in the combined hemispheres.

DISCUSSION

The aim of this study was to investigate the structural development of the social brain network across adolescence using data from the longest ongoing longitudinal pediatric neuroimaging sample (288 participants; 857 scans; ages 7–30 years) (Giedd et al., 1996). While grey matter volume in mBA10, TPJ and pSTS reached a peak in late childhood before decreasing into adulthood, the ATC increased in grey matter volume until adolescence (∼12 years), decreasing thereafter. Cortical thickness in mBA10, TPJ and pSTS decreased linearly across adolescence, largely consistent with results in previous studies (Ostby et al., 2009; Tamnes et al., 2010; van Soelen et al., 2012; but see Shaw et al., 2008). However, cortical thickness in the ATC followed a quadratic trajectory, increasing until early adulthood. Surface area for each region followed a cubic trajectory, reaching a peak in early or pre-adolescence before decreasing into the early twenties, whereas previous lobar-level studies have shown general decreases for the frontal, temporal and parietal cortices (Ostby et al., 2009). Differences in grey matter volume and surface area, but not in cortical thickness, were observed between female and male participants. Males displayed larger cortical volumes and greater surface areas than females across all ROIs. Differences in age-at-peak were only observed for the surface area of the TPJ, which reached a peak 2 years earlier in females than in males. Although we report peak ages in this study, these ages should be interpreted with caution since factors such as age range can affect estimated peaks when using global models (i.e. linear, quadratic and cubic models) (Fjell et al., 2010). As grey matter volume is the product of surface area and cortical thickness, it may be that the sex differences observed in grey matter volumes are driven by differences in surface area rather than cortical thickness.

The social brain network

While the co-activation of regions examined in this study has been demonstrated in many social cognitive fMRI experiments, the individual contributions of these anatomically distinct regions to social cognitive processes is still under debate. Electrophysiological and fMRI studies consistently report the involvement of the pSTS in the perception of biological motion and eye gaze (Puce and Perrett, 2003), and in grasping the intentionality and appropriateness of biological motion (Pelphrey et al., 2004). It may be that the pSTS is involved in decoding complex social gestures conveyed through eye gaze and body movement. The TPJ, while in close anatomical proximity to the pSTS, is involved in different aspects of social cognition. It is suggested that the TPJ is activated specifically in situations when one is inferring the mental states of others, rather than just information known about another (Saxe and Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe et al., 2009). In contrast, mBA10 is activated in multiple conditions: when inferring the mental states of others, when reflecting on knowledge of another’s traits, and when reflecting on the traits of oneself (Frith, 2007). Frith (2007) has proposed that the underlying similarity between tasks that activate mBA10 is their involvement in handling communicative intentions, which requires a second order representation of mental state, whether our own or another’s. A combination of lesion, non-human primate and fMRI studies has prompted researchers to theorize the involvement of the ATC in interpreting social narratives (Olson et al., 2007), and processing social scripts (Frith and Frith, 2003; Frith 2007).

Many of the ROIs followed similar structural trajectories, with mBA10, TPJ and pSTS reaching a peak in grey matter volume during childhood, followed by a steady decrease in adolescence before leveling off in the early twenties. However, the ATC showed a different trajectory, increasing in grey matter volume until adolescence, before gradually decreasing until leveling off in the mid-twenties. It is unclear why the ATC would continue to increase in grey matter volume so late in development, reaching a peak in grey matter volume later than the onset of puberty. This area of the cortex contains important linkages to both the mPFC and limbic structures (e.g. amygdala, hippocampus) via the uncinate fasciculus, which is one of the last white matter tracts to reach maturity (Lebel et al., 2008). Perhaps the relatively late myelination of ATC projections allows a longer window for learning social scripts. Future investigations that measure white matter in social brain regions are needed to determine the role of white matter maturity (i.e. axonal caliber, myelination) in the development of these regions. Despite differences in the timing and tempo of grey matter volume decline between regions of the social brain, each region continues to change across adolescence before relatively stabilizing in the early twenties.

Underlying anatomy and histology

Until recently, most structural MRI studies of the developing brain have examined only grey and white matter volumes in relatively large regions. Grey matter volume itself is the product of cortical thickness and surface area, which are influenced by distinct genetic (Panizzon et al., 2009; Winkler et al., 2010), evolutionary (Rakic, 1995) and cellular (Chenn and Walsh, 2002) processes, in addition to being phenotypically distinct (Winkler et al., 2010). Grey matter volume is more correlated with, and genetically and environmentally related to, surface area than cortical thickness (Winkler et al., 2010). Differences in surface area are pronounced across species (Rakic, 1995; Hill et al., 2010), whereas cortical thickness is highly conserved (Roth and Dicke, 2005; Fish et al., 2008). Interestingly, areas similar to regions explored in this study have greatly expanded in surface area across evolution (Hill et al., 2010). Furthermore, many of these regions showed relatively greater surface area expansion between infancy and adulthood, although mBA10 is a notable exception (Hill et al., 2010).

The dissimilarity of cortical thickness and surface area growth across late childhood and adolescence is observable in the results of this study. Surface area is much more variable than cortical thickness between individuals (Figure 3). Perhaps the combination of decreasing linear trajectories in cortical thickness and subtle cubic trajectories in surface area observed in mBA10, TPJ and pSTS contribute to the early-peaking (and more profoundly decreasing) grey matter volume trajectories observed in this study. In contrast, the late-peaking cortical thickness and gradually decreasing surface area of the ATC shapes the grey matter trajectory to resemble the surface area trajectory with a later peak around 12 years.

Fig. 3

Scatter plots of all participants for each measure of each region of interest. There are 288 individuals and 857 scans represented in the mBA10, TPJ and pSTS graphs, and 221 individuals and 447 scans represented in the ATC graphs.

The underlying mechanisms associated with a reduction in grey matter volume are still debated (see Paus et al., 2008), and we are unaware of any studies that have tested the relationship between developmental changes in underlying cellular or synaptic anatomy and structural MRI measures. Despite these limitations, it is thought that reductions in grey matter volume may reflect synaptic reorganization and/or increases in white matter integrity (Paus et al., 2008). Histological studies of postmortem human brain tissue support the idea that association cortices continue to undergo synaptic pruning across adolescence (Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997; Petanjek et al., 2011), although the regions examined in these studies do not include the regions included in the present analysis. There is also histological evidence for an extended period of myelination in association cortices, continuing well into adolescence (Yakovlev and Lecours, 1967).

Relationship between structure and function

While this study describes how areas of the social brain develop structurally from late childhood into early adulthood, there have been a number of fMRI studies that show functional changes in the social brain during this period (see Burnett et al., 2011 for review). Many of these studies report decreases in dorsal mPFC recruitment between adolescence and adulthood during social cognitive tasks (Wang et al., 2006; Blakemore et al., 2007; Pfeifer et al., 2007, 2009; Burnett et al., 2009; Sebastian et al., 2011). Why adolescents recruit the mPFC, an area involved in decoding communicative intent and second order mental state representation, more than adults in social cognitive tasks is still an open question. It has been suggested that the decrease in recruitment of the mPFC across adolescence may relate to changes in neuroanatomy or maturing neurocognitive strategies (Blakemore, 2008). Likewise, the protracted structural development of the PFC is often interpreted as reflecting the relative neuroplasticity of this region during adolescence (Giedd et al., 1999). This suggests that structural and functional changes in similar brain regions are co-occurring across development. This idea is supported by a study that correlated cortical grey matter thickness and functional brain activity in typically developing children performing an orthographic processing task (Lu et al., 2009). The authors of this study found that increased activation correlated with mature brain morphology in the same region, and that both of these measures correlate with performance even after accounting for age. However, this study did not involve social cognition, and future developmental studies may begin to characterize the relationship between structural and functional changes by recording structural measures as a potential covariate in fMRI analysis, as some have already begun to do (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2012). This study demonstrated that both age-related and performance-related fMRI activation during a face processing task correlated with structural changes in some, but not all, of the same brain regions (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2012). Another study by Dumontheil et al. (2010b), found that individual differences in grey and white matter volumes could not account for the age-related changes in fMRI activation during a relational reasoning task. These mixed results suggest that age-related changes in blood oxygen level dependent signal do not entirely reflect structural maturation, and may instead reflect the maturation of neurocognitive strategies.

Future directions and limitations

This analysis did not correlate brain structure with social cognitive skill. By correlating structural brain development trajectories in the social brain with social cognitive skills, future studies could characterize inter-individual differences in brain development as they relate to social cognition. It may be that extreme variations in tempo or timing of social brain development correlate with disorders of social cognition (e.g., autism spectrum disorders). However, as it is currently difficult to disentangle genetically pre-programmed developmental changes from those that are triggered by changes in the environment, it may not be possible to speculate on how social cognitive development influences the developmental changes occurring in the brain across adolescence and vice versa.

We also do not address how puberty may affect the observed structural brain development trajectories, as we do not have pubertal measures on all participants in this sample. Pubertal maturation does seem to correlate with brain structure (Neufang et al., 2009; Peper et al., 2009; Bramen et al., 2011), and it would be informative to explore the influence of puberty on the structural trajectories of these social brain regions.

To further describe how regions of the social brain develop across adolescence, it would be beneficial to utilize structural and functional connectivity analytic methods. These methods illustrate how each region within the network communicates with the others, and changes in connectivity strength may reflect maturing neurocognitive strategies. Although previous fMRI studies have shown that recruitment of specific regions of the social brain changes across development, it is unclear how these regions interact during this period of time. As various brain networks have been shown to change in both organization and connectivity strength across adolescence (Fair et al., 2007, 2008, 2009, 2010), the social brain network may also undergo reorganization during this period.

Finally, we would like to address the issue of quality control in structural MRI studies. This large sample included only high-quality structural MRI volumes that had each been visually inspected and rated for motion and artefacts. However, many of the cortical reconstructions for these high-quality scans failed the Freesurfer pipeline for one region only, the ATC. It is important to note that this is not a problem with Freesurfer alone, but also other cortical reconstruction software, and future studies investigating the ATC would benefit from visual inspection of this area for quality control purposes. While this analysis uses the standard Freesurfer pipeline, we acknowledge that the new longitudinal Freesurfer pipeline could improve the cortical reconstruction process, and are excited to implement the longitudinal pipeline in future studies.

CONCLUSION

The social brain network continues to develop structurally across adolescence before relatively stabilizing in the early twenties. This protracted development lends support to the theory that areas of the brain involved in social cognition are maturing from late childhood into early adulthood. It is likely that convergence across multiple methodologies will help us to understand how the social brain develops across adolescence.

FUNDING

This study was supported by the National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health Intramural Research Program and the NIH Graduate Partnership Program. S.J.B. is funded by a Royal Society University Research Fellowship.

The authors gratefully acknowledge the continued participation of all families and individuals involved in this longitudinal study. We thank A.L. Goddings, M. Lombardo, R. Mars and B. Dickerson for valuable discussion concerning the definition of the temporoparietal junction for this project; and the Athinoula Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging for providing software used for the analysis.

REFERENCES

Amodio
DM
Frith
CD
Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
2006
, vol. 
7
 (pg. 
268
-
77
)
Blakemore
SJ
The social brain in adolescence
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
2008
, vol. 
9
 (pg. 
267
-
77
)
Blakemore
SJ
The developing social brain: implications for education
Neuron
2010
, vol. 
65
 (pg. 
744
-
7
)
Blakemore
SJ
Development of the social brain in adolescence
Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine
2012
, vol. 
105
 (pg. 
111
-
6
)
Blakemore
SJ
den Ouden
H
Choudhury
S
Frith
C
Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
2007
, vol. 
2
 (pg. 
130
-
9
)
Brain Development Cooperative Group (2012)
Total and regional brain volumes in a population-based normative sample from 4 to 18 years: the NIH MRI Study of Normal Brain Development
Cerebral Cortex
2012
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
1
-
12
)
Bramen
JE
Hranilovich
JA
Dahl
RE
, et al. 
Puberty influences medial temporal lobe and cortical gray matter maturation differently in boys than girls matched for sexual maturity
Cerebral Cortex
2011
, vol. 
21
 (pg. 
636
-
46
)
Burnett
S
Bird
G
Moll
J
Frith
C
Blakemore
SJ
Development during adolescence of the neural processing of social emotion
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
2009
, vol. 
21
 (pg. 
1736
-
50
)
Burnett
S
Sebastian
C
Cohen Kadosh
K
Blakemore
SJ
The social brain in adolescence: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
2011
, vol. 
35
 (pg. 
1654
-
64
)
Chenn
A
Walsh
CA
Regulation of cerebral cortical size by control of cell cycle exit in neural precursors
Science
2002
, vol. 
297
 (pg. 
365
-
9
)
Cohen Kodosh
K
Johnson
MA
Dick
F
Cohen Kadosh
R
Blakemore
SJ
Effects of age, task performance and structural brain development on face processing
Cerebral Cortex
2012
Dale
AM
Sereno
MI
Improved localization of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: a linear approach
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
1993
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
162
-
76
)
Dale
AM
Fischl
B
Sereno
MI
Cortical surface-based analysis I: segmentation and surface reconstruction
NeuroImage
1999
, vol. 
9
 (pg. 
179
-
94
)
Denckla
MB
Revised neurological examination for subtle signs
Psychopharmacology Bulletin
1985
, vol. 
21
 (pg. 
773
-
800
)
Desikan
RS
Ségonne
F
Fischl
B
, et al. 
An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest
NeuroImage
2006
, vol. 
31
 (pg. 
968
-
80
)
Dumontheil
I
Apperly
IA
Blakemore
SJ
Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence
Developmental Science
2010a
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
331
-
8
)
Dumontheil
I
Hassan
B
Gilbert
SJ
Blakemore
SJ
Development of the selection and manipulation of self-generated thoughts in adolescence
Journal of Neuroscience
2010b
, vol. 
30
 (pg. 
7664
-
71
)
Fair
DA
Bathula
D
Mills
KL
, et al. 
Maturing thalamocortical functional connectivity across development
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience
2010
, vol. 
4
 pg. 
10
 
Fair
DA
Cohen
AL
Dosenbach
NU
, et al. 
The maturing architecture of the brain's default network
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
2008
, vol. 
105
 (pg. 
4028
-
32
)
Fair
DA
Cohen
AL
Power
JD
, et al. 
Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization
PLoS Computational Biology
2009
, vol. 
5
 pg. 
e1000381
 
Fair
DA
Dosenbach
NU
Church
JA
, et al. 
Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
2007
, vol. 
104
 (pg. 
13507
-
12
)
Fischl
B
Dale
AM
Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
2000
, vol. 
97
 (pg. 
11050
-
5
)
Fischl
B
Sereno
MI
Dale
AM
Cortical surface-based analysis II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system
NeuroImage
1999a
, vol. 
9
 (pg. 
195
-
207
)
Fischl
B
Sereno
MI
Tootell
RB
Dale
AM
High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface
Human Brain Mapping
1999b
, vol. 
8
 (pg. 
272
-
84
)
Fischl
B
Liu
A
Dale
AM
Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex
IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging
2001
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
70
-
80
)
Fischl
B
van der Kouwe
A
Destrieux
C
, et al. 
Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex
Cerebral Cortex
2004
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
11
-
22
)
Fish
JL
Dehay
C
Kennedy
H
Huttner
WB
Making bigger brains—the evolution of neural-progenitor-cell division
Journal of Cell Science
2008
, vol. 
121
 (pg. 
2783
-
93
)
Fjell
AM
Walhovd
KB
Westlye
LT
, et al. 
When does brain aging accelerate? Dangers of quadratic fits in cross-sectional studies
NeuroImage
2010
, vol. 
50
 (pg. 
1376
-
83
)
Frith
CD
The social brain?
Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences
2007
, vol. 
362
 (pg. 
671
-
8
)
Frith
U
Frith
CD
Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing
Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences
2003
, vol. 
358
 (pg. 
459
-
73
)
Giedd
JN
Snell
JW
Lange
N
, et al. 
Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4-18
Cerebral Cortex
1996
, vol. 
6
 (pg. 
551
-
60
)
Giedd
JN
Blumenthal
J
Jeffries
NO
, et al. 
Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study
Nature Neuroscience
1999
, vol. 
2
 (pg. 
861
-
3
)
Gilbert
SJ
Spengler
S
Simons
JS
, et al. 
Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): a meta-analysis
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
2006
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
932
-
48
)
Goddings
AL
Burnett Heyes
S
Bird
G
Viner
RM
Blakemore
SJ
The relationship between puberty and social emotion processing
Developmental Science
2012
Hill
J
Inder
T
Neil
J
Dierker
D
Harwell
J
Van Essen
D
Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America
2010
, vol. 
107
 (pg. 
13135
-
40
)
Huttenlocher
PR
Dabholkar
AS
Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex
Journal of Comparative Neurology
1997
, vol. 
387
 (pg. 
167
-
78
)
Hollingshead
AB
Four-Factor Index for Social Status
1975
New Haven
Yale University Press
Lebel
C
Walker
L
Leemans
A
Phillips
L
Beaulieu
C
Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood
NeuroImage
2008
, vol. 
40
 (pg. 
1044
-
55
)
Kovács
ÁM
Téglás
E
Endress
AD
The social sense: susceptibility to others' beliefs in human infants and adults
Science
2010
, vol. 
330
 (pg. 
1830
-
4
)
Lu
LH
Dapretto
M
O'Hare
ED
, et al. 
Relationships between brain activation and brain structure in normally developing children
Cerebral Cortex
2009
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
2595
-
604
)
Mars
RB
Sallet
J
Schüffelgen
U
Jbabdi
S
Toni
I
Rushworth
MF
Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right “Temporoparietal Junction Area”: evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks
Cerebral Cortex
2012
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
1894
-
903
)
Neufang
S
Specht
K
Hausmann
M
, et al. 
Sex differences and the impact of steroid hormones on the developing human brain
Cerebral Cortex
2009
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
464
-
73
)
Olson
IR
Plotzker
A
Ezzyat
Y
The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing
Brain
2007
, vol. 
130
 (pg. 
1718
-
31
)
Ostby
Y
Tamnes
CK
Fjell
AM
Westlye
LT
Due-Tønnessen
P
Walhovd
KB
Heterogeneity in subcortical brain development: a structural magnetic resonance imaging study of brain maturation from 8 to 30 years
Journal of Neuroscience
2009
, vol. 
29
 (pg. 
11772
-
82
)
Panizzon
MS
Fennema-Notestine
C
Eyler
LT
, et al. 
Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness
Cerebral Cortex
2009
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
2728
-
35
)
Paus
T
Keshavan
M
Giedd
JN
Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence?
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
2008
, vol. 
9
 (pg. 
947
-
57
)
Pelphrey
KA
Morris
JP
McCarthy
G
Grasping the intentions of others: the perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
2004
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
1706
-
16
)
Peper
JS
Brouwer
RM
Schnack
HG
, et al. 
Sex steroids and brain structure in pubertal boys and girls
Psychoneuroendocrinology
2009
, vol. 
34
 (pg. 
332
-
42
)
Petanjek
Z
Judaš
M
Šimic
G
, et al. 
Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
2011
, vol. 
108
 (pg. 
13281
-
6
)
Pfeifer
JH
Lieberman
MD
Dapretto
M
“I know you are but what am I?!”: neural bases of self- and social knowledge retrieval in children and adults
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
2007
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
1323
-
37
)
Pfeifer
JH
Masten
CL
Borofsky
LA
Dapretto
M
Fuligni
AJ
Lieberman
MD
Neural correlates of direct and reflected self-appraisals in adolescents and adults: when social perspective-taking informs self-perception
Child Development
2009
, vol. 
80
 (pg. 
1016
-
38
)
Pinheiro
JC
Bates
DM
Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS
2000
New York
Springer
Puce
A
Perrett
D
Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion
Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences
2003
, vol. 
358
 (pg. 
435
-
45
)
Rakic
P
A small step for the cell, a giant leap for mankind: a hypothesis of neocortical expansion during evolution
Trends in Neuroscience
1995
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
383
-
8
)
Raznahan
A
Shaw
P
Lalonde
F
, et al. 
How does your cortex grow?
Journal of Neuroscience
2011
, vol. 
31
 (pg. 
7174
-
7
)
Reuter
M
Rosas
HD
Fischl
B
Highly accurate inverse consistent registration: a robust approach
NeuroImage
2010
, vol. 
53
 (pg. 
1181
-
96
)
Roth
G
Dicke
U
Evolution of the brain and intelligence
Trends in Cognitive Science
2005
, vol. 
9
 (pg. 
250
-
7
)
Saxe
R
Kanwisher
N
People thinking about thinking people
The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. NeuroImage
2003
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
1835
-
42
)
Saxe
RR
Whitfield-Gabrieli
S
Scholz
J
Pelphrey
KA
Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children
Child Development
2009
, vol. 
80
 (pg. 
1197
-
209
)
Sebastian
CL
Tan
GC
Roiser
JP
Viding
E
Dumontheil
I
Blakemore
SJ
Developmental influences on the neural bases of responses to social rejection: implications of social neuroscience for education
NeuroImage
2011
, vol. 
57
 (pg. 
686
-
94
)
Segonne
F
Dale
AM
Busa
E
, et al. 
A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI
NeuroImage
2004
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
1060
-
75
)
Segonne
F
Pacheco
J
Fischl
B
Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops
IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging
2007
, vol. 
26
 (pg. 
518
-
29
)
Shaw
P
Kabani
NJ
Lerch
JP
, et al. 
Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex
Journal of Neuroscience
2008
, vol. 
28
 
14
(pg. 
3586
-
94
)
Shaw
P
Malek
M
Watson
B
Sharp
W
Evans
A
Greenstein
D
Development of cortical surface area and gyrification in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
Biological Psychiatry
2012
, vol. 
72
 
3
(pg. 
191
-
7
)
Sled
JG
Zijdenbos
AP
Evans
AC
A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data
IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging
1998
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
87
-
97
)
Sowell
ER
Peterson
BS
Thompson
PM
Welcome
SE
Henkenius
AL
Toga
AW
Mapping cortical change across the human life span
Nature Neuroscience
2003
, vol. 
6
 (pg. 
309
-
15
)
Sowell
ER
Thompson
PM
Leonard
CM
Welcome
SE
Kan
E
Toga
AW
Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children
Journal of Neuroscience
2004
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
8223
-
31
)
Striano
T
Reid
VM
Social cognition in the first year
Trends in Cognitive Science
2006
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
471
-
6
)
Tamnes
CK
Ostby
Y
Fjell
AM
Westlye
LT
Due-Tønnessen
P
Walhovd
KB
Brain maturation in adolescence and young adulthood: regional age-related changes in cortical thickness and white matter volume and microstructure
Cerebral Cortex
2010
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
534
-
48
)
Taylor
SJ
Whincup
PH
Hindmarsh
PC
Lampe
F
Odoki
K
Cook
DG
Performance of a new pubertal self-assessment questionnaire: a preliminary study
Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology
2001
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
88
-
94
)
Van Essen
DC
A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex
NeuroImage
2005
, vol. 
28
 (pg. 
635
-
62
)
van Soelen
IL
Brouwer
RM
van Baal
GC
, et al. 
Genetic influences on thinning of the cerebral cortex during development
NeuroImage
2012
, vol. 
59
 (pg. 
3871
-
80
)
Wang
AT
Lee
SS
Sigman
M
Dapretto
M
Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent
Social Cognitive Affective Neuroscience
2006
, vol. 
1
 (pg. 
107
-
21
)
Webb
SJ
Monk
CS
Nelson
CA
Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological development: implications for human development
Developmental Neuropsychology
2001
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
147
-
71
)
Wimmer
H
Perner
J
Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception
Cognition
1983
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
103
-
28
)
Winkler
AM
Kochunov
P
Blangero
J
, et al. 
Cortical thickness or grey matter volume? The importance of selecting the phenotype for imaging genetics studies
NeuroImage
2010
, vol. 
53
 (pg. 
1135
-
46
)
Yakovlev
PA
Lecours
IR
Minkowski
A
The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain
Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life
1967
Oxford
Blackwell
(pg. 
3
-
70
)

Supplementary data