Semin Speech Lang 2013; 34(04): 260-266
DOI: 10.1055/s-0033-1353444
Thieme Medical Publishers 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA.

Decontextualized Language Input and Preschoolers' Vocabulary Development

Meredith L. Rowe
1   Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland
› Author Affiliations
Further Information

Publication History

Publication Date:
02 December 2013 (online)

Abstract

This article discusses the importance of using decontextualized language, or language that is removed from the here and now including pretend, narrative, and explanatory talk, with preschool children. The literature on parents' use of decontextualized language is reviewed and results of a longitudinal study of parent decontextualized language input in relation to child vocabulary development are explained. The main findings are that parents who provide their preschool children with more explanations and narrative utterances about past or future events in the input have children with larger vocabularies 1 year later, even with quantity of parent input and child prior vocabulary skill controlled. Recommendations for how to engage children in decontextualized language conversations are provided.

 
  • References

  • 1 Huttenlocher J, Haight W, Bryk A, Seltzer M, Lyons T. Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Dev Psychol 1991; 27: 236-248
  • 2 Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing; 1995
  • 3 Hoff E. The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev 2003; 74: 1368-1378
  • 4 Snow CE, Tabors PO, Dickinson DK. Language development in the preschool years. In: Dickinson DK, Tabors PO, , eds. Beginning Literacy with Language: Young Children Learning at Home and School. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing; 2001: 1-25
  • 5 Dickinson DK, Tabors PO , Eds. Beginning Literacy with Language: Young Children Learning at Home and School. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing; 2001
  • 6 Cummins J. Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Rev Educ Res 1979; 49: 222-251
  • 7 Curenton SM, Justice LM. African American and Caucasian preschoolers' use of decontextualized language: literate language features in oral narratives. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2004; 35: 240-253
  • 8 Katz JR. Playing at home: the talk of pretend play. In: Dickinson DK, Tabors PO, , eds. Beginning Literacy with Language: Young Children Learning at Home and School. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing; 2001: 53-73
  • 9 Beals D. Eating and reading: links between family conversations with preschoolers and later language and literacy. In: Dickinson DK, Tabors PO, , eds. Beginning Literacy with Language: Young Children Learning at Home and School. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing; 2001: 75-92
  • 10 Tabors PO, Roach KA, Snow CE. Home language and literacy environment: final results. In: Dickinson DK, Tabors PO, , eds. Beginning Literacy with Language: Young Children Learning at Home and School. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing; 2001: 111-138
  • 11 DeTemple JM. Parents and children reading book together. In: Dickinson DK, Tabors PO, , eds. Beginning Literacy with Language: Young Children Learning at Home and School. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing; 2001: 31-51
  • 12 Curenton SM, Craig MJ, Flanigan N. Use of decontextualized talk across story contexts: how oral storytelling and emergent reading can scaffold children's development. Early Educ Dev 2008; 19: 161-187
  • 13 Reese E, Leyva D, Sparks A, Grolnick W. Maternal elaborative reminiscing increases low-income children's narrative skills relative to dialogic reading. Early Educ Dev 2010; 21: 318-342
  • 14 Anderson RC, Freebody P. Vocabulary knowledge. In: Guthrie JT, , ed. Reading Comprehension and Education. Newark, DE: International Reading Association; 1981: 77-117
  • 15 Cunningham AE, Stanovich KE. Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Dev Psychol 1997; 33: 934-945
  • 16 Rowe ML. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Dev 2012; 83: 1762-1774
  • 17 Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 3rd ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1997
  • 18 Fivush R, Fromhoff FA. Style and structure in mother-child conversations about the past. Discourse Process 1988; 11: 337-355
  • 19 Haden CA, Haine RA, Fivush R. Developing narrative structure in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool years. Dev Psychol 1997; 33: 295-307
  • 20 McCabe A, Peterson C. Getting the story: a longitudinal study of parental styles in eliciting oral personal narratives and developing narrative skill. In: McCabe A, Peterson C, , eds. Developing Narrative Structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1991: 217-253
  • 21 Peterson C, McCabe A. A social interactionist account of developing decontextualized narrative skill. Dev Psychol 1994; 30: 937-948
  • 22 Snow CE, Dickinson DK. Social sources of narrative skills at home and at school. First Lang 1990; 10: 87-103
  • 23 Bruner JS. Children's Talk: Learning to Use Language. New York, NY: Norton; 1983
  • 24 Vygotsky LS. Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1979
  • 25 Snow CE. The theoretical basis for relationships between language and literacy in development. J Res Child Educ 1991; 6: 5-10
  • 26 Snow CE, Burns MS, Griffin P , Eds. Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 1998