INTRODUCTION

Childhood sexual or physical abuse is associated with increased rates of a variety of psychiatric disorders, including post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, alcohol and substance abuse, dissociative disorders, and borderline personality disorder (McCauley et al, 1997; Beitchman et al, 1992; Brown and Anderson, 1991; Draijer and Langeland, 1999). A recent survey in the US reported that childhood sexual abuse is the most common cause of PTSD in women, currently affecting 10% of all women (Kessler et al, 1995). There is a growing body of evidence in animals demonstrating that early adverse events, such as maternal deprivation, may have a lasting impact on the neurobiology of the stress response, involving chronic increases in plasma glucocorticoid levels and a potentiation of glucocorticoid responsiveness to subsequent stressors (Sapolsky, 1997; Plotsky and Meany, 1993; Anisman et al, 1998). In humans, trauma early in life appears to increase the risk of developing PTSD in adulthood in response to other traumas, suggesting that early trauma reduces the capacity to cope with subsequent stressful events (Bremner et al, 1993). In spite of the high prevalence of childhood abuse and PTSD, little is known about the long-term effects of abuse on the regulation of stress.

Two biological stress systems, that is, the locus coeruleus/noradrenergic system and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis are likely to mediate the association between early life stress and PTSD. Long-term alterations in these stress systems, both at baseline (‘tonic’) and with exposure to trauma cues (‘phasic’), have been documented in PTSD subjects (Bremner et al, 1999). Exposure of patients with PTSD to scripts of the individual's traumatic experiences have resulted in an increase in sympathetic responses (eg heart rate, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, skin conductance, and (nor)epinephrine) in comparison to traumatized persons without PTSD (Pitman et al, 1990, 1987; Liberzon et al, 1999; Orr et al, 1993; Shalev et al, 1993).

Unlike the increased sensitivity of the locus coeruleus/noradrenergic system, baseline and challenge studies show that the HPA-axis functions in a more complicated manner. Shortly after a traumatic event (ie the ‘phasic’ responses), cortisol secretion is generally enhanced, as was found in soldiers being shelled (Howard et al, 1955), and in women shortly after being raped (Resnick et al, 1995; Yehuda et al, 1998). Paradoxically, basal 24-h cortisol excretion has been reported to be lower in patients with PTSD compared to controls (Mason et al, 1986; Yehuda et al, 1990, 1995b, 1996). This may be due to enhanced adrenocortical sensitivity to feedback regulation, given reports of enhanced dexamethasone suppressibility in PTSD (Yehuda et al, 1993; Stein et al, 1997) or decreased responsiveness of the adrenal cortex (Kanter et al, 2001). Other studies have reported increased cortisol in 24-h urine volumes both in combat veterans with PTSD (Pitman and Orr, 1990), in women with PTSD secondary to childhood sexual abuse (Lemieux and Coe, 1995), and in maltreated children with PTSD (De Bellis et al, 1999). In line with these latter studies, elevated levels of CRF in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) have been found in combat-related PTSD (Bremner et al, 1997). In sum, studies examining baseline HPA-axis function in patients with PTSD have been inconsistent, with evidence both for hypo- and hypersecretion of cortisol.

As PTSD patients specifically suffer from exaggerated stress responses to trauma cues, it is particularly relevant to evaluate the reactivity of the HPA-axis to traumatic reminders. So far, studies have largely focused on baseline differences between PTSD patients and controls. One study of adult women with depression and early abuse found increased cortisol and ACTH responses to a stressful cognitive challenge (Heim et al, 2000). Recently, in a study on neuroendocrine responses to white noise and combat sounds, veterans with PTSD showed enhanced cortisol levels compared to veteran controls without PTSD and nonveteran controls (Liberzon et al, 1999). However, the timeframe of the cortisol assessment (only once before and once immediately after exposure) was inappropriate to distinguish baseline levels and cortisol reactivity to stress. Thus, it remained unclear whether elevations reflected enhanced chronic activation or anticipatory anxiety to the combat sounds and white noise. To our knowledge, no other studies have been published that directly assessed responsivity of the HPA-axis in relation to traumatic stimuli.

Recent findings shave shown that the HPA-axis plays an important role in memory functioning by its effects on the hippocampus (Bremner, 1999; Elzinga and Bremner, 2002; McEwen, 2000; McGaugh, 2000; Sapolsky, 1996), a brain structure involved in learning and memory (Squire and Zola-Morgan, 1991). Studies in animals show that high levels of GCs may have a long-term inhibitory effect on memory function with GC-mediated hippocampal toxicity and memory dysfunction with stress (McEwen et al, 1992; Sapolsky, 1996; Arbel et al, 1994; Conrad et al, 1996; Luine et al, 1994; Ohl et al, 2000). Studies in humans have also found declarative memory impairments in healthy subjects after administration of exogenous glucocorticoids (Keenan et al, 1995; Wolkowitz et al, 1990; Newcomer et al, 1994, 1999; Lupien et al, 1997, 1999) and with exposure to real-life stressors (Kirschbaum et al, 1996). In our lab, we found that a psychosocial laboratory stressor specifically impaired consolidation of information (Elzinga et al, submitted).

There are indications that PTSD patients are characterized by increased sensitivity of central GC receptors in the hippocampus (Yehuda et al, 1995a). If so, exposure to stress in patients with PTSD might result in greater impairments of declarative memory compared to control subjects. This might explain why patients with PTSD report a variety of memory disturbances, including deficits in new learning, fragmentation of (trauma-related) autobiographical memories, and trauma-related amnesia (for a review, see Elzinga and Bremner, 2002; Golier and Yehuda, 1998). So far, no study has directly assessed the relationship between stress-induced cortisol elevations and neutral and emotional memory functioning in PTSD.

The purpose of the present study was to assess cortisol levels following exposure to personalized trauma scripts in women with current PTSD related to childhood abuse and women with a history of abuse without current PTSD. We hypothesized higher cortisol levels following exposure to trauma scripts in PTSD patients relative to controls. To investigate the relationship between cortisol levels during encoding and memory performance, neutral and emotional words and paragraphs were presented immediately after exposure to the trauma scripts to assess stress-related memory functioning and 3 days later to assess delayed recall of the information and baseline memory functioning.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Subjects

A total of 24 women with a history of severe sexual and/or physical abuse participated in this study. Subjects included women with (N=12) and without (N=12) current PTSD related to childhood abuse (for subject characteristics, see Table 1). All subjects had lifetime PTSD. Subjects were recruited by advertisements in newspapers and flyers, and were paid for participation in the study. They were free of clinically significant medical or neurological illnesses. Subjects were excluded if they presented with a history of current alcohol or substance abuse or dependence in the past 6 months, organic mental disorders, or comorbid psychotic disorders based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (SCID-I; First et al, 1995). Of the 12 control subjects, five were taking psychotropic medication during participation in the study (two subjects were using 20 mg paroxetine, one was using 40 mg nefazadone, one was using 250 mg sertraline, and one was using 300 mg bupropion), and four of 12 PTSD subjects (two were using 10 mg paroxetine, one was using 20 mg paroxetine, and one was using 40 mg citalopram). Subjects using benzodiazepines were excluded from the study because of the known negative effects on memory functioning. After an introduction to the study, all subjects gave written informed consent for their participation in a protocol approved by the Human Investigation Committee of Yale University. The procedure was in accord with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975.

Table 1 Subject Characteristics

History of childhood abuse was assessed with the Early Trauma Inventory—self-report version (ETI; Bremner et al, 2000), a reliable and valid inventory that assesses physical, emotional, and sexual abuse, as well as general traumatic events (Bremner et al, 2000). Subjects were included with a history of childhood physical or sexual abuse, defined as rape, attempted rape, molestation, physical assault or attack with injury, before the age of 18 years. Axis I diagnoses were assessed with the SCID-I disorders (First et al, 1995, see Table 1). Current and lifetime PTSD was assessed with the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-IV (CAPS-DX; Blake et al, 1997), a reliable and valid measure of PTSD symptoms with subcomponents for the individual symptom clusters. PTSD subjects had to fulfill the DSM-IV criteria for PTSD and have a total CAPS score of 50 or higher. Subjects with CAPS scores lower than 50 and who did not fulfill the DSM-IV criteria were included as control subjects. Baseline dissociative traits were assessed by the Dissociative Experience Scale (DES; Bernstein and Putnam, 1986). Depressive mood was assessed using the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD; Hamilton, 1960).

Scripts

Prior to the challenge day, the subject prepared two personalized scripts with the assistance of an interviewer, using a prior published method (Pitman et al, 1987; Bremner et al, 1999). The participant was first asked to describe a severe childhood sexual or physical abuse event on a script preparation form (trauma script 1). The participant's response was reviewed and a script approximately 1 min in length was composed by the interviewer portraying the experience in the first person, present tense. Then the participant was asked to describe a second traumatic situation, that is, a situation in which the person felt left alone or abandoned (trauma script 2). Again, a script was composed of about 1 min in length describing the event in the first person, present tense. As cortisol responses only arise after approximately 20 min starting from the onset of the stressful situation, the contribution of each individual script on cortisol response cannot be specified. Specific effects of the two scripts on the heart rate, blood pressure, and skin conductance are reported elsewhere (Schmahl et al, submitted).

The subjective impact of the personal trauma scripts was assessed after each script on a 17-item PTSD symptom scale (Southwick et al, 1993); a Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale (CADSS, Bremner et al, 1998), a reliable and valid 27-item scale for the measurement of current dissociative states; a Subjective Units of Distress Scale (a visual analog scale ranging from 0 to 100) for the assessment of current subjective levels of distress; and a Visual Analog Scale (ranging from 0 to 100) for the assessment of anxiety.

Memory Tasks

Paragraph recall

The Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Logical Memory test (Wechsler, 1987) was used as a valid and sensitive measure of verbal declarative memory that has proved to be sensitive to GC effects in previous studies (Newcomer et al, 1994, 1999). The Wechsler memory task is a typical example of declarative memory, using encoding and retrieval strategies. Participants heard two short narratives. Recall for the 25 embedded facts was tested immediately and following a 30 min intervening period, during which other neuropsychological tests were performed. Two versions (A and B) of the Wechsler memory tests were administered, one after exposure to the trauma scripts, and one 3 days later, which served as a baseline measure. To avoid any nonrandom errors, order of presentation of the two paragraphs was counterbalanced. It was expected that due to enhanced cortisol levels, immediate and delayed recall of the narrative administered immediately after the stress challenge would be impaired compared to the immediate and delayed recall of the narrative presented under resting conditions at day 3.

To assess memory processing of relevant, emotional material, an emotional paragraph was developed (of a woman alone in her house who got assaulted by her neighbor), also containing 25 embedded facts. To study the effects of stress-induced increases in cortisol levels on encoding and retrieval, this narrative was learned and recalled after exposure to the personalized scripts. To assess the effects of cortisol on long-term consolidation, recall of the story was again assessed 3 days later.

Word recall

Participants received a list of 20 emotional, trauma-related words (eg ‘tortured’, ‘blood’). In a pilot study, these words were rated as more fearful, sad, nervous, angry, and less happy than neutral words (all P<0.0001). All words had unique word-stems. The investigator (BE) read the words out loud and subjects were instructed to rate how fearful each word was on a 5-point scale. The words were then read a second time, instructing subjects to rate how aversive the word was. To study the effects of cortisol on encoding, word recall was tested 3 days later in a surprise-cued recall task, a word-stem completion task, and a recognition task. It was expected that elevated cortisol levels would be associated with impaired performance on the explicit memory tasks.

For the cued recall task, 10 word-stems were presented corresponding to 10 trauma words instructing the subjects to fill in the word-stems with words that were previously presented. For the word-stem completion task, the 10 remaining target word-stems (not presented in the cued recall task) were presented intermixed with 10 filler word-stems that did not correspond to any previously presented words, to obscure the goal of memory testing. Subjects were instructed to fill in the first word that came to mind. In the recognition task, all 20 target words were presented intermixed with 20 new trauma words. Subjects had to rate how confident they were that they had seen the word before at day 1 using the following rating scale: 1. I'm sure that the word is new (not studied); 2. Probably new; 3. Probably old; 4. Sure that the word is old (or studied). Subjects were given 2 points each time they assigned a ‘4’ (sure that the word is old) to a target word, 1 point for each ‘3’, −1 point for each ‘2’, and −2 points for each ‘1’. The total score served as a measure of emotional recognition memory.

Gordon box distractibility task

To control for attention, distractibility was measured using a computer-generated continuous performance task that presented a rapid, continual sequence of numbers in one of three positions, right, center, or left (Gordon Diagnostic System (GDS); Gordon and Mettelman, 1987). In a first presentation, participants had to press a key every time they saw a ‘9’. In a second presentation, participants were required to respond by pressing a key to each ‘9’ that appeared immediately after a ‘1’, but only if they both appeared sequentially in the center position. The total number of correct responses and reaction latencies were analyzed.

Cortisol

Cortisol levels were monitored using saliva sampling to prevent the stress-inducing effects of blood sampling. Determination of cortisol in saliva provides a reliable measure of the free unbound fraction of cortisol (Tunn et al, 1992). Saliva samples were collected at 14 time-points before, during, and after the trauma scripts exposure. Pre-exposure levels of cortisol were assessed at 20, 10, 5, and 0 min before exposure to the scripts; cortisol levels during trauma exposure were assessed at 5, 10, 15, and 20 min after exposure to the first trauma script, and cortisol levels during recovery were assessed at 25, 30, 40, 50, 60, and 75 min after exposure to the first trauma script (see Figure 1). Saliva samples were collected using Salivette collection devices and stored at −70°C. Salivette tubes were centrifuged (0–4°C) to prepare saliva, which was analyzed for cortisol using a 125I immunoradiometric assay kit available from Diagnostic Products Corporation (Los Angeles, CA). Samples and standards (200 μl) were determined in duplicate. Day-to-day coefficients for low (389 pg/ml) and high (4.12 ng/ml) concentration quality assessment (QC) samples were 10.1 and 8.4%, respectively.

Figure 1
figure 1

Cortisol levels (μg/dl) and standard error of means (SEMs) before, during, and after trauma script exposure for women with and without PTSD. Women with PTSD had elevated levels of cortisol during and shortly after the script exposure compared to controls (t0–t25).

Procedure

The study was conducted in the afternoon (between 1400 and 1700 hoursp.m.) in order to have a standard time-point relative to normal diurnal cortisol variations. Participants had to refrain from strenuous physical exercise, large meals, cigarette smoking, and coffee for at least 1 h prior to the experiment, because of the known effects of these variables on cortisol secretion. Subjects were placed in a quiet room in a reclining chair. After a resting period of 20 min, a standardized neutral story 1 min in length was read by a female investigator. Subjects were then asked to imagine the story for 2 min. After the imagination period, subjective ratings of PTSD symptoms, dissociation, anxiety, and general distress were collected. After 30 min, the first personal trauma script was read out loud, followed by an imagination period of 2 min in which subjects were asked to imagine the script as vividly as possible. After this, the subjective ratings were assessed. This procedure was repeated after 10 min with the presentation of the second trauma script. The order of the two scripts was randomized. After exposure to the trauma scripts, subjects had a resting period of 5 min after which the memory assessment took place in the same fixed order: Wechsler paragraph (A) immediate recall; rating of emotional words; learning and immediate recall of the emotional story; Wechsler paragraph (A) delayed recall; and two distractibility tasks. The complete procedure took about 1.5 h.

At 3 days after the script exposure, subjects came back for a 30-min memory assessment. In a same fixed order, the following memory tests were administered: Wechsler paragraph (B) immediate recall (baseline); word-stem completion, cued recall and recognition task of the emotional words; delayed paragraph recall of emotional story; and Wechsler paragraph (B) delayed recall (baseline).

STATISTICS

Cortisol levels during the experiment were calculated using repeated measures design with the 14 separate time-points as the within-subject factor and the PTSD vs control group as the between-subject factor. Differences in cortisol levels at separate time-points between the two groups were calculated using t-tests for independent samples (two-tailed). Memory performance between the two groups was calculated using t-tests for paired samples (two-tailed). Paragraph recall before and after stress exposure was analyzed using a 2 (immediate vs delayed recall) by 2 (stress vs baseline) repeated measures design followed by post hoc t-tests. Subjective measures were calculated using a repeated measures design with the two diagnostic groups as the between-subject factor and the three scripts (neutral, trauma script 1, and trauma script 2) as the within-subject factor, followed by post hoc t-tests. Correlations between cortisol levels and CAPS scores, and between cortisol levels and memory performance were calculated using Pearson's correlations.

RESULTS

Cortisol Levels in Response to Trauma Scripts

Abused women with PTSD demonstrated higher cortisol levels compared to controls (main effect for diagnosis: F[1,22]=4.08, P=0.056, Figure 1). Cortisol levels were elevated by an average of 122% in PTSD patients compared to controls during script exposure (mean of time-points 5, 10, 15, and 20 min; t(22)=2.82, P<0.01), vs 60% during the pre-exposure period (mean of time-points −20, −10, −5, and 0 min; t(22)=1.51, NS), and 69% during the postexposure period (mean of time-points 25, 30, 40, 50, 60, and 75 min: t(22)=1.66, NS). Comparisons at separate time-points revealed that women with PTSD had significantly higher cortisol levels than controls during and shortly after the script exposure (time-points t0–t25): tt0(22)=2.17, P<0.05; tt5(22)=2.19, P<0.05; tt10(22)=2.28, P<0.05; tt15(22)=2.83, P<0.01; tt20(22)=3.19, P<0.01; and tt25(22)=2.23, P<0.05, all two-tailed (see asterixes in Figure 1).

For the group as a whole, cortisol levels were lower during the postexposure period (from 25 min onwards) than before and during script exposure (ie significant main effect for time: F[13,299]=6.00, P<0.001). This main effect of time was specified by a significant group by time interaction (F[13,286]=1.95, P<0.05): Abused women with PTSD exhibited a strong decrease in cortisol after the script exposure, whereas controls did not.

To examine the effect of depression on cortisol levels, PTSD patients with (n=3) and without (n=9) a comorbid depression were compared. No differences were detected (mean cortisol of women without depression=0.082±0.052 μg/dl vs mean cortisol of women with depression=0.075±0.014 μg/dl). To assess the effects of medication on cortisol levels, mean cortisol levels of medicated subjects with (n=4) and without PTSD (n=5) were compared to cortisol levels of unmedicated subjects with (n=8) and without PTSD (n=7). In both groups, mean cortisol levels were somewhat lower in medicated subjects (PTSD=0.071±0.008 μg/dl and controls=0.016± 0.004 μg/dl) relative to unmediated subjects (PTSD= 0.086±0.056 μg/dl and controls=0.065±0.043 μg/dl). This effect appeared to be stronger in the control group, but sample sizes were too small to allow statistical analyses.

Correlations Between Cortisol and PTSD Symptomatology

Correlations between cortisol levels and severity of PTSD symptoms as assessed with the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) during the diagnostic assessment (indicating the severity of post-traumatic symptoms during the past 2 weeks) were high during and shortly after exposure to the trauma scripts, but moderate under resting conditions (both pre- and postexposure) (see Figure 2, correlations higher than±0.40 are significant at the 0.05 level). Re-experiencing symptoms (subscale B) and arousal symptoms (subscale D) were especially correlated with cortisol release, whereas avoidance behavior (subscale C) yielded lower correlations.

Figure 2
figure 2

Mean Pearson's correlations between cortisol levels at the separate time-points during the study and baseline PTSD symptomatology as measured with the CAPS during diagnostic assessment. Note: Correlations higher than±0.40 are significant at the 0.05 level.

Subjective Measures

Abused women with PTSD reported more PTSD symptoms, dissociative symptoms, and anxiety to the trauma scripts than abused women without PTSD (Table 2). For the sample as a whole, exposure to the trauma scripts resulted in elevated PTSD symptoms (F[2,44]=36.41, P<0.001), dissociative symptoms (F[2,44]=7.64, P<0.001), anxiety (F[2,44]=11.76, P<0.001), and subjective distress (F[2,44]=24.38, P<0.001) compared to the neutral script. No interaction was found between the script and group.

Table 2 Mean (±SD) Psychological Ratings for Women with and without PTSD After Listening to a Neutral Script, and the Two Trauma Scripts Describing Their Childhood Abuse

Memory

Compared to the abused women without current PTSD, abused women with PTSD tended to recall less emotional words 3 days after the challenge day, and recognized new words less well than words not previously studied (see Table 3). With Bonferroni correction, these differences were no longer significant. No differences were found between the two groups on (neutral or emotional) paragraph recall at the day of the challenge or at baseline.

Table 3 Memory Data of Women with and without PTSD

In a repeated-measures ANOVA on the immediate vs delayed recall of the (neutral) paragraphs after exposure vs baseline, the average recall after the trauma script exposure was impaired relative to the average recall during baseline (F[1,21]=49.41, P<0.001). The expected main effect of time was also found (F[1,21]=6.81, P<0.05), indicating that delayed paragraph recall was decreased compared to immediate recall. Interestingly, these main effects were specified by a significant interaction between time and testing condition (F[1,21]=8.41, P<0.01). A post hoc analysis revealed that delayed recall was particularly decreased after the script exposure (delayed paragraph recall after stress exposure=12.46±3.84 units vs delayed paragraph recall during baseline=16.57±4.30 units, t(22)=−3.39, P<0.01), whereas immediate recall was not (immediate recall after stress exposure=15.20±4.05 units vs immediate recall during baseline=17.18±4.20 units, t(22)=−1.53, NS).

Correlations with Cortisol

A positive correlation was found between cortisol levels during encoding (t30) and recall of the emotional paragraph at day 3 (r=0.48, P<0.05). No other significant correlations were found between cortisol levels and recall of the neutral or emotional paragraphs at day 3 or with any other measure of memory function immediately after the stress exposure.

DISCUSSION

Women with PTSD demonstrated significantly elevated cortisol levels compared to abused women without PTSD when exposed to personalized scripts of childhood abuse, with the greatest magnitude of elevations during and shortly after exposure to the trauma scripts. During recovery, cortisol levels in the PTSD group dropped significantly to a level similar to the non-PTSD group. Taken together, these findings are consistent with findings of increased negative feedback sensitivity (Yehuda et al, 1993; Stein et al, 1997). In conjunction with prior studies showing decreased or normal cortisol levels at baseline, these findings may suggest increased cortisol levels following exposure to stress with suppressed baseline activation.

During and shortly after exposure to the trauma scripts, cortisol levels were strongly associated with PTSD symptoms as assessed with the CAPS. Thus, the severity of PTSD symptoms experienced during the 2 weeks prior to the study was highly predictive of cortisol levels with exposure to the trauma scripts. This was particularly true for re-experiencing and arousal symptoms, and less so for avoidance (that measures behavior rather than an ‘internal state’). It was striking that both before and after exposure to the trauma scripts, correlations between cortisol and CAPS scales dropped to nonsignificant levels, indicating that PTSD symptoms are not a good predictor of cortisol levels under resting conditions.

This pattern of results may provide new insights into the mixed findings on baseline HPA-axis functioning in PTSD, with several studies reporting lowered (Mason et al, 1986; Yehuda et al, 1990, 1996), and others reporting elevated levels of cortisol measured in 24-h urine in patients with PTSD (Pitman and Orr, 1990; Lemieux and Coe, 1995; De Bellis et al, 1999). Rather than a consistent pattern of static ‘hypocortisolism’ or ‘hypercortisolism’, (internal or external) confrontations with traumatic reminders or other stressful conditions may determine levels of cortisol, with decreases in cortisol levels being related to suppression of cortisol function to compensate for periods of increased cortisol levels during stress. This is supported by the finding of marked lability of urinary free cortisol levels at admission, midcourse, and discharge during a 90-day hospitalization period in male Vietnam combat veterans with PTSD (Mason et al, 2002).

Women with PTSD, and control subjects to a lesser extent, had higher cortisol levels before being exposed to the trauma scripts than during the period following the scripts exposure. This probably reflects heightened HPA activity due to anticipatory anxiety, as the women were informed about the procedure, and all were anxious in anticipation of it. Previous studies have demonstrated that anticipation of a stressful event may be a potent activator of HPA activity, with HPA activation often starting even before an acute stressor is applied (Kirschbaum et al, 1993; Richter et al, 1996). Moreover, elevated levels of cortisol were also found in PTSD patients in anticipation of a cognitive stress challenge (Bremner et al, 2003b). Owing to this lack of a proper baseline, it could not be determined with certainty whether cortisol levels had increased in PTSD patients in response to the trauma scripts (starting already prior to the experiment). The significant drop in cortisol levels during the recovery phase may suggest that this was the case. Moreover, a large part of the present sample took part in a study on the assessment of the HPA-axis over a 24-h diurnal period and in response to neuroendocrine challenges, in which lower levels of cortisol were found for the PTSD women during the afternoon hours (12–8 pm) compared to non-PTSD women (see Bremner et al, 2003a). Clearly, future studies on stress responsivity of the HPA-axis in PTSD should also assess proper baseline cortisol levels, measured on a separate day than the challenge.

In both PTSD patients and controls, memory consolidation was impaired following stress exposure compared to baseline measurements. However, this appeared to be independent of cortisol levels. With Bonferroni correction, no differences were found between the two groups in the recall of emotional or neutral words or paragraphs. Moreover, we did not find any of the expected negative correlations between cortisol and memory performance. One explanation for this may be related to the fact that exposure to the trauma scripts caused rather moderate elevations in cortisol levels. The peak levels of cortisol were (far) below cortisol levels associated with memory impairments in healthy subjects who have been exposed to a psychosocial stress challenge (Kirschbaum et al, 1996; Elzinga et al, submitted) or to synthetic cortisol (Lupien et al, 1999; Kirschbaum et al, 1996). Apparently, stressors with the duration and the intensity of the trauma scripts do not induce cortisol levels high enough to reduce memory functioning. It cannot be dismissed, however, that more elevated cortisol levels, elicited by exposure to more intense or aversive stressors (eg chronic stressors or highly emotional or traumatic events such as sexual or physical abuse) may be associated with decreases in memory functioning.

The only significant correlation that did arise was a positive relation between cortisol levels and recall of the emotional story, so that the higher the cortisol, the better the story was recalled. The positive correlation was inconsistent with our hypothesis based on prior studies (Newcomer et al, 1994, 1999; Kirschbaum et al, 1996). However, these studies included only neutral material. Based on animal studies, it might be hypothesized that cortisol has a differential effect on neutral vs emotional memory performance. Emotional arousal may enhance memory functioning due to a complex interaction between corticosteroids and adrenergic activity in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (McGaugh, 2000; Roozendaal, 2000). This was supported by a recent study in healthy subjects reporting enhancing effects of cortisol on the recall of emotional pictures (Buchanan and Lovallo, 2001). Clearly, more studies with various levels of both stress-induced and pharmacologically administered cortisol need to be conducted to gain more insight into the association between various cortisol levels, norepinephrine, and memory functioning related to neutral and emotional events.

Several limitations should be considered in evaluating our findings. First, the sample size was rather small with 12 women with and 12 women without current PTSD. Second, about a third of the subjects, equally distributed among the two groups, was taking psychotropic medication during the investigation. As evidence is accumulating that antidepressants may reduce reactivity of the HPA-axis, more studies are needed that investigate the effects of cortisol reactivity in nonmedicated vs medicated PTSD patients and control subjects. Moreover, all subjects, including the control subjects, had a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD. It is still unknown whether the cortisol responses of traumatized women with a history of PTSD can be generalized to individuals with a history of childhood abuse who have never developed PTSD. This would be another interesting question for future study. Furthermore, no subjects were included who had no history of sexual or physical abuse and no PTSD. It cannot be dismissed that nonabused healthy individuals may show even higher cortisol levels in response to similar stressors compared to the two abused samples. Instead of PTSD patients showing elevated cortisol levels following exposure to traumatic reminders, this would rather be indicative of a relative hyposecretion of cortisol in abused individuals without current PTSD. For a definite answer to this interesting issue, a direct comparison among these three groups would be necessary. Obviously, in such a study, participants would have to be exposed to a more general type of stressor, as a nonabused group can by definition not generate a personal trauma script as was used in the present study. Another limitation is that only cortisol levels were assessed in this study. To study HPA regulation in more detail, the separate components of the stress axis need to be measured, including ACTH. Moreover, the HPA-axis and the locus coeruleus/noradrenergic system have mutual interconnections. PTSD may be associated with dysfunctional connectivity between those systems. It would be informative, therefore, to assess these different stress systems within one study.

A question that arises from this study is whether the pattern of elevated cortisol levels in patients with PTSD when confronted with traumatic reminders will be reduced after successful treatment. As all women in the control group had a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD, this might suggest that this is indeed the case. To study this issue more directly, cortisol responses to trauma scripts both before and after either pharmacological or psychological treatment should be assessed.