INTRODUCTION

Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) such as fluoxetine and paroxetine are effective in a wide range of emotional disorders, including depression, panic disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder and social phobia (Fuller, 1995; Kent et al, 1998). The ability of SSRIs to potentiate brain serotonin neurotransmission has been well characterised in numerous studies of human and other animal species (Beasley et al, 1992; Blier and De-Montigny, 1994), but there is little understanding of the neuropsychological mechanisms by which increases in brain serotonin function could be translated into therapeutic efficacy in clinical disorders characterised by emotional and social dysfunction.

Investigations with animals and humans suggest that serotonin is involved in social behaviour. In nonhuman primates, administration of SSRIs or the serotonin precursor, l-tryptophan, facilitated the acquisition of social dominance, reduced aggression and increased the frequency of affiliative behaviours such as grooming (Raleigh et al, 1991). Similarly, in healthy human volunteers, repeated (4 week) administration of the SSRI, paroxetine increased cooperation in a problem-solving task and reduced ratings of negative affect (Knutson et al, 1998). Administration of l-tryptophan has also been reported to increase assertive behaviours over a 12-day period in healthy volunteers (Moskowitz et al, 2001). Taken together, these results suggest that serotonergic mechanisms may be involved in social affiliation and dominance.

Facial expressions are crucially important in social cognition and communication, providing information concerning the internal emotional states and intentions of other people. Ekman (1992) has described a number of basic emotions accompanied by specific facial expressions that are universally recognised throughout the human societies tested so far. Recognition of these different expressions appears to be processed by neural substrates associated with the behavioural functions of these emotions. For example, the processing of fear from facial expressions is modulated by the amygdala, which plays a wider role in both conditioned and unconditioned fear (reviewed by Davis and Whalen, 2001). Patients with bilateral lesions of this structure show a selective impairment in the recognition of fear (Adolphs et al, 1994, 1999; Broks et al, 1998), although similar impairments have been reported following neurologic damage unrestricted to the amygdala (Rapcsak et al, 2000). Neuroimaging studies in healthy volunteers reported activation of the amydgala during the processing of fear (Morris et al, 1996; Phillips et al, 1998), which correlated with the intensity of emotion presented (Morris et al, 1996). By contrast, the anterior insula and caudate have been implicated in the processing of disgust. Patients with Huntington's disease show impaired recognition of disgusted facial expressions even before the appearance of clinical symptoms, when the caudate nucleus is preferentially affected (Gray et al, 1997). Insula and basal ganglia damage has also been associated with an impairment in the recognition of facial and vocal signs of disgust (Calder et al, 2000) and, consistent with this, the anterior insula shows increased activity when viewing disgusted facial expressions (Phillips et al, 1997, 1998).

We reasoned that differentiated neuronal representations of the basic emotions might be accompanied by relatively selective modulation by different neurotransmitters. Some recent studies have described selective effects of pharmacological challenges on recognition of specific facial expressions. For example, administration of the benzodiazapine diazepam specifically decreased the recognition of anger (Blair and Curran, 1999) and fear (Zangara et al, 2002), while beta-adrenergic blockade impaired the processing of sad facial expressions (Harmer et al, 2001). Peturbutation of a particular neurochemical system may, therefore, lead to selective changes in the processing of one or a subset of emotional expressions. This might impose consequent biases in perception and interpersonal functioning. Such effects would be of relevance to the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders and also the mode of action of the pharmacological agents used to treat them.

Assessment of serotonergic function in the recognition of facial expression was of particular interest given its central role in the aetiology and treatment of mood disorder. There exists little understanding of the role of this neurotransmitter in normal emotional and social processing. Further, investigation of the early effects of antidepressant drugs on emotional processing may be pertinent to their clinical effects on mood, anxiety and social performance seen after longer term treatment. The present study, therefore, assessed the acute effects of an SSRI (citalopram) relative to placebo upon recognition of facial expression in healthy volunteers. It was predicted that citalopram administration would reduce the perception of negative facial expressions of emotion, while facilitating identification of positive facial expressions.

METHODS

Subjects

In all, 24 healthy female volunteers between the ages of 21 and 59 years took part in this study. Participants were screened to exclude those with a current or previous history of psychiatric disorder (assessed using a semistructured interview for DSM IV) or significant physical illness. All gave their written consent to participate in the study, which was approved by the local ethical committee. Volunteers were randomly allocated to receive citalopram (10 mg, i.v.) or placebo. These two groups were matched in terms of age (mean age: 40.1±3.6 and 37.3±3.7 years, respectively) and years of education (mean: 13.4±0.7; 14. 6±0.7).

Facial Expression Recognition

The facial expression recognition task featured five basic emotions—happiness, sadness, fear, anger and disgust— taken from the Ekman and Friesen (1976) Pictures of Affect Series. These had been morphed between each prototype and neutral using techniques described by Young et al (1997). Briefly, this procedure involved taking a variable percentage of the shape and texture differences between the two standard images 0% (neutral) and 100% (full emotion) in 10% steps. Four examples of each emotion at each intensity were given. Each face was also given in a neutral expression, giving a total of 210 stimuli presentations. Each face was presented for 500 ms and was immediately replaced by a blank screen. Subjects made their response by pressing a labelled key on a response box. The task was broken down into three parts, with an untimed rest period between each to prevent fatigue. Subjects were asked to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible.

Subjective State

Subjective state was recorded using visual analogue scales for the following variables: happiness, sadness, fear, disgust, anger, alertness and anxiety. The Befindlichkeits Scale (BFS: von Zerrsen et al, 1974) was also given to provide an additional measure of mood.

Procedure

Subjects attended the laboratory at midday, having fasted from breakfast, and an intravenous cannula was inserted. After a 30-min rest period, subjects received infusion of either citalopram 10 mg or placebo over 30 min in a double-blind between-groups design. Subjects completed a facial recognition task 30 min after the end of the infusion. Previous work indicates that plasma prolactin levels, which are indicative of central serotonin, are elevated from the end of the citalopram infusion for at least 2 h (Attenburrow et al, 2001). Subjective state was assessed at baseline and prior to the psychological testing.

STATISTICS

Interrelationships between subjective ratings of mood and energy were explored using principal component analysis on the scores of all volunteers at baseline. Summed ratings were analysed using two-way analysis of variance with group (citalopram or placebo) and time of rating (baseline or postinfusion) as factors.

Speed and accuracy from the facial expression recognition task were also analysed using two-way analysis of variance with group and facial expression (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, and sadness) as factors. Significant interactions were analysed further using simple main effect analyses. Analyses of covariance were used to control for subjective mood on facial expression recognition.

RESULTS

Subjective State

Exploratory factor analysis on ratings of subjective state yielded a three-factor solution, which accounted for 80% of the observed variance. These factors (from highest to lowest eigenvalue) were negative affect, positive affect and anxiety (see Table 1). There was no effect of citalopram on positive mood or anxiety (see Table 1) although there was a marginal interaction between time of rating and group in terms of negative affect (F(1,22)=4.2, p=0.05). However, pairwise comparisons at baseline (t(1,22)=0.4, p=0.7) and after infusion (t(1,22)=−0.5, p=0.6) failed to reveal significant differences between the two groups in negative mood ratings.

Table 1 Subjective State Ratings before and after Infusion of Citalopram or Placebo

Facial Expression Recognition

Compared to saline, citalopram-treated subjects were selectively more accurate at identifying facial expressions of fear and happiness in the absence of changes in recognition of the other facial expressions (see Figure 1: group×emotion interaction F(4,88)=3.0, p=0.03). Inclusion of the three subjective state ratings (post-infusion) as covariates did not abolish the significant differences between the two groups in terms of fear (F(1,19)=4.7, p<0.05) or happiness (F(1,19)=5.1, p<0.05) recognition. Hence, the enhanced recognition of these emotions appears unrelated to subjective state.

Figure 1
figure 1

Performance in the facial expression recognition task following citalopram (dark bars) or placebo (light bars). Top graph: Percentage of correct responses for each emotion. Asterisks illustrate the statistical significance of simple main effect analyses: *p<0.05. Lower graph: Reaction time of correct responses for each emotion. Simple main effect analyses revealed a significant facilitation in the speed with which fear (p<0.05) and happiness (p<0.02) were detected in the absence of changes in speed to recognise other basic emotions.

The number of emotions misclassified as fear and happiness did not differ significantly between the two groups, suggesting that increased recognition of these emotions was not the result of a nondiscriminatory change in labeling faces as these emotions (group×emotion: F(5,110)=1.6, p=0.2; happiness F(1,22)=1.5, p=0.2; fear F(1,22)=1.6, p=0.2).

Because of the possibility that these differences may represent a change in speed–accuracy trade-off, the reaction time of correct identifications was also computed. The speed to identify facial expressions of fear and happiness was selectively faster in subjects treated with citalopram relative to placebo (Figure 1: happiness F(1,22)=7.1, p=0.01; fear: F(1,22)=4.5, p<0.05; all other emotions NS), suggesting that this SSRI improved the overall efficiency with which these emotions were recognised. These group differences were not affected by inclusion of subjective ratings as covariates (fear: F(1,19)=5.8, p=0.03; happiness: F(1,19)=7.2, p=0.02).

To explore the effect of citalopram in more detail, accuracy of fear and happiness recognition was analysed over the different morphed intensity levels presented in a random order during this task (Figure 2). In both cases, citalopram shifted the recognition function to the left: lower levels of emotional intensity were recognised more consistently in the citalopram-treated subjects, while the higher levels were, as expected, at ceiling levels in both groups (group×intensity: happiness F(9,198)=2.8, p<0.01; fear F(9,198)=3.6, p<0.001). Examination of the other facial expressions over different intensity levels failed to identify any differences between citalopram- and placebo-treated subjects in this more detailed analysis, confirming a specific action on happiness and fear (main effect of group and group×intensity interactions respectively: sadness F(1,22)=0.01, p=0.9; F(9,198)=1.4, p=0.2; anger F(1,22)=1.4, p=0.3; F(9,198)=0.5, p=0.9; disgust F(1,22)=0.2, p=0.7; F(9,198)=0.8, p=0.6).

Figure 2
figure 2

Recognition of fear and happiness over the different intensity levels of facial expression used in this task. -•-, following citalopram; --, following placebo. Top graph: fear recognition. Lower graph: happiness recognition. Asterisks illustrate the statistical significance of simple main effect analyses: *p<0.05, ***p<0.001.

DISCUSSION

These data indicate that acute potentiation of central serotonin neurotransmission facilitated the recognition of facial expressions of happiness and fear. This effect was highly selective: subjects were both faster and better at detecting facial expressions of these emotions in the absence of changes in processing other emotions. These data are in line with the hypothesis that the recognition of different emotional states involves dissociable neurocognitive systems. In particular, serotonin may play a specific role in the processes involved in the recognition of happiness and fear.

The present differences in the recognition of emotional states following citalopram occurred in the absence of significant changes in subjective mood and were not affected by inclusion of mood ratings as covariates. These results suggest that shifts in the processing of emotional information can occur independently, or at a lower threshold, than overt differences in mood. As such, this model may be useful in the detection of early effects of antidepressants preceding the development of conscious mood changes.

The present facilitation of happiness recognition is consistent with evidence suggesting a role for serotonin in social behaviour. Repeated SSRI administration has been reported to increase affiliative behaviour in primates (Raleigh et al, 1991) and humans (Knutson et al, 1998). However, while the previous studies have measured affiliative behaviour, the present results suggest that enhanced perception of affiliative signals also occurs with SSRI administration. Indeed, an enhanced perception of happiness would be predicted to facilitate approach behaviour and social interaction. These changes may also be relevant to aspects of the action of SSRIs in patients with depression, who have been reported to show negative biases in the perception of social stimuli (eg Bouhuys et al, 1999).

Citalopram administration also facilitated the recognition of fear from facial expression, which was contrary to our hypothesis of reduced identification of negative emotions following SSRI administration. This result may be related to the fact that citalopram was given acutely in the present study. Indeed, we have recently found decreased recognition of fearful, disgusted and surprised facial expressions following a 7-day oral treatment with this SSRI (Harmer et al, 2002). Opposite effects of acute and repeated administration of antidepressants have also been reported in preclinical models of anxiety (Griebel et al, 1994) and, clinically, symptoms of agitation or panic can be exacerbated initially with SSRI treatment (Kent et al, 1998). Hence, the present facilitation of fear recognition may reflect enhanced processing of threatening cues in the environment following SSRI challenge.

A role for serotonin in the processing of fear-related signals has been identified both in animal and human models. In rats, serotonin levels in the amygdala increased during inescapable stress and conditioned fear (Amat et al, 1998; Kawahara et al, 1993; Inoue et al, 1993). In humans, acute administration of serotonin-receptor blockers has been reported to decrease the development of conditioned responses to an aversive loud noise as measured by subjective report and skin conductance responses (Hensman et al, 1991; Silva et al, 2001). The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear is well established (Maren, 2001); and has also been implicated in the processing of fearful facial expressions (Adolphs et al, 1994, 1999; Morris et al, 1996). It is therefore possible that serotonergic input to the amygdala may modulate the detection of fearful expressions in addition to its role in conditioned anxiety in human and animal models (see Graeff et al, 1996). It is tempting to speculate that the increased and decreased fear perception found after acute compared to repeated administration of SSRIs relates to opposing effects on neural substrates involved in fear processing such as the amygdala, and that these changes may be relevant to the therapeutic actions of SSRI in depression and anxiety, both of which have been associated with amygdala hyperarousal (Drevets et al, 1992; Sheline et al, 2001; Rauch et al, 2000).

In summary, acute administration of the SSRI citalopram facilitated the recognition of fear and happiness from facial expressions. These effects may relate to effects of serotonin on affiliative behaviour and the processing of fear-related cues. Further research is needed to assess whether these effects interact with changes in the processing of facial expressions produced by mood and anxiety disorders. Also, future studies should assess whether similar effects are apparent in male as well as female volunteers. If confirmed, these interpretations may begin to bridge the gap between narrowly defined pharmacological theories of drug action and an alternative, more integrated understanding based on the function of brain networks subserving emotional processing.