Main

Finland has one of the highest relative survival for cancer patients in Europe (Capocaccia et al, 2009). Studies in Denmark and the UK, countries with lower relative survival than in Finland, showed fairly large and consistent survival differences between educational and social groups (Shack et al, 2007; Dalton et al, 2008; Rachet et al, 2008). Increased survival does not necessarily mean the eradication of survival differences between subgroups (Coleman et al, 2008). Studies in Finland have demonstrated systematic differences by social class in cancer survival (Karjalainen and Pukkala, 1990; Auvinen, 1992; Auvinen et al, 1995; Dickman et al, 1997). It is important to determine whether these differences exist and their size, to plan actions on the basis of their likely causes from which they might have emerged. The causes for educational survival differences may be related to, e.g., behavioural characteristics, tumour stage at diagnosis or the health care system (Dalton et al, 2008).

The aim of the present study is to explore systematic social differences in cancer survival in Finland and how it has changed over time. Educational level was selected as the main variable defining the social subgroups, as it has been shown to strongly predict cancer survival in Denmark (Dalton et al, 2008) and as its definition has remained comparable over time. The impact of these differences was demonstrated by numbers and proportions of potentially avoidable deaths under the assumption of cause-specific survival in all educational groups being equal to that in the subgroup with the highest education.

Materials and methods

Patients diagnosed with cancer in 1971–2005 and registered by the countrywide Finnish Cancer Registry were linked, through the use of the unique personal identity codes with the population censuses done every 5 years in 1970–2000, to obtain information on patients’ educational levels and occupations (Pukkala et al, 2009). Persons born in 1906–80 were included in the analyses. Using the most recent antecedent information from census, the educational levels were divided into three categories according to the highest attained educational degree or certificate as follows: basic (lasting typically <10 years), secondary (10–12 years) and high education (13 years or more). Patients below 25 years of age were excluded as their educational level was not necessarily the final one.

Occupational details were used to create a supplementary group that should be particularly conscious of their health and cancer care possibilities, namely persons in medical and nursing work, teaching work, research and physical education. In comparative analyses involving in this category, such individuals were excluded from the educational categories (mainly from the high educational category) to form a fourth group. Cancers were divided into 27 site categories (Table 1). The patients were followed up from diagnosis until death, emigration or to the end of 2005, whichever came first: for a few (0.13%), the follow-up was terminated owing to emigration.

Table 1 Age-standardised 5-year cancer-specific survival proportions for the cancer patients diagnosed in Finland in 1996–2005 by site, sex and educational level (high, secondary, basic) and differences (in % units) between the high and basic categories, by site and period. Patients' site-specific age distributions in 1971–2005 have been used as site-specific standards (both genders combined)

The main outcome measure of the patients was the cancer (cause)-specific 5-year survival, where deaths owing to causes other than the cancer of the patients are counted as events censoring the follow-up time. The Cancer Registry corrects, when necessary, the official cause-of-death information, as it has more information than the person signing the death certificate (Hakulinen and Teppo, 1977). The Registry receives, on average, five notifications per cancer case, and registration is regarded as reliable both in diagnostic accuracy and completeness (Teppo et al, 1994; Curado et al, 2007).

The relative survival ratio, another measure preferred by population based cancer registries in estimating the cause-specific survival without relying on the quality of death certificates, was used for comparison (Ederer et al, 1961). Here, mortality owing to competing causes of death other than the patient's cancer is estimated as an overall mortality from the general population life tables by sex, age and calendar period. A linkage between a 20% random sample of the individuals from the censuses of 1970–2000 with the register of deaths and emigrations made it possible to also obtain general population mortality rates in the three educational categories. Thus, differences in general mortality by educational category were taken into account in relative survival analyses. For comparison, relative survival analyses without accounting for educational level were also conducted. The estimation of relative survival was based on the method of Hakulinen (1982).

Traditional direct age-standardisation was used for survival comparisons between the educational groups, as the groups differed in age structures (Pokhrel and Hakulinen, 2008); age structure of all patients diagnosed in 1971–2005 was used as a standard for each site. The ages used in the standardisation were 25–44, 45–54, 55–64, 65–74 and 75 years and more. This method maintained the interpretability of the standardised figures and the comparability between calendar periods, genders and educational categories within a cancer site but not between sites. Results were calculated for patient diagnosed in the periods 1971–85, 1986–95 and 1996–2005.

To study and control simultaneously for all prognostic variables, the regression model of Hakulinen and Tenkanen (1987) was used for the cancer (cause)-specific and relative survival analyses. For the former, the expected survival was defined to be uniformly one. The models included five annual follow-up intervals, sex, age (the same categories as in age-standardisation), calendar period (the three periods mentioned) and educational level (three categories). In some analyses, the tumour stage was also included as a covariate. Localised tumours were always classified separately and those remaining were divided into 1–4 categories (regional metastases; distant metastases; non-localised, extent unknown; unknown) depending on the site; the unknown stage category comprised of 10–20% of tumours.

Interactions between age and follow-up year, and age and stage were applied in all models including age and stage to allow for non-proportional cause-specific or excess hazards by these variables (Dickman et al, 2004). Cause-specific survival model results are reported as relative risks (RRs) of cause-specific death for a given category compared with a chosen reference category. The corresponding quantities for relative survival models, the relative risks of (cancer-related) excess mortality or the relative excess risk (RER) of death were also derived.

Numbers and proportions of potentially avoidable deaths were calculated for each educational group by assuming that the age- and sex-specific hazards of dying were equal to those in the high educational category by cancer site and cause of death (cancer of the patients, other causes of death). First, the assumption was made for cancer-specific mortality only. Second, the mortality owing to the competing risks of death was, in addition, assumed to be on the level observed in the high educational category. The numbers of avoidable deaths for all sites combined were obtained by summing the site-specific numbers. The theory of competing risks of death (Chiang, 1968) was used to obtain the disease-specific (‘crude’) probabilities of death needed in a hypothetical situation of reduced cause-specific mortality.

Results

The educational level could be identified from the censuses for 99.2% of patients; those with unknown education were excluded. Altogether, 526 143 patients were included in the analyses (Table 1). Patients with basic education tended to be older than those with a high education, and the educational level increased in the population by calendar time.

The group with a high educational level had a higher age-standardised cancer-specific 5-year survival than that with basic educational level, almost without exception (Table 1). The age-specific and, thereby, also the age-standardised 5-year cancer-specific survival proportions and their differences could not be estimated for every educational group in 1996–2005 for five sites in males and four sites in females. Statistical modelling showed that the higher survival among the highly educated had persisted over calendar time. Differences in RRs of cancer-specific mortality between the educational categories were larger when the stage was not included in the model, and existed only when that RR was under 0.85 (Figure 1). In other words, when there was a larger mortality difference related to educational level, it was partly accounted for by differences in stage distribution, almost regardless of the site. The attenuation of the relative differences varied strongly but was, with the exception of testicular and kidney cancers, quite modest. The health-conscious occupational group still clearly had a more favourable cancer-specific survival than the rest of the high education category (Figure 2). The RRs of this group were also somewhat attenuated when the stage was adjusted for by the model compared with the results derived without adjusting for the stage.

Figure 1
figure 1

Risk ratios for cancer death of patients diagnosed in 1971–2005 (high vs basic education) without and with accounting for stage, by site (sites: OE=oesophagus; ST=stomach; CO=colon; RE=rectum; LI=liver; GB=gallbladder; PA=pancreas; LU=lung; ME=skin, melanoma; NM=skin, non-melanoma; BR=breast; CE=cervix uteri; CU=corpus uteri; OV=ovary; PR=prostate; KI=kidney; UB=urinary bladder; TH=thyroid; HL=Hodgkin's lymphoma; NHL=non-Hodgkin's lymphoma; OTH=other cancers as specified in Table 1). The model accounts for gender where applicable. The diagonal indicates equality of the two RRs. Results are shown for sites with model convergence only.

Figure 2
figure 2

Risk ratios for cancer death high vs basic education and health-conscious occupational group vs basic education and for patients diagnosed in 1996–2005, by site (sites: ST=stomach; CO=colon; RE=rectum; PA=pancreas; LU=lung; ME=skin, melanoma; NM=skin, non-melanoma; BR=breast; CE=cervix uteri; CU=corpus uteri; OV=ovary; PR=prostate; KI=kidney; TH=thyroid; NHL=non-Hodgkin's lymphoma; LE=leukaemia; OTH=other cancers as specified in Table 1). The model accounts for gender where applicable. Persons in the health-conscious occupational group have been removed from the education categories. The diagonal indicates equality of the two RRs.

The relative survival analysis, where the baseline general mortality had been calculated specific to the educational category, mostly gave similar results (RER) to those (RR) of the cause-specific analyses (results not shown). Not using comparable baseline mortality with respect to educational category resulted in larger differences between the RERs and the RRs, which also held true when stage was taken into account in the model.

If in the most recent period 1996–2005, the cancer-specific mortality of all male and female patients equalled that of the high education group, 8% of cancer deaths in patients diagnosed at ages 25–64 years during the first 5 years after diagnosis are theoretically avoidable (Table 2). For ages 65–89, the corresponding proportion is higher, 10%; these proportions are lower, 7%, when prostate and breast cancers are excluded. When the same estimation is done for the first 10 years after diagnosis, the numbers and proportions of avoidable cancer deaths decrease. At 25–64 years, the proportions of avoidable deaths were fairly stable over time. In contrast, in the first 5-year follow-up period after diagnosis, there has been a clear increase in them at ages 65–89 years, even when prostate and breast cancers are excluded. There has been an increasing trend in avoidable cancer deaths for 10-year follow-up period after diagnosis, which almost disappears after excluding prostate and breast cancers. The numbers and proportions of potentially avoidable deaths varied greatly by cancer site (Table 2). Proportions exceeding 10% were obtained for cancers of the skin, breast, female genital organs, prostate and urinary organs, whereas practically no or few avoidable deaths were obtained for stomach and lung cancers.

Table 2 Number (N) and proportion (%) of potentially avoidable cancer deaths in Finland by period, age and site in a hypothetical situation where all patients would have the same cancer-specific mortality as those with a high education

The avoidable cancer deaths will contribute to an increase in the number of persons exposed to competing causes of death, so, there will be negative numbers in the avoidable deaths owing to causes other than the cancer (Table 3, upper panel A). At ages 25–64 years, the total proportion of avoidable deaths is 5–7% in the first 5 years and about 3–5% in the first 10-year period after diagnosis; at ages 65–89 years, the respective proportions are 4–6 and 3%.

Table 3 Number (N) and proportion (%) of potentially avoidable deaths due to cancer and other causes in cancer patients in Finland in 1996–2005, by age, in two hypothetical situations: (A) all patients would have the same cancer-specific mortality and (B) same cancer and competing-risk mortality as those with a high education

By assuming that both the cancer-specific mortality and the mortality owing to other causes would be the same as those observed in patients with high education, the numbers and proportions of avoidable cancer deaths are slightly smaller than those without the assumption of an improved mortality from other causes of death (Table 3, lower panel B). On the other hand, marked numbers of avoidable deaths from other causes are obtained, ranging from 9 to 28%. At ages 25–64 years, avoidable deaths, regardless of the cause, are 8–10% during the first 5 years of follow-up after diagnosis and 7–8% in the first 10 years; at ages 65–89 years, these proportions are 8–11 and 6–8%, respectively.

Discussion

It has been previously shown and generally accepted that cancer incidence is strongly influenced by a person's social position or education, as the risk factors of cancer have not evenly distributed (Pukkala, 1995; Dalton et al, 2008). It is less easy to accept that cancer survival should depend on these factors as this may indicate differential access to care. Nordic countries, among them Finland, have been emphasising equality among residents as far as health care services are concerned. Cancer control plans have been made in Finland since 1952 to secure equal rights and potential access to services (Finnish Cancer Committee, 1984). Our study suggests that this goal has not been reached.

Social group differences in cancer survival have been seen wherever it has been studied (Kogevinas et al, 1997; Mackillop et al, 1997; Rosso et al, 1997; Singh et al, 2003; Madison et al, 2004; Dejardin et al, 2006; Shack et al, 2007; Dalton et al, 2008; Rachet et al, 2008). Several factors related to risk profiles, tumour characteristics or the health system might lie behind these survival differences.

Risk-factor related risks of death may well be working to different extents for patient survival in different groups (Dalton et al, 2008). It is quite conceivable that cancer survival, not only survival with respect to competing causes of death, will depend on factors like smoking, alcohol use and nutritional status, which may also influence the available treatment options.

In a country with good equity in health services, it may sound implausible that health services are better targeted towards the more educated, but the less educated may well be less aware of early symptoms and may experience a delay in diagnosis (Auvinen et al, 1995; Neal and Allgar, 2005; Hansen et al, 2008). Indeed, when the stage distribution was adjusted for, the cancer-specific survival figures of the educational groups were closer.

Early detection by the prostate-specific antigen (PSA) test will increase the apparent survival for prostate cancer (Schröder et al, 2009), and higher educated persons may have more intense PSA diagnostics than others. There is also an increase in survival for breast cancer patients related to mammographic screening (Berry et al, 2005), and that may be associated with the education of patient due to differences in the proportion of participation. A higher education may help in navigating within the health system thus enabling better and more timely care. For example, a higher education of the patient can improve the patient–doctor interaction and the ability to follow care regimens. The fact that the health-conscious patient group (e.g., doctors, nurses, teachers) clearly had the highest cancer-specific survival figures points out that much is achievable, provided that their cancer outcomes could be achieved for everyone. Although higher educated patients may have better financial resources to obtain additional care from the private sector, in Finland this did not exist during the period studied.

The numbers and proportions of avoidable deaths have been used as a measure of what possibly could be achieved if cancer patients’ relative survival could be raised to an optimal level defined by a higher survival in other subgroups (Dickman et al, 1997) or other populations (Abdel-Rahman et al, 2009). These quantities should be estimated regardless of the cause, as some of the persons saved from cancer death are likely to die from competing causes of death even within a 5- or 10-year period after diagnosis. The savings do not last long, as the competing-risk mortality will wear them off rapidly, depending on the age of the patients. Rather than ‘avoidable’ the term could be ‘postponable’ deaths, and person-years saved (Hakulinen and Teppo, 1976) or expected life years lost per patient might be more useful (Hakama and Hakulinen, 1977; Seppä and Hakulinen, 2009).

Conclusion

Clear educational differences in cancer patient survival are still observed in Finland, despite the level of survival being internationally very high and regardless of the very strong improvement in survival over the past decades.