Introduction

Pregnancy is an exceptional and temporary condition in a woman’s life, when rapid changes occur in the body - both internally and externally. During this short period a woman’s body rapidly changes in size and shape while new and compelling signals arise from inside. During pregnancy, the maternal brain’s representation of the internal body would be expected to change, firstly because of the entirely new signals due to foetal growth and movements, and the consequent abdominal changes, and secondly because the maternal brain needs to monitor a new entity: the foetal body. Additionally, the maternal brain should update the representation of the external body to accommodate its new dimensions, for example to ensure that the mother does not sustain injury to her protruding, vulnerable abdomen from nearby objects.

Given the magnitude and rapidity of the bodily changes occurring during pregnancy, one would expect them to be coupled with a likewise rapid plastic reorganization of the neural representation of both the body and the space surrounding it.

Neuropsychological, neuroimaging and behavioural data have highlighted the presence of both slow, long-term neuroplastic changes that are associated with expertise1, or that simply reflect developmental processes2 and more rapid updates of the body representation that occur during the continuous interaction with the external world3,4. For example, blind subjects who use a cane to navigate, respond to auditory stimuli arising from locations near the tip of the cane in a similar way to those presented near their hand. This is likely due to an expansion of audio-tactile neuronal receptive fields around the hand to represent the space surrounding the cane, suggesting that when we become expert at using a tool, we perceive it as part of our own body5,6,7.

The space around our body - the so-called peripersonal space (PPS)8,9 - is the interface between the body and the environment since it is the area of space where physical interactions with the external world take place. This special region of space is constantly monitored by the brain because only within its boundaries can we reach and act upon objects. Recently, it has been shown that the representation of the PPS is not stable, but rather flexible10,11. For example, being in proximity to an individual we have previously co-operated with induces an expansion of our PPS towards that person, so that it grows to encompass the space between ourselves and the other12. More recent results show that sharing a sensory experience with another person induces a remapping of the other’s PPS onto one’s own PPS13. Moreover, experimental evidence has demonstrated an expansion of the PPS representation after tool use: when an individual acts upon far space with a tool, their brain’s representation of near and far space changes, with the far space being treated as near space14,15,16. This effect was first described in monkeys17 and later, a similar mechanism was observed also in humans18. In brief, after training with a tool, plastic changes both to the representation of the dimensions of the body part acting upon the tool - and of the PPS, were observed. Finally, Longo and Lourenco14 conducted an experiment highly relevant to the current study. It aimed to test whether the size of the near space changes with the size of the body, in particular with the key body part used to navigate within the near space, i.e. the arm. Interestingly, they found that participants with longer arms showed a larger near space.

Since there are no reports to date (according to a recent review paper19), about the brain changes in own-body representation that very likely occur during pregnancy this study investigated the changes in the representation of the space around the body in pregnant women. In order to test whether the boundary between near and far space is modulated in pregnancy we measured the size of the PPS using an audio-tactile Reaction Time (RT) task20,21 in pregnant women at three stages: at an early stage of their gestational period (~20th week of gestational period), at a later stage (~34th week) and a few weeks (~8 weeks) postpartum and we compared each measure with that taken from a control group of non-pregnant women. We expected to find that the representation of the PPS would expand with advancing gestation and that it would shrink back to its former size after birth, while it would remain unchanged over time for non-pregnant women.

Results

Participants’ RTs to tactile stimulation of the abdomen were recorded for each trial at each tapping delay after sound onset. Trials that were faster or slower than 2.5 SD of their average RT for that onset delay were removed (<5% of total trials). To investigate whether PPS representation changes in pregnancy, mean RTs to the tactile stimulus administered at the different delays were calculated and compared between the two groups across each testing session.

Due to the high drop-out rate (mostly in the pregnant women group), a mixed model analysis was considered most suitable to deal with missing values.

A mixed model analysis in SPSS was run with the maximum likelihood (ML) method used for parameters estimate. The fixed effects of Session (within-subjects factor: Session I vs II vs III), Group (between-subjects factor: Pregnant vs Non-Pregnant women), Delay (within-subjects factor: D1-D5) and their three-way interaction were evaluated. The intercept across subject was set as random.

Results showed a significant main effect of Session (F(2, 645.19) = 31.45, p < 0.001) and a significant main effect of Delay (F(4, 611.71) = 103.74, p < 0.001). Importantly, a significant interaction Group x Session x Delay was also found (F (22, 614.79) = 3.15, p < 0.001). In order to follow-up the significant 3-way interaction we conducted three separate models for each testing session, with Group and Delay and their interaction as fixed effects.

For Session I a significant main effect of Delay (F(4, 232) = 50.09, p < 0.001) was found. Main effect of Group and Group x Delay interaction were not significant. To follow up the significant main effect of Delay, four Bonferroni corrected t-tests were run (alpha level set to p = 0.012). Post-hoc tests revealed non-significant difference between RTs at D4 (M = 1050.87, SE = 11.76) and D5 (M = 1042.88, SE = 10.76), (t(55) = 1.82, p = 0.074), whereas RTs significantly differ between D3 (M = 1069.61, SE = 13.02) and D4 (t(55) = 3.92, p < 0.001), between D2 (M = 1141.85, SE = 13.31) and D3 (t(55) = 5.86, p < 0.001) and between D1 (M = 1182.44, SE = 16.01) and D2 (t(55) = 4.19, p < 0.001). See Fig. 1 (graph “a”).

Figure 1
figure 1

Results of the audio-tactile task. Plots of performance of two groups on the audio-tactile PPS task, when pregnant women were in their second trimester (i.e. at their ~20th week of gestation period, graph “a”), in their third trimester (i.e. at their ~34th week, graph “b”) and ~8 weeks postpartum (graph “c”). Mean reaction times (RTs) to tactile stimuli (in ms, y axis) were measured at five distinct time periods, during which an auditory stimulus was perceived moving towards the participant’s own body. Error bars reflect standard error of the mean, and asterisk indicates p-value < 0.05, two-tailed. Solid lines refer to pregnant women’s performance and dashed lines to that of non-pregnant women.

For Session II a significant main effect of Delay (F (4, 172) = 64.03, p < 0.001) and a significant Group x Delay interaction (F(4, 172) = 4.69, p = 0.001) were found. To follow up the significant main effect of Delay, four Bonferroni-corrected (p = 0.012) paired-sample t-tests were run, showing a non-significant difference between RTs at D4 (M = 1034.93, SE = 9.70) and D5 (M = 1019.42, SE = 8.34), (t(44) = 2.58, p = 0.013) and non-significant difference between RTs at D3 (M = 1054.46, SE = 9.89) and D4 (t(44) = 2.43, p = 0.019), whereas a significant difference was found between RTs at D2 (M = 1101.43, SE = 10.49) and D3 (t(44) = 8.78, p < 0.001), and between RTs at D1 (M = 1146.25, SE = 14.27) and D2 (t(44) = 4.58, p < 0.001).

Additionally, to study the source of the significant two-way interaction, we ran four post-hoc Holm-Bonferroni corrected paired samples t-tests for each group. The aim of these analyses was to identify the critical distance at which the sound speeded up tactile RTs (that can be taken as a proxy of the boundary of PPS, see12,20), and to test whether this distance differed between the two groups. In the Control group a non-significant difference between RTs at D4 (M = 1017.11, SE = 16.71) and D5 (M = 998.71, SE = 13.03) was found, (t(16) = 1.87, p = 0.079), whereas RTs at D3 (M = 1061.39, SE = 17.86) were slower than at D4 (t(16) = 2.91, p = 0.010), RTs at D2 (M = 1100.99, SE = 20.28) were slower than RTs at D3 (t(16) = 4.46, p < 0.001) and RTs at D1 (M = 1170.56, SE = 30.50) were slower than RTs at D2 (t(16) = 3.78, p = 0.002).

Interestingly, in the pregnant group, t-tests showed a non-significant difference between RTs at D4 (M = 1045.76, SE = 11.59) and D5 (M = 1031.99, SE = 10.27), (t(27) = 1.78, p = 0.085) and between RTs at D3 (M = 1050.25, SE = 11.81) and D4, (t(27) = 0.56, p = 0.576), whereas RTs at D2 (M = 1101.70, SE = 11.86) were slower than RTs at D3 (t(27) = 7.69, p < 0.001), and RTs at D1 (M = 1131.50, SE = 13.32) were slower than RTs at D2 (t(27) = 2.88, p = 0.008). See Fig. 1 (graph “b”).

This means that in the two groups, tactile processing is differently boosted by co-occurring sounds, with a facilitation effect of sound on RTs occurring between D2 and D3 for pregnant women, and between D3 and D4 for the control group. By taking the critical distance at which the sound speeds up tactile RTs as a proxy of the PPS boundary, we can conclude that the PPS size of pregnant women at a late stage of gestational period is larger than that in non-pregnant women.

For Session III a significant main effect of Delay (F(4, 140) = 42.28, p < 0.001) was found. Main effect of Group and Group x Delay interaction were not significant. To follow up the significant main effect of Delay, four Bonferroni corrected t-tests were run (alpha level set to p = 0.012), revealing non-significant difference between RTs at D4 (M = 1013.71, SE = 9.43) and D5 (M = 1004.28, SE = 9.34), (t(34) = 1.62, p = 0.113), whereas RTs significantly differ between D3 (M = 1036.16, SE = 8.37) and D4 (t(34) = 2.99, p = 0.005), between D2 (M = 1070.02, SE = 10.99) and D3 (t(34) = 3.28, p = 0.002) and between D1 (M = 1122.54, SE = 14.56) and D2 (t(34) = 4.99, p < 0.001). See Fig. 1 (graph “c”).

Additionally on a subject-per-subject basis, RTs were fitted to both Linear and Sigmoidal curves (as in22). The Linear (R2 = 0.62, SE = 0.03) model better fitted the data than the Sigmoidal (R2 = 0.52, SE = 0.06) model did, t(133) = 2.11, p = 0.03. We therefore fitted the data of the two groups to a linear function for all sessions, to assess any group differences between the steepness of the slopes of the PPS gradient. The linear function was described by the following equation:

$${\rm{y}}({\rm{x}})={\rm{y}}0+{\rm{kx}}$$

where x represents the delay, y the RT, y0 the intercept at x = 0, and k the slope of the function.

According to a previous study23, the slope of the PPS gradient can be taken as an index of the speed in the transition from peri- to extrapersonal space, with shallow slope indicating a blurred, slow transition, whereas a steep slope indicates a more abrupt change. To investigate whether PPS representation changes in pregnancy, we analysed the slope of the linear function computed in the pregnant and non-pregnant women groups in the three sessions, by running a mixed model analysis. The maximum likelihood (ML) method was used for parameters estimate.

The fixed effects of Session, Group, and their interaction were evaluated with intercepts across participants set as random.

Results showed a significant Group x Session interaction (F(2, 70.82) = 4.81, p = 0.011). Main effects of Group and Session were non-significant. In order to follow-up the significant interaction we conducted three separate models for each session. The models specified were the same as the initial model, with the only difference that now the fixed factor we evaluated was Group only.

For Session I (Mean R2 = 0.735. SE = 0.024) no main effect of Group was found (F(1, 56) = 0.012, p = 0.913). Similarly, for Session III (Mean R2 = 0.65. SE = 0.047), no main effect of Group was found (F (1, 35) = 0.27, p = 0.607). Interestingly, for Session II (Mean R2 = 0.61. SE = 0.048) a main effect of Group was found (F(1, 45) = 6.108, p = 0.017), with the slope for the pregnant women less steep (k = −0.044, SE = 0.004) than that in the control group (k = −0.07, SE = 0.013). See Fig. 1.

In order to ascertain the role of the increase in body size on the reshaping of the PPS, in Session I we measured the waist size of each participant. First, an Independent sample t-test showed that in the second trimester the abdomen of pregnant women was significantly larger (M = 94 cm, SE = 1.53 cm) than that of non-pregnant women (M = 77.9 cm, SE = 1.61 cm), (t(54) = 6.58, p < 0.001). We then tested if the slope of the PPS gradient changed between the two groups, after controlling for the effect of the waist size. To do so we ran a One-way ANCOVA on the individual slopes, with Group (pregnant vs non-pregnant women) as between-subjects factor and Waist size as Covariate. No significant effect of the Group on the slope was found after controlling for the waist size (F(1,53) = 0.049, p = 0.826). This result suggests that the change in body size as a result of pregnancy is not directly coupled with a change in the PPS size.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate whether changes in the representation of peripersonal space occur during pregnancy. In line with previous research12,20, in this study we took the critical distance at which coincident sounds speed up tactile RTs as a proxy of the PPS boundary, and we used the slope of the PPS gradient as an index of the speed in the transition from peri- to extrapersonal space. Our results show that at a late stage of pregnancy (i.e. at around the 34th week of gestational period) significant changes occur in the representation of the space around the body. In particular, both the size of the PPS increases and the gradient in the transition between near and far space becomes shallower. This PPS reshaping is not observed at an earlier stage of pregnancy nor a few weeks postpartum, when the size and shape of the PPS is comparable to that of non-pregnant women. These results therefore suggest that only when the body undergoes significantly large changes, does the brain adapt the representation of the surrounding space accordingly. Our findings are in line with several studies revealing how rapidly the representation of the PPS adapts to experimentally-induced changes. For example, in a recent study Canzoneri and colleagues investigated the effect of tool use on both body and PPS representations18. After twenty minutes of using a long tool to reach far objects, participants perceived the shape of their forearm as becoming similar to the one of the tool, i.e. narrower and longer, as compared to before the tool-use. At the same time, participants’ representation of the PPS expanded towards the tip of the tool, as to incorporate it into one’s own body representation.

In the cognitive neuroscience literature two main, non-mutually exclusive functional roles of the PPS have been identified so far: the PPS has been described as the sensorimotor interface for goal-oriented actions8,24; it also acts as a “protective bubble” that keeps a margin of safety around the body, with the aim of coordinating defensive behaviours against potentially dangerous stimuli25. Several studies have confirmed this defensive role of the PPS. For example, Taffau and Viaud-Delmon showed that in response to an approaching sound of a barking dog, participants’ PPS increased in size. This effect was specifically observed in cynophobic people - i.e. people with phobia for dogs – therefore leading to the conclusion that the PPS expands only when there is a judgment of a need to protect the body from an approaching, potentially threatening stimulus26. Similarly, in a time-to-collision study, Vagnoni and colleagues showed that the looming image of a feared animal - such as a spider or snake - is perceived as colliding with the observer’s body sooner than a neutral looming image, indicating an expansion of the observer’s PPS boundaries only in the presence of a threatening stimulus27. The present results seem partially in line with the defensive account of the PPS. Pregnancy involves massive and rapid changes in the body both externally – as the body suddenly assumes new dimensions – and internally – while the foetus is growing. As a consequence, the maternal brain has to adequately react to such critical changes. Therefore, we suggest that the observed expansion of the PPS at a late stage of pregnancy might be aimed at protecting the vulnerable abdomen – and the new entity held within it – during the mother’s daily interaction with the external environment.

Importantly, our study not only shows an increased size of the PPS in late pregnancy, but also a shallower gradient with which the perceived space transitions from peripersonal to extrapersonal. In a recent review paper, Noel and colleagues suggest that the shallowness or steepness of the PPS reflect the gradient in the boundary between one’s own body and the others’23. Whereas a slow transition space, indexed by a shallow PPS gradient, is related with a weak self-other distinction, a steep PPS gradient seems in line with a sharp and inflexible self-other boundary. Although Noel et al.’s theoretical account of the PPS is built on evidence from studies on schizophrenia and Autism Spectrum Disorder28,29 (see also Mul et al.’s recently published study30), the suggested neurocognitive mechanisms involved could plausibly explain our results in pregnant women as well.

The shallower slope of the PPS gradient observed at the late stage of pregnancy seems therefore to indicate a weaker and more variable bodily boundary, perhaps caused by the inability of the brain to accurately keep track of the fast body changes. Interestingly, the PPS has been defined as a “stochastic bubble” where computations about the probability of the body interacting with external objects continuously occur31. The wider margin of the “safety zone” around the body observed late in pregnancy could be a consequence of the brain’s reduced ability to accurately compute the exact spatial location of an external stimulus with respect to one’s own – rapidly growing - body. Therefore, given this enhanced uncertainty, the brain starts treating stimuli - usually perceived as far away – as if located in the near space. The current results are in line with previous qualitative studies investigating the experience of one’s own body during pregnancy32,33,34. By interviewing pregnant women at different stages of their gestational period, the previous studies identified an interesting theme: some women reported a sense of disrupted body boundaries and confusion in their bodies’ separation from both the fetus and the external world.

However, the current results regarding the PPS gradient seem to go against our initial interpretation of the observed expansion of the PPS being related to defensive mechanisms. It would be more intuitive to expect that given the increased vulnerability of the abdomen, the PPS boundary would become sharper, but this is not the case. We therefore hypothesize that the space around the body is treated as a defensive zone only in specific threatening circumstances. The main challenge for a pregnant women is to adequately navigate in the world, an activity that becomes more difficult with her reduced agility. She does not need to constantly protect her abdomen from danger, as dangers are not ever-present. This “navigational” rather than “defensive” priority could be investigated in future studies by measuring the PPS boundary while different types of sound approach the abdomen – such as threatening vs non-threatening sounds.

An alternative – although highly speculative – explanation for the shallower PPS gradient takes into consideration the role of empathy. It has been suggested that whereas a steep and inflexible boundary between self and others prevents social communication and the ability to adequately understand others’ mental and physical states (as in people with autism30), a shallower gradient seems to facilitate the process of empathising with others23. Given this idea of a relationship between the steepness of the PPS gradient and empathic traits, we might expect that the boundary between a pregnant woman’s body and the bodies of others will be expanded, since this might facilitate bonding with the future newborn. Indeed, maternal behaviour - critical for an infant’s survival35,36 - strongly depends on the mother’s ability to promptly understand her infant’s cues, predict their needs and adequately react to them37, i.e. her empathic ability. Additionally, we could hypothesise that the changes in the hormone oxytocin that occur during pregnancy might be related with this increased empathy. Oxytocin is secreted during uterine contraction, milk ejection and is important for socio-affiliative processes36,38. Indeed, a recent study by Daughters and colleagues39 demonstrated that lower levels of oxytocin are associated with reduced empathic ability. We therefore suggest that increased levels of oxytocin in pregnancy could be responsible for an enhanced empathic ability. We acknowledge that this explanation is highly speculative, as - according to a recent review40 on empathy in pregnancy and in the postpartum period - no clear evidence yet exists to support our hypothesis.

One limitation of the current study should be highlighted. The size of the PPS can be affected by body size; in particular Longo and Lourenco14 found that participants with longer arms showed a larger near space. This finding could undermine the interpretation of the current results, as the observed changes in the PPS size in the third trimester of pregnancy could be simply due to a change in abdomen size. However, in session I we measured participants’ waists and we found that pregnant women had a significantly larger abdomen than non-pregnant women. Nevertheless, this difference in body size was not coupled with a difference in the PPS size, leading us to conclude that in pregnancy the noticeable change in the body size is not the critical factor inducing a reshaping of the PPS.

To conclude, research on the neural representation of the body usually relies on the generation of transient illusory effects, such as experimentally-induced changes in the perception of one’s own body and its surrounding space (see the Rubber Hand Illusion3, the Full Body Illusion41). Additional evidence on the mechanisms underlying the representation of one’s own body and the PPS is provided by investigations of the slower and more long-lasting plastic changes in body representations following training and learning (e.g., effect of tool-use training on the PPS10). Although such experimentally-induced changes are needed to shed light on the different sources of information that contribute to the representation of the body, body illusions cannot reveal whether natural changes in body size and shape are coupled with plastic changes in the neural representation of the body. With this study, for the first time, we overcame this limitation, by investigating an exquisite case of non-experimentally induced change in body size and its effect on the neural representation of the body and its surrounding space. Rapid changes in the representation of PPS may also occur – but have yet to be studied – as a result of other developmental processes, e.g. during growth spurts. We predict that changes in PPS representation would also occur following large increases or decreases in abdomen size due to weight gain or loss. Pregnancy, however, might result in more rapid PPS changes than those arising from weight gain, because of the greater vulnerability of the foetus and the strong evolutionary imperative to protect it. Further investigation of brain plasticity induced by the bodily changes accompanying pregnancy is likely to be a fertile avenue for future research.

Methods

Participants

37 pregnant women (Age range = 21–43; Mage = 31; SD = 4,8) and 19 non-pregnant controls (Age range = 21–43; Mage = 31,4, SD = 7,3) took part in the first testing session.

28 pregnant women (Age range = 23–43; Mage = 31,8; SD = 4,7) and 17 controls (Age range = 21–43; Mage = 30,5, SD = 7,3) took part in the second testing session.

20 pregnant women (Age range = 23–43; Mage = 32,4; SD = 5,4) and 15 controls (Age range = 21–43; Mage = 30,6; SD = 6,9) took part in the third testing session. Due to a high dropout rate in the pregnant women’s group between the first and the second session, extra participants (N = 4) were recruited late in their pregnancy, therefore they took part only in the second and third sessions. Additionally, some participants in the control group did not complete the three testing sessions (N = 7) and therefore a few extra participants were recruited for session II (N = 1) and III (N = 3).

Procedures were approved by the East of England - Cambridgeshire and Hertfordshire Research Ethics Committee (15/EE/0294) and were in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Participants were recruited by the midwife (NF-G) involved in the project at Addenbrooke’s Hospital in Cambridge (UK), provided written informed consent and were reimbursed for participation.

For the experimental group, only participants with low-risk singleton pregnancy and with BMI below 28 – as measured at the booking scan at 12 weeks - were allowed to take part in the study. No participants reported any neurological conditions.

Procedure

In order to assess any changes in the boundaries of PPS during pregnancy, pregnant women in their second trimester – Mean week of gestational period = 20th (SD = 3.5) - in their third trimester - Mean week of gestational period = 34th (SD = 0.9) - and approximately 8 weeks postpartum (SD = 1.5) were tested. Additionally, a control group of non-pregnant women was tested in each testing session. Participants were asked to perform an audio-tactile task adapted from Serino et al.21 and Canzoneri et al.20, where participants sat blindfolded with their left arm resting on a response box on a table beside them. Participants were sitting on an adjustable chair, adjusted on an individual basis so that the hand and the stimulated area of the abdomen were aligned. On each trial, a task-irrelevant sound was presented for 3000 ms. The sound was generated by two loudspeakers: one was placed on the table close to the participant’s hand and the other one, 1 m further away. Auditory stimuli were samples of pink-noise, at 44.1 kHz. Sound intensity was manipulated using Audacity software, so that the sound had an exponentially rising acoustic intensity from 55 to 70 dB Sound Pressure Level (SPL) as measured with an audiometer positioned at the participant’s ear at the beginning of the experiment. The sound was a combination of two identical samples of pink noise, one of increasing and the other one of decreasing intensity, emitted by the near and far loudspeakers respectively. Both loudspeakers were activated simultaneously, but whereas the far loudspeaker activated at the maximum intensity and then its intensity decreased up to silence along the trial, the near loudspeaker activated at the minimum intensity, and then its intensity increased up to the maximum value along the trial. In this way, participants had the impression of a sound source moving from the far to the near loudspeaker, i.e. towards their own body.

While the sound was played, the participant’s abdomen was stimulated by a custom built small tapper attached to it. In each trial, the tactile stimulation could be delivered at any of five possible delays from the onset of the sound: D1, tactile stimulation administered 300 ms after the sound onset; D2, tactile stimulation administered 800 ms after the sound onset; D3, tactile stimulation administered 1500 ms after the sound onset; D4, tactile stimulation administered 2200 ms after the sound onset; D5, tactile stimulation administered 2500 ms after the sound onset. In this way, tactile stimulation occurred when the sound source was perceived at different locations with respect to the body: i.e., far from the participant’s body - at short temporal delays - and gradually closer to the participant’s body, as the temporal delays increased (see Fig. 2). In previous studies18,42 unimodal trials were added, with tactile stimulation occurring during the silent periods before and after the sound. These trials were used to correct for an anticipation effect on RT, by excluding the possibility that participants are more ready to respond at a late delay as the expectation of receiving a tactile stimulation increases along the trial duration. In a recent work, Kandula and colleagues43 found that these increasing levels of tactile expectation partially masked the effect of multisensory processing within the PPS. However several studies so far have included unimodal tactile trials to control for any expectancy effects26,42, consistently showing that RTs for unimodal trials were slower than RTs for bimodal trials, therefore ruling out the possibility that the observed effects were due to expectancy rather than to multisensory processing of stimuli within the PPS.

Figure 2
figure 2

Experimental set-up of the audio-tactile task. Participants made speeded button-press responses to tactile stimuli (mechanical stimulation from a tapper attached to the abdomen), whilst seated blindfolded. During each trial, a 3 sec sound was played via two loudspeakers, which gave the perception of a sound travelling towards the participant’s body. The tactile stimuli could be presented at one of five time-points during the sound, which corresponded to five perceived distances from the participant’s body ranging from far (D1 = 300 ms) to near (D5 = 2500 ms) the participant. RTs to the tactile stimulus were recorded.

In order to optimize testing time and limit fatigue – especially in pregnant women – we decided to keep the duration of the testing as short as possible. As a consequence, only experimental trials and a limited number of catch trials (when no touch was delivered) were used. It is worth mentioning that in the same day participants took part in other experiments, therefore we tried to keep each testing session as short as possible.

An Arduino board and LabView 8 (National Instruments, Austin, TX) software were used to control the sound, onset delay of the tap and record reaction times (RTs). Participants were asked to respond as quickly as possible to the tactile stimulation by pressing a key with their left hand and to ignore the sound. Ten trials for each temporal delay and 15 catch trials (where the sound was played but not tactile stimulation delivered) were presented in a random order, resulting in a total of 65 trials. The task lasted approximately 7 minutes. As sounds facilitate tactile RTs only when presented close to the body1, we expected RTs to progressively decrease as the sound was approaching. The critical distance where the sound speeds up tactile RTs can be taken as a proxy of the PPS boundary.