Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Review Article
  • Published:

The eloquent ape: genes, brains and the evolution of language

Key Points

  • Human language seems to be unique and has often been studied as an isolated phenomenon, but few traits are entirely without precedent. As originally suggested by Darwin, our extraordinary capacity to acquire language should be explicable as the product of descent with modification.

  • Molecular methods offer a new kind of data for shedding light on language evolution, especially in tandem with changing views of neural substrates according to which language depends on a distributed network of cortical and subcortical circuits with substantial overlap with other cognitive domains.

  • Comparisons between the genomes of humans and chimpanzees, our closest extant non-speaking relatives, show over 35 million substitutions and 5 million indels, with most changes resulting from neutral drift. Large-scale between- and within-species sequence comparisons have not yet uncovered specific genes that are related to language.

  • Genes that are expressed in the brain tend to show less human–chimpanzee divergence than other genes, both in terms of protein structure and expression level, probably owing to higher functional constraint in neural tissue. Nonetheless, against this background, there seems to have been accelerated change for brain-related genes on the human lineage when compared with the branch leading to the chimpanzee.

  • Expression-profiling studies have yet to identify any particular human gene (or set of genes) that is uniquely expressed in language-related regions of adult brains. Most observed expression differences between the brains of humans and chimpanzees have been found to be common to all cortical regions, although this might reflect a lack of power when using current approaches.

  • Positional cloning studies of human neurodevelopmental disorders, such as specific language impairment, developmental dyslexia and autism, can provide candidate genes for targeted evolutionary investigation.

  • Direct evidence of a specific gene that influences language acquisition has come from a rare monogenic communication disorder, caused by mutation of a forkhead box transcription factor (FOXP2). Disruption of FOXP2 leads to problems in learning and producing complex sequences of mouth movements, accompanied by wide-ranging linguistic deficits.

  • FOXP2 is highly conserved in distant vertebrates, but underwent a 60-fold increase in amino-acid substitution rate on the human lineage — of three changes that distinguish human and mouse orthologues, two occurred on the human branch after splitting from the chimpanzee. The human within-species variability flanking these substitutions indicates a recent selective sweep.

  • Human FOXP2 gene expression shows intriguing overlaps with sites of pathology identified by structural/functional neuroimaging of patients with FOXP2-associated disorder. These data implicate FOXP2 in the development and/or function of circuitry involving the frontal cortex, striatum and cerebellum — networks that support (among other processes) learning and production of speech sequences.

  • Rodents, birds, reptiles and fish similarly show FoxP2 expression in circuits related to sensory-motor function and control of skilled coordinated movements. Species-specific differences in FoxP2 regulation in the song system of vocal-learning birds seem to relate to changes in vocal plasticity; ancient neural functions of FoxP2 might have been co-opted to subserve vocal communication on more than one occasion.

  • Although FoxP2 is probably just one piece of the evolutionary puzzle, research into the gene highlights how multidisciplinary studies of the functions of language-related genes in humans, animals and birds offer new routes for addressing old questions about the neural systems that subserve language.

Abstract

The human capacity to acquire complex language seems to be without parallel in the natural world. The origins of this remarkable trait have long resisted adequate explanation, but advances in fields that range from molecular genetics to cognitive neuroscience offer new promise. Here we synthesize recent developments in linguistics, psychology and neuroimaging with progress in comparative genomics, gene-expression profiling and studies of developmental disorders. We argue that language should be viewed not as a wholesale innovation, but as a complex reconfiguration of ancestral systems that have been adapted in evolutionarily novel ways.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: A multidisciplinary perspective on language evolution.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J. & Bever, T. G. in Children's Language Vol. 2 (ed. Nelson, K. E.) 371–496 (Gardner, New York, 1980).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Hauser, M. D. The Evolution of Communication (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Chomsky, N. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1965).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Pinker, S. The Language Instinct (Allen Lane, London, 1994).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  5. Petitto, L. A. & Marentette, P. F. Babbling in the manual mode: evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science 251, 1493–1496 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Goldin-Meadow, S. & Mylander, C. Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures. Nature 391, 279–281 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Damasio, A. R. Aphasia. N. Engl. J. Med. 326, 531–539 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Poeppel, D. & Hickok, G. Towards a new functional anatomy of language. Cognition 92, 1–12 (2004). The introduction to a collection of articles that describe contemporary views of the neurological basis of language, which highlights the limitations of classical models and lays out an agenda for future research in this area.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Pennisi, E. Bird wings really are like dinosaurs' hands. Science 307, 194 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Darwin, C. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex (John Murray, London, 1871).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  11. Christiansen, M. H. & Kirby, S. Language Evolution (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford; New York, 2003).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  12. Pinker, S. & Bloom, P. Natural language and natural selection. Behav. Brain Sci. 13, 707–726 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Gould, S. J. Evolution: the pleasures of pluralism. NY Rev. Books 44, 47–52 (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  14. Crow, T. J. Schizophrenia as the price that Homo sapiens pays for language: a resolution of the central paradox in the origin of the species. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 31, 118–129 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Klein, R. G. & Edgar, B. The Dawn of Human Culture (Wiley, New York, 2002).

    Google Scholar 

  16. Dennett, D. C. Darwin's Dangerous Idea: Evolution and the Meanings of Life (Simon & Schuster, New York, 1995).

    Google Scholar 

  17. Marcus, G. F. Before the word. Nature 431, 745 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Miller, G. F. The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature (Doubleday, New York, 2000).

    Google Scholar 

  19. Dunbar, R. Evolution of the social brain. Science 302, 1160–1161 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Liberman, A. M. & Whalen, D. H. On the relation of speech to language. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 187–196 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Arensburg, B. et al. A Middle Palaeolithic human hyoid bone. Nature 338, 758–760 (1989).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kay, R. F., Cartmill, M. & Balow, M. The hypoglossal canal and the origin of human vocal behavior. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 5417–5419 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. MacLarnon, A. M. & Hewitt, G. P. The evolution of human speech: the role of enhanced breathing control. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 109, 341–363 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Lieberman, P., Laitman, J. T., Reidenberg, J. S., Landahl, K. & Gannon, P. J. Folk psychology and talking hyoids. Nature 342, 486–487 (1989).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. DeGusta, D., Gilbert, W. H. & Turner, S. P. Hypoglossal canal size and hominid speech. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96, 1800–1804 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Fitch, W. T. The evolution of speech: a comparative review. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 258–267 (2000). An accessible overview of the ways in which comparisons between diverse species can help to shed light on the origins of speech.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Bickerton, D. Language and Species (Univ. Chicago Press, Chicago, 1990).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  28. Jackendoff, R. Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 272–279 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Senghas, A., Kita, S. & Ozyurek, A. Children creating core properties of language: evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science 305, 1779–1782 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Nowak, M. A., Komarova, N. L. & Niyogi, P. Evolution of universal grammar. Science 291, 114–118 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Kirby, S. Natural language from artificial life. Artif. Life 8, 185–215 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Lieberman, P. On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. Suppl. 35, 36–62 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D. Jr, Redfern, B. B. & Jaeger, J. J. Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition 92, 145–177 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Nishitani, N. & Hari, R. Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 913–918 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Maess, B., Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C. & Friederici, A. D. Musical syntax is processed in Broca's area: an MEG study. Nature Neurosci. 4, 540–545 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Gannon, P. J., Holloway, R. L., Broadfield, D. C. & Braun, A. R. Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale: humanlike pattern of Wernicke's brain language area homolog. Science 279, 220–222 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Cantalupo, C. & Hopkins, W. D. Asymmetric Broca's area in great apes. Nature 414, 505 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Petrides, M., Cadoret, G. & Mackey, S. Orofacial somatomotor responses in the macaque monkey homologue of Broca's area. Nature 435, 1235–1238 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Demonet, J. F., Thierry, G. & Cardebat, D. Renewal of the neurophysiology of language: functional neuroimaging. Physiol. Rev. 85, 49–95 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Riede, T., Bronson, E., Hatzikirou, H. & Zuberbuhler, K. Vocal production mechanisms in a non-human primate: morphological data and a model. J. Hum. Evol. 48, 85–96 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D. & Mehler, J. Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science 288, 349–351 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. & Fitch, W. T. The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298, 1569–1579 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Pinker, S. & Jackendoff, R. The faculty of language: what's special about it? Cognition 95, 201–236 (2005). References 42 and 43 provide in-depth discussions of the relationship between language and other aspects of cognition, and the extent to which the evolution of human communication depended on language- and human-specific adaptations.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Marcus, G. F. The Birth of the Mind: How a Tiny Number of Genes Creates the Complexities of Human Thought (Basic Books, New York, 2004).

    Google Scholar 

  45. Olson, M. V. & Varki, A. Sequencing the chimpanzee genome: insights into human evolution and disease. Nature Rev. Genet. 4, 20–28 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Preuss, T. M., Caceres, M., Oldham, M. C. & Geschwind, D. H. Human brain evolution: insights from microarrays. Nature Rev. Genet. 5, 850–860 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S. & Monaco, A. P. Deciphering the genetic basis of speech and language disorders. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 26, 57–80 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Pääbo, S. et al. Genetic analyses from ancient DNA. Annu. Rev. Genet. 38, 645–679 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Noonan, J. P. et al. Genomic sequencing of Pleistocene cave bears. Science 309, 597–599 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. The Chimpanzee Sequencing and Analysis Consortium. Initial sequence of the chimpanzee genome and comparison with the human genome. Nature 437, 69–87 (2005). In this landmark paper, the availability of a complete draft sequence of the Pan troglodytes genome facilitated the first comprehensive comparison with that of Homo sapiens , as well as a large-scale search for signatures of selection. It also showed that most sequence differences between these species are probably due to neutral drift.

  51. Nielsen, R. et al. A scan for positively selected genes in the genomes of humans and chimpanzees. PLoS Biol. 3, e170 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Khaitovich, P. et al. Parallel patterns of evolution in the genomes and transcriptomes of humans and chimpanzees. Science 309, 1850–1854 (2005). This investigation makes use of the draft chimpanzee genome sequence in a thorough exploration of how human–chimpanzee divergence in expression levels and protein structure might differ for genes that are active in different tissues (brain, heart, liver, kidney and testis). Although neural genes diverge the least overall, they have tended to accumulate more changes on the human lineage than on the chimpanzee lineage.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Dorus, S. et al. Accelerated evolution of nervous system genes in the origin of Homo sapiens. Cell 119, 1027–1040 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Tajima, F. Statistical method for testing the neutral mutation hypothesis by DNA polymorphism. Genetics 123, 585–595 (1989).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Fay, J. C. & Wu, C. I. Hitchhiking under positive Darwinian selection. Genetics 155, 1405–1413 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. King, M. C. & Wilson, A. C. Evolution at two levels in humans and chimpanzees. Science 188, 107–116 (1975).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Bentwich, I. et al. Identification of hundreds of conserved and nonconserved human microRNAs. Nature Genet. 37, 766–770 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Enard, W. et al. Differences in DNA methylation patterns between humans and chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 14, R148–149 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Fortna, A. et al. Lineage-specific gene duplication and loss in human and great ape evolution. PLoS Biol. 2, e207 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Cheng, Z. et al. A genome-wide comparison of recent chimpanzee and human segmental duplications. Nature 437, 88–93 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Heissig, F. et al. Functional analysis of human and chimpanzee promoters. Genome Biol. 6, R57 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Enard, W. et al. Intra- and interspecific variation in primate gene expression patterns. Science 296, 340–343 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Càceres, M. et al. Elevated gene expression levels distinguish human from non-human primate brains. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 13030–13035 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Gu, J. & Gu, X. Induced gene expression in human brain after the split from chimpanzee. Trends Genet. 19, 63–65 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Hsieh, W. P., Chu, T. M., Wolfinger, R. D. & Gibson, G. Mixed-model reanalysis of primate data suggests tissue and species biases in oligonucleotide-based gene expression profiles. Genetics 165, 747–757 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Uddin, M. et al. Sister grouping of chimpanzees and humans as revealed by genome-wide phylogenetic analysis of brain gene expression profiles. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 2957–2962 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Khaitovich, P., Pääbo, S. & Weiss, G. Toward a neutral evolutionary model of gene expression. Genetics 170, 929–939 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Khaitovich, P. et al. Regional patterns of gene expression in human and chimpanzee brains. Genome Res. 14, 1462–1473 (2004). This study included a systematic comparative analysis of gene expression in Broca's area and other brain areas in human adults, including the corresponding region of the right hemisphere, but did not uncover any clear changes that might relate to language.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Sun, T. et al. Early asymmetry of gene transcription in embryonic human left and right cerebral cortex. Science 308, 1794–1798 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Rossion, B. et al. Hemispheric asymmetries for whole-based and part-based face processing in the human fusiform gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 793–802 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Amunts, K., Schleicher, A., Ditterich, A. & Zilles, K. Broca's region: cytoarchitectonic asymmetry and developmental changes. J. Comp. Neurol. 465, 72–89 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Musso, M. et al. Broca's area and the language instinct. Nature Neurosci. 6, 774–781 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Clayton, D. F. Songbird genomics: methods, mechanisms, opportunities, and pitfalls. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1016, 45–60 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Jarvis, E. D. Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1016, 749–777 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Scharff, C. & White, S. A. Genetic components of vocal learning. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1016, 325–347 (2004). References 73–75 demonstrate the promise of using contemporary molecular methods to shed light on vocal-learning mechanisms in song-birds, and discuss the relevance of this strategy for understanding human language.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Gilbert, S. L., Dobyns, W. B. & Lahn, B. T. Genetic links between brain development and brain evolution. Nature Rev. Genet. 6, 581–590 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Woods, C. G., Bond, J. & Enard, W. Autosomal recessive primary microcephaly (MCPH): a review of clinical, molecular, and evolutionary findings. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 76, 717–728 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Zhang, J. Evolution of the human ASPM gene, a major determinant of brain size. Genetics 165, 2063–2070 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Evans, P. D. et al. Adaptive evolution of ASPM, a major determinant of cerebral cortical size in humans. Hum. Mol. Genet. 13, 489–494 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Kouprina, N. et al. Accelerated evolution of the ASPM gene controlling brain size begins prior to human brain expansion. PLoS Biol. 2, e126 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Evans, P. D., Anderson, J. R., Vallender, E. J., Choi, S. S. & Lahn, B. T. Reconstructing the evolutionary history of microcephalin, a gene controlling human brain size. Hum. Mol. Genet. 13, 1139–1145 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Wang, Y. Q. & Su, B. Molecular evolution of microcephalin, a gene determining human brain size. Hum. Mol. Genet. 13, 1131–1137 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Ferland, R. J. et al. Abnormal cerebellar development and axonal decussation due to mutations in AHI1 in Joubert syndrome. Nature Genet. 36, 1008–1013 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Piao, X. et al. G protein-coupled receptor-dependent development of human frontal cortex. Science 303, 2033–2036 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Bishop, D. V. Genetic and environmental risks for specific language impairment in children. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 356, 369–380 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  86. Bishop, D. V., North, T. & Donlan, C. Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 37, 391–403 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Bartlett, C. W. et al. Examination of potential overlap in autism and language loci on chromosomes 2, 7, and 13 in two independent samples ascertained for specific language impairment. Hum. Hered. 57, 10–20 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. The SLI Consortium. Highly significant linkage to the SLI1 locus in an expanded sample of individuals affected by specific language impairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 74, 1225–1238 (2004).

  89. Stein, C. M. et al. Pleiotropic effects of a chromosome 3 locus on speech-sound disorder and reading. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 74, 283–297 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Fisher, S. E. & DeFries, J. C. Developmental dyslexia: genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 3, 767–780 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  91. Wassink, T. H., Brzustowicz, L. M., Bartlett, C. W. & Szatmari, P. The search for autism disease genes. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 10, 272–283 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Demonet, J. F., Taylor, M. J. & Chaix, Y. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 363, 1451–1460 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Snowling, M. J. From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia 7, 37–46 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Taipale, M. et al. A candidate gene for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein dynamically regulated in brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 11553–11558 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Scerri, T. S. et al. Putative functional alleles of DYX1C1 are not associated with dyslexia susceptibility in a large sample of sibling pairs from the UK. J. Med. Genet. 41, 853–857 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Francks, C. et al. A 77-kilobase region of chromosome 6p22.2 is associated with dyslexia in families from the United Kingdom and from the United States. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 75, 1046–1058 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Cope, N. et al. Strong evidence that KIAA0319 on chromosome 6p is a susceptibility gene for developmental dyslexia. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 76, 581–591 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  98. Tager-Flusberg, H., Joseph, R. & Folstein, S. Current directions in research on autism. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 7, 21–29 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Bradford, Y. et al. Incorporating language phenotypes strengthens evidence of linkage to autism. Am. J. Med. Genet. 105, 539–547 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Alarcón, M., Yonan, A. L., Gilliam, T. C., Cantor, R. M. & Geschwind, D. H. Quantitative genome scan and ordered-subsets analysis of autism endophenotypes support language QTLs. Mol. Psychiatry 10, 747–757 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Hurst, J. A., Baraitser, M., Auger, E., Graham, F. & Norell, S. An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 32, 352–355 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Lai, C. S., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F. & Monaco, A. P. A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413, 519–523 (2001). The first demonstration that specific genetic mutations (a missense mutation and a translocation, both involving FOXP2 ) can lead to a developmental disruption of speech and language acquisition.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Vargha-Khadem, F. et al. Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 12695–12700 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  104. Gopnik, M. & Crago, M. B. Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition 39, 1–50 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Watkins, K. E., Dronkers, N. F. & Vargha-Khadem, F. Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: comparison with acquired aphasia. Brain 125, 452–464 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Marcus, G. F. & Fisher, S. E. FOXP2 in focus: what can genes tell us about speech and language? Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 257–262 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Copp, A. & Mishkin, M. FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 6, 131–138 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  108. Fisher, S. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K. E., Monaco, A. P. & Pembrey, M. E. Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature Genet. 18, 168–170 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Lai, C. S. et al. The SPCH1 region on human 7q31: genomic characterization of the critical interval and localization of translocations associated with speech and language disorder. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 67, 357–368 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  110. Newbury, D. F. et al. FOXP2 is not a major susceptibility gene for autism or specific language impairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 70, 1318–1327 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  111. MacDermot, K. D. et al. Identification of FOXP2 truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language deficits. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 76, 1074–1080 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  112. Sarda, P. et al. [Interstitial deletion in the long arm of chromosome 7]. Ann. Genet. 31, 258–261 (1988) (in French).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Enard, W. et al. Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418, 869–872 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Zhang, J., Webb, D. M. & Podlaha, O. Accelerated protein evolution and origins of human-specific features: FOXP2 as an example. Genetics 162, 1825–1835 (2002). Driven by the finding that FOXP2 disruption is implicated in a human speech and language disorder, references 113 and 114 describe two independent targeted examinations of the primate evolution of this gene, each reaching the conclusion that FOXP2 was subject to positive selection in recent human history.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  115. Webb, D. M. & Zhang, J. FoxP2 in song-learning birds and vocal-learning mammals. J. Hered. 96, 212–216 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Belton, E., Salmond, C. H., Watkins, K. E., Vargha-Khadem, F. & Gadian, D. G. Bilateral brain abnormalities associated with dominantly inherited verbal and orofacial dyspraxia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 18, 194–200 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  117. Liégeois, F. et al. Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nature Neurosci. 6, 1230–1237 (2003). References 116 and 117 provide essential insights into the consequences of FOXP2 disruption for the structure and function of the adult human brain, uncovering a distributed pathology that involves cortical and subcortical structures.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Lai, C. S., Gerrelli, D., Monaco, A. P., Fisher, S. E. & Copp, A. J. FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder. Brain 126, 2455–2462 (2003). This study revealed striking overlaps between the sites of FOXP2 expression in the human fetal brain and known sites of anomaly in a FOXP2 -related disorder, previously highlighted by structural or functional neuroimaging. There was also remarkable concordance between the human neural expression pattern and that observed for rodent Foxp2 , described here and in references 119 and 120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Ferland, R. J., Cherry, T. J., Preware, P. O., Morrisey, E. E. & Walsh, C. A. Characterization of Foxp2 and Foxp1 mRNA and protein in the developing and mature brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 460, 266–279 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Takahashi, K., Liu, F. C., Hirokawa, K. & Takahashi, H. Expression of Foxp2, a gene involved in speech and language, in the developing and adult striatum. J. Neurosci. Res. 73, 61–72 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Haesler, S. et al. FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners. J. Neurosci. 24, 3164–3175 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  122. Teramitsu, I., Kudo, L. C., London, S. E., Geschwind, D. H. & White, S. A. Parallel FoxP1 and FoxP2 expression in songbird and human brain predicts functional interaction. J. Neurosci. 24, 3152–3163 (2004). The molecular studies of song-bird brains in references 121 and 122 support the intriguing hypothesis that regulation of FoxP1 and FoxP2 in the song nuclei of vocal-learning species might be related to vocal plasticity.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  123. Bonkowsky, J. L. & Chien, C. B. Molecular cloning and developmental expression of foxP2 in zebrafish. Dev. Dyn. 234, 740–746 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  124. Wang, B., Lin, D., Li, C. & Tucker, P. Multiple domains define the expression and regulatory properties of Foxp1 forkhead transcriptional repressors. J. Biol. Chem. 278, 24259–24268 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  125. Shu, W., Yang, H., Zhang, L., Lu, M. M. & Morrisey, E. E. Characterization of a new subfamily of winged-helix/forkhead (Fox) genes that are expressed in the lung and act as transcriptional repressors. J. Biol. Chem. 276, 27488–27497 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Ehret, G. Infant rodent ultrasounds — a gate to the understanding of sound communication. Behav. Genet. 35, 19–29 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Holy, T. E & Guo Z. Ultrasonic songs of male mice. PLoS Biol. 3, e386 (2005). This is the first description of the properties of mouse song, which demonstrates unexpected similarities with bird song. These findings indicate that the neural underpinnings of song production and perception might be studied using the well-established techniques of genetic manipulation that are available for the mouse.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  128. Shu, W. et al. Altered ultrasonic vocalization in mice with a disruption in the Foxp2 gene. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 9643–9648 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  129. Kuhl, P. K. Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 5, 831–843 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  130. Crosson, B. Subcortical Functions in Language and Memory (Guilford, New York, 1992).

    Google Scholar 

  131. Jarvis, E. D. et al. Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 6, 151–159 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  132. Clark, A. G. et al. Inferring nonneutral evolution from human–chimp–mouse orthologous gene trios. Science 302, 1960–1963 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  133. McDonald, J. H. & Kreitman, M. Adaptive protein evolution at the Adh locus in Drosophila. Nature 351, 652–654 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  134. Groszer, M. et al. Negative regulation of neural stem/progenitor cell proliferation by the Pten tumor suppressor gene in vivo. Science 294, 2186–2189 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  135. Chenn, A. & Walsh, C. A. Regulation of cerebral cortical size by control of cell cycle exit in neural precursors. Science 297, 365–369 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank W. Enard for his helpful comments on this manuscript, and C. S. Lai and F. J. Liégeois for assistance with the figures. S.E.F. is a Royal Society Research Fellow, and his research is also supported by the Wellcome Trust, the 6th European Community Framework Programme and the Brain Sciences Initiative of the UK Medical Research Council. G.F.M. thanks the US National Institutes of Health and the Human Frontier Science Program for financial support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Simon E Fisher.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Related links

Related links

DATABASES

OMIM

BFPP

developmental verbal dyspraxia

JBTS3

primary microcephaly

SLI1

SLI2

SLI3

SSD

FURTHER INFORMATION

Gary Marcus's

Simon Fisher's

Glossary

Linguistics

The scientific study of language.

Phonetics

The study of the production, perception and physical properties of speech sounds.

Phonology

The study of the sound systems of languages and the ways in which speech sounds can be combined.

Cerebral cortex

The outer layer of the mammalian cerebrum, which is involved in processes such as sensation, perception, cognition and (in humans) language.

Macromutation

A single evolutionary event that has large-scale effects on a phenotype, which involves concurrent alteration of numerous characteristics.

Sexual selection

The evolution of a trait as a consequence of competition among members of one sex (usually males) for fertilization opportunities with the other sex.

Hominin

Modern humans and all extinct human-like ancestors and their relatives that existed following divergence from other ape lineages. Includes all species of the genera Homo and Australopithecus, and other ancient forms such as Paranthropus and Ardipithecus.

Larynx

The specialized upper portion of the respiratory tract that houses the vocal cords — folds of mucous membrane that provide the source for vocal sounds. The low position of the larynx in adult humans allows a rich phonetic repertoire, but its significance for language evolution remains a matter of debate.

Lexicon

The vocabulary and word forms of a language.

Aphasia

An inability to produce and/or comprehend language that is due to brain injury or disease.

Subcortical

Describes the brain structures that are below the cerebral cortex.

Striatum

Part of the group of interconnected subcortical nuclei that are known as the basal ganglia. The striatum comprises two nuclei the — caudate and putamen — and is involved in the planning and modulation of movement pathways, as well as a range of other cognitive processes.

Thalamus

A forebrain structure that is located beneath the cerebral hemispheres and that modulates and relays sensory signals to and from the cerebral cortex.

Cerebellum

A multilayered structure in the vertebrate hindbrain that comprises a complex mixture of different cell types. The cerebellum modulates the force and range of movements, maintains balance and is involved in motor learning.

Hemispheric asymmetries

These are differences in the structure or function of the left and right hemisphere counterparts of a particular brain region.

Motor control

The ability to direct and coordinate muscle movements.

Formants

Peaks in the acoustic energy spectrum that result from the resonant frequencies of vocal tracts.

Recursion

A process by which ever more complex elements are generated through the repeated recombination of simpler elements.

Positive selection

When a novel allele that increases the fitness of an organism becomes more prevalent in the population.

Purifying selection

Selection against alleles that have harmful phenotypic effects, which leads to their loss from the population.

Outgroups

When two closely related species are compared, the status of the common ancestor (for example, at a site of substitution) could be deduced by including a more distant third species that branched from the parent group before the other two groups diverged.

Functional constraint

The degree to which changes in gene sequence are tolerated. For genes that have higher functional constraints a larger proportion of potential mutations are deleterious, reducing the substitution rate.

Maximum likelihood

A statistical method that is commonly used to make inferences about the most likely value of one of more parameters that underlie a given data set.

Selective sweep

Occurs when an allele increases in frequency as a consequence of positive selection and concurrently eliminates neutral variation at linked chromosomal sites.

Indel

A difference between sequences of related genomes that results from an insertion or deletion event; a term that is especially used when the evolutionary direction of the change is unknown.

Encephalization quotient

A measure of the relative brain size, in which the brain weight is compared with that of the average living mammal of equal body weight.

Cytoarchitecture

The cellular composition of a bodily structure. In neuroscience the term is used to refer to local differences in the arrangement of nerve cells in particular regions of the brain.

Speech-sound disorder

An inability to produce speech sounds that would be expected on the basis of age and dialect, but in the absence of an obvious cause (such as cerebral palsy or hearing impairment). This disorder might occur in isolation or together with other linguistic deficits.

Pragmatics

Social aspects of communication — in particular the influence of context on the interpretation of meaning.

Endophenotype

A measurable intermediate trait that is assumed to provide a closer link to the neurobiological substrate of a disorder.

Forkhead box

An 80–100 amino-acid motif that is found in a similar form in every member of the forkhead box family of transcription factors. It forms a winged-helix structure that allows the protein to bind to DNA.

Nuclear localization signals

Short stretches of amino acids that help to mediate the transport of proteins to the nucleus of the cell.

Atavism

The reappearance in an organism of characteristics that were typical of the organism's remote ancestors.

Purkinje cells

The output neurons of the cerebellum, which integrate complex inputs and project to the deep motor nuclei of the brain.

Inferior olivary nucleus

A precerebellar nucleus that provides direct input to the Purkinje cells through a network of climbing fibres. Olivocerebellar circuits have a crucial role in controlling movement.

Area X

A striatal nucleus that is present in the song system in the brains of vocal-learning birds.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fisher, S., Marcus, G. The eloquent ape: genes, brains and the evolution of language. Nat Rev Genet 7, 9–20 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg1747

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg1747

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing