Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Brief Communication
  • Published:

Adaptation of gender derived from biological motion

Abstract

Human observers adapted to complex biological motions that distinguish males from females: viewing the gait of one gender biased judgments of subsequent gaits toward the opposite gender. This adaptation was not simply due to local features of the stimuli but instead relied upon the global motion of the figures. These results suggest the existence of neurons selective for gender and demonstrate that gender-from-motion judgments are not fixed but depend upon recent viewing history.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Gender discrimination performance after viewing male (blue), female (red) and gender-neutral (black) PLWers.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Barlow, H.B. & Hill, R.M. Nature 200, 1345–1347 (1963).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Levinson, E. & Sekuler, R. Vision Res. 16, 779–781 (1976).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Blakemore, C. & Campbell, F.W. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 203, 237–260 (1969).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Leopold, D.A., O'Toole, A.J., Vetter, T. & Blanz, V. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 89–94 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Webster, M.A., Kaping, D., Mizokami, Y. & Duhamel, P. Nature 428, 557–561 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Gross, C.G., Bender, D.B. & Rocha-Miranda, C.E. Science 166, 1303–1306 (1969).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Desimone, R., Albright, T.D., Gross, C.G. & Bruce, C. J. Neurosci. 4, 2051–2062 (1984).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Kozlowski, L.T. & Cutting, J.E. Percept. Psychophys. 21, 575–580 (1977).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Barclay, C.D., Cutting, J.E. & Kozlowski, L.T. Percept. Psychophys. 23, 145–152 (1978).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Johansson, G. Percept. Psychophys. 14, 201–211 (1973).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Mather, G. & Murdoch, L. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 258, 273–279 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Sergent, J., Ohta, S. & McDonald, B. Brain 115, 15–36 (1992).

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. McCollough, C. Science 149, 1115–1116 (1965).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Engel, S.A. Neuron 45, 613–623 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Grill-Spector, K. et al. Neuron 24, 187–203 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank B. Krekelberg and J. Reynolds for their useful comments. This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (grant 5T32MH020002-05 to H.J.) and the National Eye Institute (grant 2RO1EY12872-05 to G.R.S.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

H.J. designed and conducted the experiment, constructed the stimuli, analyzed the data and contributed to discussions and writing the manuscript. M.F. contributed to discussions and writing the manuscript. G.R.S. had the original experimental idea, contributed to discussions and writing the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Heather Jordan.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Video 1

Examples of the coherent adapting PLWer stimuli used in both experiment 1 and 2, showing the female (left), neutral (center) and male (right) stimuli (MOV 518 kb)

Supplementary Video 2

Two examples of the test stimuli are shown here, namely the extreme ends of the test stimuli range (.64/.36 female/male & .36/.64 female/male). (MOV 694 kb)

Supplementary Video 3

Examples of the dephased adapting PLWer stimuli used in experiment 2, showing the female (left), neutral (center) and male (right) stimuli. (MOV 735 kb)

Supplementary Methods (PDF 72 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jordan, H., Fallah, M. & Stoner, G. Adaptation of gender derived from biological motion. Nat Neurosci 9, 738–739 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1710

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1710

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing