Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Corticolimbic gating of emotion-driven punishment

Subjects

Abstract

Determining the appropriate punishment for a norm violation requires consideration of both the perpetrator's state of mind (for example, purposeful or blameless) and the strong emotions elicited by the harm caused by their actions. It has been hypothesized that such affective responses serve as a heuristic that determines appropriate punishment. However, an actor's mental state often trumps the effect of emotions, as unintended harms may go unpunished, regardless of their magnitude. Using fMRI, we found that emotionally graphic descriptions of harmful acts amplify punishment severity, boost amygdala activity and strengthen amygdala connectivity with lateral prefrontal regions involved in punishment decision-making. However, this was only observed when the actor's harm was intentional; when harm was unintended, a temporoparietal-medial-prefrontal circuit suppressed amygdala activity and the effect of graphic descriptions on punishment was abolished. These results reveal the brain mechanisms by which evaluation of a transgressor's mental state gates our emotional urges to punish.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Effects of blameworthiness and graphic language on punishment ratings.
Figure 2: Amygdala activity mediates blameworthiness-by-language interaction during punishment decision-making.
Figure 3: BOLD amplitude SPMs displaying dACC and dlPFC areas engaged in punishment decision-making (rendered on a single-subject T1-weighted image).
Figure 4: GCM of prefrontal regions influenced by amygdala seed.
Figure 5: GCM-based connectivity identified by a contrast of unintentional > intentional scenarios based on dACC seed region.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Greene, J. & Haidt, J. How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 517–523 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Haidt, J. The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol. Rev. 108, 814 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Bright, D.A. & Goodman-Delahunty, J. Gruesome evidence and emotion: anger, blame and jury decision-making. Law Hum. Behav. 30, 183–202 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Darley, J.M. Morality in the law: the psychological foundations of Citizens' desires to punish transgressions. Ann. Rev. Law Soc. Sci. 5, 1–23 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  5. Goldberg, J.H., Lerner, J.S. & Tetlock, P.E. Rage and reason: the psychology of the intuitive prosecutor. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 29, 781–795 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Shen, F., Hoffman, M., Jones, O., Greene, J. & Marois, R. Sorting guilty minds. N. Y. Univ. Law Rev. 86, 1–55 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  7. Cushman, F. Crime and punishment: distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition 108, 353–380 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Alter, A.L., Kernochan, J. & Darley, J.M. Transgression wrongfulness outweighs its harmfulness as a determinant of sentence severity. Law Hum. Behav. 31, 319–335 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Darley, J.M. & Pittman, T.S. The psychology of compensatory and retributive justice. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 7, 324–336 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Shenhav, A. & Greene, J.D. Moral judgments recruit domain-general valuation mechanisms to integrate representations of probability and magnitude. Neuron 67, 667–677 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Buckholtz, J.W. & Marois, R. The roots of modern justice: cognitive and neural foundations of social norms and their enforcement. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 655–661 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Feigenson, N. & Park, J. Emotions and attributions of legal responsibility and blame: a research review. Law Hum. Behav. 30, 143–161 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Young, L., Camprodon, J.A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A. & Saxe, R. Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 6753–6758 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M. & Saxe, R. The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 8235–8240 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Buckholtz, J.W. et al. The neural correlates of third-party punishment. Neuron 60, 930–940 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Heekeren, H.R. et al. Influence of bodily harm on neural correlates of semantic and moral decision-making. Neuroimage 24, 887–897 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Sanfey, A.G., Rilling, J.K., Aronson, J.A., Nystrom, L.E. & Cohen, J.D. The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science 300, 1755–1758 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V. & Fehr, E. Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science 314, 829–832 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Spitzer, M., Fischbacher, U., Herrnberger, B., Gron, G. & Fehr, E. The neural signature of social norm compliance. Neuron 56, 185–196 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Greene, J.D., Nystrom, L.E., Engell, A.D., Darley, J.M. & Cohen, J.D. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron 44, 389–400 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Goebel, R., Roebroeck, A., Kim, D.S. & Formisano, E. Investigating directed cortical interactions in time-resolved fMRI data using vector autoregressive modeling and Granger causality mapping. Magn. Reson. Imaging 21, 1251–1261 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Ochsner, K.N. et al. For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 23, 483–499 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Urry, H.L. et al. Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults. J. Neurosci. 26, 4415–4425 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Banks, S.J., Eddy, K.T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P.J. & Phan, K.L. Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 303–312 (2007).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Buhle, J.T. et al. Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex published online 10.1093/cercor/bht154 (13 June 2012).

  26. Loewenstein, G. & Lerner, J.S. The role of affect in decision making. In Handbook of Affective Science (eds. Davdison, R., Goldsmith, H. & Scherer, K.) 619–642 (2003).

  27. Damasio, A. Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain (Penguin, 2005).

  28. Bright, D.A. & Goodman-Delahunty, J. The influence of verbal gruesome evidence on mock juror verdicts. Psychiatry Psychol. Law 11, 154–166 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  29. Douglas, K.S., Lyon, D.R. & Ogloff, J.R. The impact of graphic photographic evidence on mock jurors' decisions in a murder trial: probative or prejudicial? Law Hum. Behav. 21, 485–501 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Clore, G.L. & Storebeck, J. Affect as information in social judgments and behaviors. in Hearts and Minds: Affective Influences on Social Thinking and Behavior (ed. J.P. Forgas) (Psychological Press, 2006).

  31. Schwarz, N. Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. in Handbook of Motivation and Cognition (eds. E.T. Higgins & R. Sorrentino) (Guilford Press, New York, 1990).

  32. Forgas, J.P. Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychol. Bull. 117, 39 (1995).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Weiner, B. Judgements of Responsibility: a Foundation For a Theory of Social Conduct (Guilford Press, 1995).

  34. Gray, K. & Wegner, D.M. The sting of intentional pain. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1260–1262 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Banich, M.T. et al. Cognitive control mechanisms, emotion and memory: a neural perspective with implications for psychopathology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 613–630 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Miller, E.K. & Cohen, J.D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202 (2001).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Stefanacci, L. & Amaral, D.G. Topographic organization of cortical inputs to the lateral nucleus of the macaque monkey amygdala: a retrograde tracing study. J. Comp. Neurol. 421, 52–79 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Ghashghaei, H.T. & Barbas, H. Pathways for emotion: interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience 115, 1261–1279 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Medalla, M. & Barbas, H. Synapses with inhibitory neurons differentiate anterior cingulate from dorsolateral prefrontal pathways associated with cognitive control. Neuron 61, 609–620 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Anticevic, A., Repovs, G. & Barch, D.M. Emotion effects on attention, amygdala activation, and functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 38, 967–980 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Siegle, G.J., Thompson, W., Carter, C.S., Steinhauer, S.R. & Thase, M.E. Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: related and independent features. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 198–209 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Diekhof, E.K., Geier, K., Falkai, P. & Gruber, O. Fear is only as deep as the mind allows: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on the regulation of negative affect. Neuroimage 58, 275–285 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Hartley, C.A. & Phelps, E.A. Changing fear: the neurocircuitry of emotion regulation. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 136–146 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Ochsner, K.N. & Gross, J.J. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 242–249 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Baas, D., Aleman, A. & Kahn, R.S. Lateralization of amygdala activation: a systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 45, 96–103 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Fusar-Poli, P. et al. Laterality effect on emotional faces processing: ALE meta-analysis of evidence. Neurosci. Lett. 452, 262–267 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Costafreda, S.G., Brammer, M.J., David, A.S. & Fu, C.H. Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: a meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies. Brain Res. Rev. 58, 57–70 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G.L. & Jordan, A.H. Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1096–1109 (2008).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Wheatley, T. & Haidt, J. Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychol. Sci. 16, 780–784 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Carlsmith, K.M., Darley, J.M. & Robinson, P.H. Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 284–299 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Kassin, S.M. & Garfield, D.A. Blood and guts: general and trial specific effects of videotaped crime scenes on mock jurors. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1877–1887 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  52. Goldberg, J.H., Lerner, J.S. & Tetlock, P.E. Rage and reason: the psychology of the intuitive prosecutor. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 29, 781–795 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  53. Aron, A.R. & Poldrack, R.A. Cortical and subcortical contributions to Stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J. Neurosci. 26, 2424–2433 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Forman, S.D. et al. Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn. Reson. Med. 33, 636–647 (1995).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Kriegeskorte, N., Simmons, W.K., Bellgowan, P.S. & Baker, C.I. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 535–540 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Poldrack, R.A. & Mumford, J.A. Independence in ROI analysis: where is the voodoo? Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 208–213 (2009).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Granger, C.W. Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods. Econometrica 37, 424–438 (1969).

    Google Scholar 

  58. Rogers, B.P., Morgan, V.L., Newton, A.T. & Gore, J.C. Assessing functional connectivity in the human brain by fMRI. Magn. Reson. Imaging 25, 1347–1357 (2007).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Harrison, L., Penny, W.D. & Friston, K. Multivariate autoregressive modeling of fMRI time series. Neuroimage 19, 1477–1491 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Kamin´ski, M., Ding, M., Truccolo, W.A. & Bressler, S.L. Evaluating causal relations in neural systems: granger causality, directed transfer function and statistical assessment of significance. Biol. Cybern. 85, 145–157 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  61. Roebroeck, A., Formisano, E. & Goebel, R. Mapping directed influence over the brain using Granger causality and fMRI. Neuroimage 25, 230–242 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  62. Wen, X., Yao, L., Liu, Y. & Ding, M. Causal interactions in attention networks predict behavioral performance. J. Neurosci. 32, 1284–1292 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Hamilton, J.P., Chen, G., Thomason, M.E., Schwartz, M.E. & Gotlib, I.H. Investigating neural primacy in Major Depressive Disorder: multivariate Granger causality analysis of resting-state fMRI time-series data. Mol. Psychiatry 16, 763–772 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Webb, J.T., Ferguson, M.A., Nielsen, J.A. & Anderson, J.S. BOLD granger causality reflects vascular anatomy. PLoS One 8, e84279 (2013).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would also like to acknowledge helpful comments made by N.A. Farahany on the development of the scenario stimuli. Preparation of this article was supported by an award to the Vanderbilt University Central Discovery Grant Program to R.M. and O.D.J., as well as contributions from the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation. Its contents reflect the views of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of either the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation or The MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Law and Neuroscience. The authors also gratefully acknowledge support from the Center for Integrative and Cognitive Neuroscience at Vanderbilt University as well as support by the National Center for Research Resources, Grant UL1 RR024975-01, which is now at the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, Grant 2 UL1 TR000445-06.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

M.T.T., J.W.B. and R.M. designed the study. M.T.T., J.W.M., K.J., J.W.B., O.D.J. and R.M. developed the scenario stimuli. M.T.T., J.W.M., K.J., J.W.B., M.R.G. and R.M. collected and analyzed the data with the aid of critical tools provided by C.L.A. M.T.T., J.W.B. and R.M. drafted the paper. J.W.M. and O.D.J. provided critical comments.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Michael T Treadway or René Marois.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Emotion intensity ratings for scenario stimuli.

A separate sample of participants was recruited to provide ratings of affective responses to GL and PL scenarios (GL group: n = 21; PL group: n = 20). The design of this study was identical to our imaging study, except that subjects performed the study in a laboratory at Vanderbilt rather than in an MRI scanner, and instead of punishment decisions they were asked to report on how strong their emotional experience was for six discrete emotions using a 0-9 scale, where 0 indicated “not at all” and 9 indicated “extreme”. The emotions rated were anger, contempt, disgust, fear, and sadness. Across all emotion categories we found a trend-level main effect of graphic language, such that individuals in the GL group gave higher ratings than those in the PL group (F(1,39) = 2.98, p < 0.092), as well as a significant Group X Emotion interaction (F(1,36) = 3.13, p = 0.027). This interaction was primarily driven by low ratings for fear across both groups; when fear was dropped from the ANOVA, a significant main effect of graphic language was present across the remaining 4 emotions, such that individuals in the GL group gave higher ratings than those in the PL group (F(1,39) = 4.54, p = 0.040). Reported p-values indicate results of post-hoc between-groups t-tests (*p > 0.05, **p > 0.01, one-tailed). Error-bars represent standard error of the mean.

Supplementary Figure 2 Reaction time (RT) during punishment decision-making.

All subjects were given up to 60 s to read and respond to each scenario, though average trial RTs were only 30 s. This temporal window was determined based on pilot data suggesting that >99% trials would be completed within a 60 s time span. Timeouts occurred for a total of 1.57% of scenarios, and they did not differ between the GL group (M = 1.40, SD = 2.29) and the PL group (M = 0.73, SD =1.33) (t28 = 0.97, p = 0.40). Because condition-specific differences in “time-on-task” can have significant effects on BOLD signal, we assessed reaction times (RT) in each of the scenario conditions. There were no significant differences in RT between Intentional (M = 30.00 s, SD = 5.46) and Unintentional (M = 30.84 s, SD = 5.30) scenarios when averaged across both GL and PL groups (t29 = -1.15, p = 0.26). There were also no differences in RT between the GL and PL groups for either the Intentional (GL: M = 28.87 s, SD = 4.6; PL: M = 31.10 s, SD = 6.15; t28 = -1.13, p = 0.27) or Unintentional scenarios (GL M = 30.5, SD = 5.25; PL: M = 31.15, SD = 5.52; t28 = -0.31, p = 0.76), and there was no significant blameworthiness X language interaction for RT (F(1,28) = 1.21, p = 0.28).

Supplementary Figure 3 Conjunction analyses between BOLD amplitude and GCM results.

To examine the spatial overlap between areas identified in our BOLD amplitude and GCM results, conjunction analyses of the corresponding SPMs were performed. Specifically, for each conjunction analysis, a binary mask was generated for each SPM such that voxels that were included in a whole-brain, cluster-corrected area were coded as ‘1’, while all other voxels were coded as ‘0’. These masks were then added together, and all voxels with a resulting value of ‘2’ were interpreted as representing areas of conjunction between the two maps.A. SPM showing conjunction of independently whole-brain corrected SPMs for the BOLD amplitude Intentional > Unintentional contrast and the blameworthiness-by-language interaction contrast of GCM analysis with amygdala seed. B. SPM showing conjunction of independently whole-brain corrected SPMs for the BOLD amplitude left TPJ between Unintentional > Intentional contrast and the Unintentional > Intentional GCM analysis with left dACC seed.

.

Supplementary Figure 4 SPM displaying left amygdala activation identified by the interaction contrast of blameworthiness and harm.

To test whether the findings of our blameworthiness-by-language interaction generalized to other sources of affect, a whole-brain blameworthiness-by-harm interaction contrast was examined. The four levels of harm (Murder, Maim, Assault, and Property Damage) were dummy-coded from 4 to 1, with 4 representing Murder. The interaction contrast specified was as: (Intentional [+2, +1, -1, -2])> (Unintentional [+2, +1, - 1, -2]), with the numbers in brackets referring to the weights applied to each harm-level within each blameworthiness condition. This analysis identified a significant cluster in the left amygdala, that showed overlap with the peak of the left amygdala cluster identified by our blameworthiness X language interaction (Left Amygdala peak x = -28, y = -8, z = -22, Z = 4.01, p < 0.05 (cluster-corrected). Importantly, this effect was present equally for the both the GL and PL groups, suggesting that the amygdala is generally involved in the integration of harm information in a blameworthiness-dependent manner. Map is displayed at a whole-brain corrected threshold of p < 0.05 (cluster-corrected), rendered on a single-subject T1-weighted image. In addition to the amygdala, there are bilateral activation foci in lateral temporal cortex.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–4 and Supplementary Tables 1–4 (PDF 1536 kb)

Supplementary Methods Checklist (PDF 362 kb)

Supplementary Scenarios

All scenarios used in the current study are presented in a separate PDF file included with this submission. Scenarios in this PDF file are organized by harm-type (murder/death, maim, assault, property damage), with each scenario “stem” presented in its four variations: Intentional/GL, Intentional/PL, Unintentional/GL and Unintentional/PL. (PDF 25169 kb)

Source data

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Treadway, M., Buckholtz, J., Martin, J. et al. Corticolimbic gating of emotion-driven punishment. Nat Neurosci 17, 1270–1275 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3781

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3781

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing