Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Visual search has no memory

Abstract

Humans spend a lot of time searching for things, such as roadside traffic signs1, soccer balls2 or tumours in mammograms3. These tasks involve the deployment of attention from one item in the visual field to the next. Common sense suggests that rejected items should be noted in some fashion so that effort is not expended in re-examining items that have been attended to and rejected. However, common sense is wrong. Here we asked human observers to search for a letter ‘T’ among letters ‘L’. This search demandsvisual attention and normally proceeds at a rate of 20–30 milliseconds per item4. In the critical condition, we randomly relocated all letters every 111 milliseconds. This made it impossible for the subjects to keep track of the progress of the search. Nevertheless, the efficiency of the search was unchanged. Theories of visual search all assume that search relies on accumulating information about the identity of objects over time5,6,7. Such theories predict that search efficiency will be drastically reduced if the scene is continually shuffled while the observer is trying to search through it. As we show that efficiency is not impaired, the standard theories must be revised.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Two example stimulus frames from experiment 1, each followed by its corresponding masking frame.
Figure 2: Results of experiments 1 and 2.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Fairclough, S. & Maternaghan, M. in Visual Search 2 (eds Brogan, D., Gale, A. & Carr, K.) 419–431 (Taylor & Francis, London, (1993)).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Helsen, W. & Pauwels, J. M. in Visual Search 2 (eds Brogan, D., Gale, A. & Carr, K.) 379–388 (Taylor & Francis, London, (1993)).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Gale, A. G. & Walker, G. E. in Visual Search 2 (eds Brogan, D., Gale, A. & Carr, K.) 231–238 (Taylor & Francis, London, (1993)).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Wolfe, J. M. What can 1 million trials tell us about visual search? Psychol. Sci. 9, 33–39 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Treisman, A. & Gelade, G. Afeature–integration theory of attention. Cogn. Psychol. 12, 97–136 (1980).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Palmer, J. Attention in visual search: distinguishing four causes of a set-size effect. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 4, 118–123 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Grossberg, S., Mingolla, E. & Ross, W. D. Aneural theory of attentive visual search: interactions of boundary, surface, spatial, and object representations. Psychol. Rev. 101, 470–489 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Chun, M. M. & Wolfe, J. M. Just say no: how are visual searches terminated when there is no target present? Cogn. Psychol. 30, 39–78 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Wolfe, J. M. Guided search 2.0: a revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 1, 202–238 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Irwin, D. E. Integrating information across saccadic eye movements. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 5, 94–100 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Rensink, R. A., O'Regan, J. K. & Clark, J. J. To see or not to see: the need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychol. Sci. 8, 368–373 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Simons, D. J. & Levin, D. T. Change blindness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1, 261–267 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank L. Adams, K. Dahlen, H. Egeth, G. Gancarz, I. Horowitz, J. Mangels, R.Klein, A.Treisman, R. Ward and S. Yantis for comments on the manuscript, and N. Klempen for assistance with data collection. Supported by grants from AFOSR, NIH and NSF (to J.M.W.) and an NIH individual NRSA (T.S.H.)

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Todd S. Horowitz.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Horowitz, T., Wolfe, J. Visual search has no memory. Nature 394, 575–577 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1038/29068

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/29068

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing