Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Stimulus Probability and Simple Reaction Time

Abstract

SIMPLE reaction time tasks usually contain procedures designed to reduce or eliminate the frequency with which subjects anticipate the onset of the stimulus. These measures include varying the interval or foreperiod between the warning stimulus and the subsequent stimulus, and introducing a number of “catch trials” on which the stimulus does not follow the warning. Obviously, one effect of including catch trials is to lower the overall probability that the stimulus will follow the warning stimulus. In view of the known importance of stimulus probability in choice reaction time situations, it is perhaps surprising that, although there have been several studies (for example, refs. 3–5) of the effects of changes in the range and variability of foreperiods, there appears to have been only a single study in which the effect of changing the frequency of catch trials has received systematic investigation. Drazin2 introduced catch trials to produce three levels of probability of occurrence of the stimulus (s): P(s) = 1.0, 0.9 and 0.5. These levels were allowed to interact with mean fore-period, so that Drazin's results regarding the effects of P(s) on reaction time are limited to the demonstration of (1) an inverse relationship between P(s) and reaction time, and (2) an interaction between P(s) and the reaction time–foreperiod relationship. The study reported here was designed to examine the relationship of the P(s) and reaction time over a wide range of P(s) values in a sequential, simple reaction time situation.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Annett, J., Quart. J. Exp. Psychol., 18, 273 (1966).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Drazin, D. H., J. Exp. Psychol., 62, 43 (1961).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Karlin, L., J. Exp. Psychol., 58, 185 (1959).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Klemmer, E. T., J. Exp. Psychol., 51, 179 (1956).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Woodrow, H., Psychol. Monog., 17 (5,Whole No. 76).

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

GORDON, I. Stimulus Probability and Simple Reaction Time. Nature 215, 895–896 (1967). https://doi.org/10.1038/215895a0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/215895a0

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing