Skip to main content
Original Article

Sequential Processing of Two-Digit Numbers

Evidence of Decomposition From a Perceptual Number Matching Task

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000042

Two experiments using a number matching task (NMT) explored whether two-digit numbers are processed holistically or in a compositional fashion. In the NMT participants are required to decide whether one of the two numbers initially provided (cues) is presented some milliseconds later or not (probe). Probes which have some arithmetic relationship to the cues (e.g., cues: 2 3, probe: 6) are rejected more slowy than probes unrelated to their cues (e.g., cues: 2 3, probe: 7) – interference effect –, and this is considered as evidence of the automatic activation of that arithmetic relationship. Participants were presented with two-digit cues and probes which had an arithmetic progression relationship only detectable once the numbers were decomposed (Experiment 1: cues: 56 7, probe: 89; Experiment 2: cues: 45 67, probe: 89). Results showed longer response times in these conditions compared to unrelated conditions. Data support componential processing even when the numbers to be matched are presented serially.

References

  • Beringer, J. (1999). Experimental Run Time System (ERTS). Frankfurt, Germany: BeriSoft Cooperation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brysbaert, M. (1995). Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 434–452. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dehaene, S. , Bossini, S. , & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and numerical magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fias, W. , Fischer, M. (2005). Spatial Representation of Numbers. In J. I. D. Campbell, (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 43–54). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Galfano, G. , Rusconi, E. , & Umiltá, C. (2003). Automatic activation of multiplication facts: Evidence from the nodes adjacent to the product. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 31–61. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ganor-Stern, D. , Pinhas, M. , Tzelgov, J. (2009). Comparing two-digit numbers: The importance of being presented together. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 444–452. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • García-Orza, J. , Perea, M. (2011). Two-digit coding in number processing: Evidence from number decision and same-different task. Experimental Psychology, 58, 85–91. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Gazzellini, S. , Laudanna, A. (2011). Digit repetition effect in two-digit number comparison. Zeitschrift für Psychologie / Journal of Psychology, 219, 30–37. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • LeFevre, J. A. , Bisanz, J. , & Mrkonjic, L. (1988). Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts. Memory & Cognition, 16, 45–53. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mann, A. , Moeller, K. , Pixner, S. , Kaufmann, L. , Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Attentional strategies in place-value integration: A longitudinal study on two-digit number comparison. Zeitschrift für Psychologie / Journal of Psychology, 219, 43–50. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McCloskey, M. (1992). Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia. Cognition, 44, 107–157. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moeller, K. , Nuerk, H.-C. , Willmes, K. (2009). Internal number magnitude representation is not holistic, either. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 672–685. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner, G. W. Humphreys, (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nuerk, H.-C. , Moeller, K. , Klein, E. , Willmes, K. , & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Extending the mental number line: A review of multi-digit number processing. Zeitschrift für Psychologie / Journal of Psychology, 219, 3–22. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Nuerk, H.-C. , Weger, U. , & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back into different bins. Cognition, 82, B25–B33. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nuerk, H.-C. , Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representations of two-digit numbers. Psychology Science, 47, 52–72. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Reynvoet, B. , Brysbaert, M. (1999). Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numbers address the same semantic number line. Cognition, 72, 191–201. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reynvoet, B. , Notebaert, K. , Van den Bussche, E. (2011). The processing of two-digit numbers depends on task instructions. Zeitschrift für Psychologie / Journal of Psychology, 219, 38–42. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rusconi, E. , Galfano, G. , Rebonato, E. , Umiltá, C. (2006). Bidirectional links in the network of multiplication facts. Psychological Research, 70, 32–42. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Szücs, D. , Soltész, F. (2010). Event-related brain potentials to violations of arithmetic syntax represented by place value structure. Biological Psychology, 84, 354–367. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thibodeau, M. H. , LeFevre, J. , Bisanz, J. (1996). The extension of the interference effect to multiplication. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 393–396. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verguts, T. , De Moor, W. (2005). Two-digit comparison: Decomposed, holistic, or hybrid? Experimental Psychology, 52, 195–200. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Zhang, J. , Wang, H. (2005). The effect of external representation on numeric tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 817–838. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhou, X. , Chen, C. , Chen, L. , Dong, Q. (2008). Holistic or compositional representation of two-digit numbers? Evidence from the distance, magnitude, and SNARC effects in a number matching task. Cognition, 106, 1526–1536. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar