Skip to main content
Original Article

Powerful People Feel Less Fear of Negative Evaluation

The Mediating Role of Personal Control Belief

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000299

Abstract. To directly examine why an individual’s capacity to influence others by providing valued resources (i.e., power) could decrease the concerns about negative evaluation from others (i.e., the fear of negative evaluation, FNE) in daily life, two studies were conducted. Results found that perceived power (Study 1) was associated with lower FNE, and manipulating power levels (Study 2) caused less FNE. Furthermore, results indicate that personal control belief mediated this link. These findings provide empirical evidence of the underlying mechanism of the effect of power on reducing FNE. The current research contributes significantly because it sheds light on how power transferred from a person’s “external world” to their “internal world” (i.e., personal control belief) can influence their cognition and behavior.

References

  • Anderson, C. & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). The experience of power: Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1362–1377. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1362 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C. & Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power, optimism, and risk‐taking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 511–536. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.324 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C., John, O. P. & Keltner, D. (2012). The personal sense of power. Journal of Personality, 80, 313–344. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00734.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Avison, W. R. & Cairney, J. (2003). Social structure, stress, and personal control. In S. H. ZaritL. I. PearlinK. W. SchaieEds., Personal control in social and life course contexts (pp. 127–164). New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bombari, D., Schmid Mast, M. & Bachmann, M. (2017). Felt power explains the link between position power and experienced emotions. Emotion, 17(1), 55–66. doi: 10.1037/emo0000207 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cuddy, A. J., Wilmuth, C. A., Yap, A. J. & Carney, D. R. (2015). Preparatory power posing affects nonverbal presence and job interview performance. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 1286–1295. doi: 10.1037/a0038543 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fast, N. J., Gruenfeld, D. H., Sivanathan, N. & Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Illusory control: A generative force behind power’s far-reaching effects. Psychological Science, 20, 502–508. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02311.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H. & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453–466. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.453 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Gruenfeld, D. H., Whitson, J. A. & Liljenquist, K. A. (2008). Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1450–1466. doi: 10.1037/a0012633 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guinote, A., Brown, M. & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Minority status decreases sense of control and increases interpretive processing. Social Cognition, 24, 169–186. doi: 10.1521/soco.2006.24.2.169 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heckhausen, J. & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102, 284–304. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.284 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inesi, M. E. (2010). Power and loss aversion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 112, 58–69. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.01.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inesi, M. E., Botti, S., Dubois, D., Rucker, D. D. & Galinsky, A. D. (2011). Power and choice: Their dynamic interplay in quenching the thirst for personal control. Psychological Science, 22, 1042–1048. doi: 10.1177/0956797611413936 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kang, S. K., Galinsky, A. D., Kray, L. J. & Shirako, A. (2015). Power affects performance when the pressure is on: Evidence for low-power threat and high-power lift. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 726–735. doi: 10.1177/0146167215577365 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H. & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.265 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kocovski, N. L. & Endler, N. S. (2000). Social anxiety, self-regulation, and fear of negative evaluation. European Journal of Personality, 14, 347–358. doi: 10.1002/1099-0984(200007/08)14:4<347::AID-PER381>3.0.CO;2-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lammers, J., Dubois, D., Rucker, D. D. & Galinsky, A. D. (2013). Power gets the job: Priming power improves interview outcomes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 776–779. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lammers, J., Stoker, J. I., Rink, F. & Galinsky, A. D. (2016). To have control over or to be free from others? The desire for power reflects a need for autonomy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 498–512. doi: 10.1177/0146167216634064 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the fear of negative evaluation scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 371–375. doi: 10.1177/0146167283093007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G. & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151–173. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pearlin, L. I. & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19, 2–21. doi: 10.2307/2136319 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Preacher, K. & Hayes, A. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rodin, J. (1986). Aging and health: Effects of the sense of control. Science, 233, 1271–1276. doi: 10.1126/science.3749877 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schat, A. C. H. & Kelloway, E. K. (2000). Effects of perceived control on the outcomes of workplace aggression and violence. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 386–402. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.5.3.386 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scheepers, D., de Wit, F., Ellemers, N. & Sassenberg, K. (2012). Social power makes the heart work more efficiently: Evidence from cardiovascular markers of challenge and threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 371–374. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.06.014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmid, P. C. & Schmid Mast, M. (2013). Power increases performance in a social evaluation situation as a result of decreased stress responses. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 201–211. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1937 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scholl, A. & Sassenberg, K. (2014). Where could we stand if I had…? How social power impacts counterfactual thinking after failure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.02.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar