Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000005

This study examines the extent to which implicit measures of aggressiveness predict actual aggressive behavior in response to provocation. Participants (n = 77) completed implicit measures of aggressiveness, were or were not exposed to insult from an experimenter, evaluated the performance of the experimenter (i.e., opportunity for aggressive behavior), and completed explicit measures of aggressiveness. Results showed that the implicit measure of aggressiveness significantly predicted aggressive behavior in response to provocation, whereas it was not predictive when there was no provocation. The discussion deals with the validity of implicit measures as predictors of aggressive behavior and their moderators.

References

  • Aiken, L.S. , West, S.G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C.A. , Bushman, B.J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27–51. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Archer, J. , Coyne, S.M. (2005). An integrated review of indirect, relational, and social aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 212–230. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Asendorpf, J.B. , Banse, R. , Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 380–393. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R. , Fischer, I. (2002, July). Implicit and explicit aggressiveness and the prediction of aggressive behavior. Poster presented at the 11th Conference on Personality of the European Association of Personality Psychology, Jena. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bargh, J.A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. In E.T. Higgins, R.M. Sorrentino, (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 93–130). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Baron, R.A. , Richardson, D.R. (1994). Human aggression. New York: Plenum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Berkowitz, L. (1994). Is something missing? Some observations prompted by the cognitive-neoassociationist view of anger and emotional aggression. In L.R. Huesmann, (Ed.), Aggressive behavior: Current perspectives (pp. 35–57). New York: Plenum. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bettencourt, B.A. , Talley, A. , Benjamin, A.J. , Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 751–777. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bluemke, M. , Zumbach, J. (2007). Implicit and explicit measures for analyzing the aggression of computer gamers. In G. Steffgen, M. Gollwitzer, (Eds.), Emotions and aggressive behavior (pp. 38–57). Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bushman, B.J. (1995). Moderating role of trait aggressiveness in the effects of violent media on aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 950–960. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bushman, B.J. , Anderson, C.A. (2001). Is it time to pull the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy? Psychological review, 108, 273–279. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bushman, B.J. , Wells, G.L. (1998). Trait aggressiveness and hockey penalties: Predicting hot tempers on the ice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 969–974. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buss, A.H. , Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452–459. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dasgupta, A.G. , Greenwald, A.G. (2001). Exposure to admired group members reduces automatic intergroup bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 800–814. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeWall, C.N. , Baumeister, R.F. , Stillman, T.F. , Gailliot, M.T. (2007). Violence restrained: Effects of self-regulation and its depletion on aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 62–76. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Denzler, M. , Förster, J. , Liberman, N. (2009). How goal-fulfillment decreases aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 90–100. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Egloff, B. , Schmukle, S.C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441–1455. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R.H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In M.P. Zanna, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109). New York: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R.H. , Olson, M.A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 1013–1027. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Florack, A. , Scarabis, M. , Bless, H. (2001). Der Einfluss wahrgenommener Bedrohung auf die Nutzung automatischer Assoziationen bei der Personenbeurteilung [The impact of perceived threat on the use of automatic associations in person judgment]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 32, 249–259. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Friese, M. , Hofmann, W. , & Schmitt, M. (2008). When and why do implicit measures predict behavior? Empirical evidence for the moderating role of opportunity, motivation, and process reliance. European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 285–338. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Bodenhausen, G.V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, M. , Banse, R. , Eisenbach, K. , Naumann, A. (2007). Effectiveness of the Vienna social competence training on explicit and implicit aggression: Evidence from aggressiveness-IAT. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 150–156. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Gray, N. S , MacCulloch, M.J. , Smith, J. , Morris, M. , Snowden, R.J. (2003). Implicit affective associations to violence in psychopathic murderers. Nature, 423, 497–498 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Banaji, M.R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Farnham, S.D. (2000). Using the Implicit Association Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , McGhee, D.E. , Schwartz, J.K.L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Nosek, B.A. , Banaji, M.R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Poehlman, T.A. , Uhlmann, E.L. , Banaji, M.R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hammock, G.S. , Richardson, D.R. (1992). Predictors of aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 219–229. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gawronski, B. , Gschwendner, T. , Le, H. , Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Rauch, W. , Gawronski, B. (2007). And deplete us not into temptation: Automatic attitudes, dietary restraint, and self-regulatory resources as determinants of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 497–504. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krieglmeyer, R. , Wittstadt, D. , Strack, F. (2009). How attribution influences aggression: Answers to an old question by using an implicit measure of anger. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 379–385. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Langley, T. , O’Neal, E.C. , Craig, K.M. , Yost, E.A. (1992). Aggression-consistent, -inconsistent, and -irrelevant priming effects on selective exposure to media violence. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 349–356. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Metcalfe, J. , Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565–584. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olson, M.A. , Fazio, R.H. (2004). Reducing the influence of extra-personal associations on the Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 653–667. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perugini, M. , Prestwich, A. (2007). The gatekeeper: Individual differences in the chain from perception to behavior. European Journal of Personality, 21, 303–317. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perugini, M. , O’Gorman, R. , & Prestwich, A. (2007). An ontological test of the IAT: Self-activation can increase predictive validity. Experimental Psychology, 54, 134–147. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Perugini, M. , Richetin, J. , Zogmaister, C. (in press). Prediction of behavior. In B. Gawronski, K. Payne, (Eds., in press), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Richardson, D.S. (2005). The myth of female passivity: Thirty years of revelations about female aggression. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 238–247. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richardson, D.S. , Green, L.R. (2003). Defining direct and indirect aggression: The Richardson Conflict Response Questionnaire. International Review of Social Psychology, 16, 11–30. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Richetin, J. , Richardson, D.S. (2008). Automatic processes and individual differences in aggressive behavior. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13, 423–430. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richetin, J. , Perugini, M. , Adjali, I. , Hurling, R. (2007). The moderator role of Intuitive versus Deliberative decision making for the predictive validity of implicit and explicit measures. European Journal of Personality, 21, 529–546. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richetin, J. , Perugini, M. , Prestwich, A. , O’Gorman, R. (2007). The IAT as a predictor of food choice: The case of fruits versus snacks. International Journal of Psychology, 42, 166–173. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schnabel, K. , Banse, R. , Asendorpf, J.B. (2006). Assessment of implicit personality self-concept using the implicit association test (IAT): Concurrent assessment of anxiousness and angriness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 373–396 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strack, F. , Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stucke, T. , Baumeister, R.F. (2006). Ego depletion and aggressive behavior: Is the inhibition of aggression a limited resource? European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 1–13. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Todorov, A. , Bargh, J.A. (2002). Automatic sources of aggression. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 7, 53–68. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uhlmann, L. , Swanson, J. (2004). Exposure to violent games increases automatic aggressiveness. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 41–52. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, T.D. , Lindsey, S. , Schooler, T. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–129. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar