Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000347

Abstract. We investigated the role of cognitive control in intentional forgetting by manipulating working memory load during the think/no-think task. In two experiments, participants learned a series of cue-target word pairs and were asked to recall the target words associated with some cues or to avoid thinking about the target associated with other cues. In addition to this, participants also performed a modified version of the n-back task which required them to respond to the identity of a single target letter present in the currently presented cue word (n = 0 condition, low working memory load), and in either the previous cue word (n = 1 condition, high working memory load, Experiment 1) or the cue word presented two trials previously (n = 2 condition, high working memory load, Experiment 2). Participants’ memory for the target words was subsequently tested using same and novel independent probes. In both experiments it was found that although participants were successful at forgetting on both the same and independent-probe tests in the low working memory load condition, they were only successful at forgetting on the same-probe test in the high working memory load condition. We argue that our findings suggest that the high load working memory task diverted attention from direct suppression and acted as an interference-based strategy. Thus, when cognitive resources are limited participants can switch between the strategies they use to prevent unwanted memories from coming to mind.

References

  • Anderson, M. C. (2005). The role of inhibitory control in forgetting unwanted memories: A consideration of three methods. In C. MacLeodB. UttlEds., Dynamic cognitive processes (pp. 159–189). Tokyo, Japan: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A. & Bjork, E. L. (1994). Retrieval can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 1063–1087. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C. & Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 410, 366–369. doi: 10.1038/35066572 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C. & Huddleston, E. (2012). Towards a cognitive and neurobiological model of motivated forgetting. In R. F. BelliEd., True and false recovered memories: Toward a reconciliation of the debate (Vol. 58, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. pp. 53–120). :  New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C. & Huddleston, E. (2012). Reassessing critiques of the independent-probe method for studying inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 38, 1408–1418. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K. N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S. W., … Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science, 303, 232–235. doi: 10.1126/science.1089504 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C. & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological Review, 102, 68–100. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C. & Weaver, C. (2009). Inhibitory control over action and memory. In L. R. SquireEd., The new encyclopaedia of neuroscience (pp. 153–163). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Ltd. doi: 10.1016/b978-008045046-9.00421-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aslan, A., Zellner, M. & Bauml, K.-H. T. (2010). Working memory capacity predicts listwise directed forgetting in adults and children. Memory, 18, 442–450. doi: 10.1080/09658211003742698 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A. & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory – II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Benoit, R. G. & Anderson, M. C. (2012). Opposing mechanisms support the voluntary forgetting of unwanted memories. Neuron, 76, 450–460. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.025 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Benoit, R. G., Hulbert, J. C., Huddleston, E. & Anderson, M. C. (2015). Adaptive top-down suppression of hippocampal activity and the purging of intrusive memories from consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 96–111. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00696 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bergström, Z. M., de Fockert, J. & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2009a). Event-related potential evidence that automatic recollection can be voluntarily avoided. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1280–1301. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21075 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bergström, Z. M., de Fockert, J. & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2009b) ERP and behavioural evidence for direct suppression of unwanted memories. NeuroImage, 48, 726–737. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.051 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A. & MacLeod, M. D. (2006). Types and consequences of forgetting: Intended and unintended. In L.-G. NilssonN. OhtaEds., Memory and society: Psychological perspectives (pp. 134–158). Hove and New York: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bulevich, J. B., Roediger, H., Balota, D. A. & Butler, A. C. (2006). Failures to find suppression of episodic memories in the think/no-think paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1569–1577. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Camp, G., Pecher, D., Schmidt, H. G. & Zeelenberg, R. (2009). Are independent-probes truly independent? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 934–942. doi: 10.1037/a0015536 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • De Fockert, J. W. (2013). Beyond perceptual load and dilution: A review of the role of working memory in selective attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 287. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00287 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Del Prete, F., Hanczakowski, M., Bajo, M. T. & Mazzoni, G. (2015). Inhibitory effects of thought substitution in the think/no-think task: Evidence from independent cues. Memory, 23, 507–517. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.907429 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Depue, B. E., Burgess, G. C., Willcutt, E. G., Ruzic, L. & Banich, M. T. (2010). Inhibitory control of memory retrieval and motor processing associated with the right lateral prefrontal cortex: Evidence from deficits in individuals with ADHD. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3909–3917. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.013 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Depue, B. E., Curran, T. & Banich, M. T. (2007). Prefrontal regions orchestrate suppression of emotional memories via a two-phase process. Science, 317, 215–219. doi: 10.1126/science.1139560 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Depue, B. E., Orr, J. M., Smolker, H. R., Naaz, F. & Banich, M. T. (2015). Organization of right prefrontal networks that coordinate inhibitory regulation of cognitive, emotional, and motor processes. Cerebral Cortex, 1–13. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu324 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gagnepain, P., Henson, R. N. & Anderson, M. C. (2014). Suppressing unwanted memories reduces their unconscious influence via targeted cortical inhibition. Proceedings of the National Academy of the United States of America, 111, E1310–E1319. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1311468111 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., Murphy, K., Roche, R. A. P. & Stein, E. A. (2002). Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: Inhibition, error detection, and correction. NeuroImage, 17, 1820–1829. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Glucksberg, S. & King, J. L. (1967). Motivated forgetting mediated by implicit verbal chaining: A laboratory analog of repression. Science, 152, 517–519. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hanslmayr, S., Leipold, P. & Bauml, K.-H. (2010). Anticipation boosts forgetting of voluntarily suppressed memories. Memory, 18, 252–257. doi: 10.1080/09658210903476548 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology: In two volumes, V. 2. New York, NY: Dover Publications. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. NJ: Prentice-Hall. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kane, M. J. & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-Memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kopp, B., Rist, F. & Mattler, U. (1996). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology, 33, 282–294. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J. W. & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. J. & Anderson, M. C. (2008). Individual differences in suppressing unwanted memories: The executive deficit hypothesis. Acta Psychologica, 127, 623–635. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.12.004 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, B. J. & Anderson, M. C. (2012). Purging of memories from conscious awareness tracked in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 16785–16794. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2640-12.2012 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • MacLeod, M. D. (2002). Retrieval-induced forgetting in eye-witness memory: Forgetting as a consequence of remembering. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 135–149. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • MacLeod, M. D. & Macrae, C. N. (2001). Gone today but here tomorrow: The transient nature of retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 12, 148–152. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Mecklinger, A., Parra, M. & Waldhauser, G. T. (2009). ERP correlates of intentional forgetting. Brain Research, 1255, 132–147. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.073 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Menon, V., Adleman, N. E., White, C. D., Glover, G. H. & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Error-related brain activation during a Go/No Go response inhibition task. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 131–143. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Navon, D. & Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Noreen, S., Bierman, R. N. & MacLeod, M. D. (2014). Forgiving you is hard, but forgetting seems easy: Can forgiveness facilitate forgetting? Psychological Science, 25, 1295–1302. doi: 10.1177/0956797614531602 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Noreen, S. & MacLeod, M. D. (2013). It’s all in the detail: Intentional forgetting of autobiographical memories using the autobiographical think/no-think task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 39, 375–393. doi: 10.1037/a0028888 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Noreen, S. & MacLeod, M. D. (2014). To think or not to think, that is the question: Individual differences in suppression and rebound effects in autobiographical memory. Acta Psychologica, 145, 84–97. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.10.011 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Noreen, S. & MacLeod, M. D. (2015). What do we really know about cognitive inhibition? Task demands and inhibitory effects across a range of memory and behavioural tasks. PLos One, 10, e0134951. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134951 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ortega, A., Gomez-Ariza, C. J., Roman, P. & Bajo, M. T. (2012). Memory inhibition, aging and the executive deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 38, 178–186. doi: 10.1037/a0024510 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Paz-Alonso, P. M., Ghetti, S., Matlen, B. J., Anderson, M. C. & Bunge, S. A. (2009). Memory suppression is an active process that improves over childhood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 24. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.024.2009 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Racsmány, M., Conway, M. A., Keresztes, A. & Krajcsi, A. (2012). Inhibition and interference in the think no-think task. Memory and Cognition, 40, 168–176. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0144-6 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Redick, T. S., Heitz, R. P. & Engle, R. W. (2007). Working memory capacity and inhibition: Cognitive and social consequences. In D. S. GorfeinC. M. MacLeodEds., Inhibition and Cognition (chapter 7) (pp. 125–142). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roman, P., Soriano, M. P., Gomez-Ariza, C. J. & Bajo, M. T. (2009). Retrieval-induced forgetting and executive control. Psychological Science, 20, 1053–1058. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02415.x First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenzweig, S. & Mason, G. (1934). An experimental study of memory in relation to the theory of repression. British Journal of Psychology, 24, 247–265. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1934.tb00701.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shaw, J. S., Bjork, R. A. & Handal, A. (1995). Retrieval-induced forgetting in an eyewitness-memory paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 249–253. doi: 10.3758/BF03210965 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, Y., Cao, Z., Zhu, Z., Cai, H. & Wu, Y. (2015). Cue-independent forgetting by intentional suppression – Evidence for inhibition as the mechanism of intentional forgetting. Cognition, 143, 31–35. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.025 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar