Skip to main content
Original Article

Is a Trait Really the Mean of States?

Similarities and Differences Between Traditional and Aggregate Assessments of Personality

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000083

Although several definitions exist, a personality trait can be defined as the average or expected value of personality-relevant behaviors. However, recent evidence suggests that, while trait questionnaires and aggregated momentary assessments of personality are highly related, they may also differ in meaningful ways. In this study, we examine the relationship between trait and mean state personality. Results indicate that these two assessment strategies, although highly related, do not show convergence (r = .39–.64) levels that would signify an equity of constructs. In line with this, these two assessment strategies show differential predictive utility. Although the pattern of this differential predictive utility suggests that measurement error may account for differences, the difference between trait and mean state personality predicts affect in a manner consistent with self-discrepancy theory. Thus, although these two constructs are highly related, the differences between trait and mean state personality are meaningful.

References

  • Brown, K. W. , Moskowitz, D. S. (1997). Does unhappiness make you sick? The role of affect and neuroticism in the experience of common physical symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 907–917. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buss, D. M. , Craik, K. H. (1983). The act-frequency approach to personality. Psychological Review, 90, 105–126. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Catanzaro, S. J. , Mearns, J. (1990). Measuring generalized expectancies for negative mood regulation: Initial scale development and implications. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, 546–563. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Costa, P. T. , McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Côté, S. , & Moskowitz, D. (1998). On the dynamic covariation between interpersonal behavior and affect: Prediction from neuroticism, extraversion, and agreeableness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1032–1046. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diener, E. , Emmons, R. A. , Larsen, R. J. , Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diener, E. , Larsen, R. J. (1984). Temporal stability and cross-situational consistency of affective, behavioral, and cognitive responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 871–883. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: On predicting most of the people much of the time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1097–1126 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Epstein, S. (1983). Aggregation and beyond: Some basic issues on the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality, 51I, 360–392. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Epstein, S. (1986). Does aggregation produce spuriously high estimates of behavior stability? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1199–1210. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fleeson, W. , Gallagher, P. (2009). The implications of big five standing for the distribution of trait manifestation in behavior: Fifteen experience-sampling studies and a meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 1097–1114. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Funder, D. C. (1991). Global traits: A neo-Allportian approach to personality. Psychological Science, 2, 31–39. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gosling, S. D. , Rentfrow, P. J. , Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504–528. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heller, D. , Komar, J. , Lee, B. W. (2007). The dynamics of personality states, goals, and well- being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 898–910. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hogan, R. , Hogan, J. (1996). Personality measurement and employment decisions. American Psychologist, 51, 469. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Larsen, R. J. (1992). Neuroticism and selective encoding and recall of symptoms: Evidence from a combined concurrent-retrospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 480–488. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Larsen, R. J. , Augustine, A. A. (2008). Basic personality dispositions: Extraversion/neuroticism, BAS/BIS, positive/negative affectivity, and approach/avoidance. In A. J. Elliot, (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 151–164). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Larsen, R. J. , Augustine, A. A. , Prizmic, Z. (2009). A process approach to emotion and personality: Using time as a facet of data. Cognition and Emotion Special Issue: Individual Differences in Emotion Components and Dynamics, 23, 1407–1426. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Larsen, R. J. , Prizmic, Z. (2004). Affect regulation. In R. Baumeister K. Vohs, Eds., Handbook of self-regulation research (pp. 40–60). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pavot, W. , Fujita, F. , Diener, E. (1997). The relation between self-aspect congruence, personality and subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 183–191. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robinson, M. D. , Clore, G. L. (2002a). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 934–960. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robinson, M. D. , Clore, G. L. (2002b). Episodic and semantic knowledge in emotional self- report: Evidence for two judgment processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 198–215. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robinson, M. D. , Johnson, J. T. (1997). Is it emotion or is it stress? Gender stereotypes and the perception of subjective experience. Sex Roles, 36, 235–258. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robinson, M. D. , Johnson, J. T. , Shields, S. A. (1998). The gender heuristic and the database: Factors affecting the perception of gender related differences in the experience and display of emotions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 20, 206–219. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rusting, C. L. , Larsen, R. J. (1995). Moods as sources of stimulation: Relationships between personality and desired mood states. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 321–329. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sadler, P. , Woody, E. (2003). Is who you are who you’re talking to? Interpersonal style and complementarily in mixed-sex interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 80–96. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schwartz, J. E. , Neale, J. , Marco, C. , Shiffman, S. S. , Stone, A. A. (1999). Does trait coping exist? A momentary assessment approach to the evaluation of traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 360–369. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wu, K. D. , Clark, L. A. (2003). Relations between personality traits and self-reports of daily behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 231–256. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar