Skip to main content
Original Article

An Alternative to Self-Reports of Trait and State Affect

The Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (IPANAT)

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000190

Self-report measures for the assessment of trait or state affect are typically biased by social desirability or self-delusion. The present work provides an overview of research using a recently developed measure of automatic activation of cognitive representation of affective experiences, the Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (IPANAT). In the IPANAT, participants judge the extent to which nonsense words from an alleged artificial language express a number of affective states or traits. The test demonstrates appropriate factorial validity and reliabilities. We review findings that support criterion validity and, additionally, present novel variants of this procedure for the assessment of the discrete emotions such as happiness, anger, sadness, and fear.

References

  • Barsade, S. G. , Ramarajan, L. , Westen, D. (2009). Implicit affect in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 29, 135–162. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blaison, C. , Imhoff, R. , Hühnel, I. , Hess, U. , Banse, R. (2012). The affect misattribution procedure: Hot or not? Emotion, 12, 403–412. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bode, R. C. , Quirin, M. (2013). The Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test for Discrete Emotions II: Evidence for emotion specific priming effects. Manuscript in preparation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bode, R. C. , Quirin, M. , Kuhl, J. (2013). The Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test for Discrete Emotions I: Factor structure and validity. Manuscript in preparation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T. , Berntson, G. G. (1999). The affect system: Architecture and operating characteristics. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 133–137. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Campbell, D. T. , Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the Multitrait-Multimethod Matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cattell, R. B. , Warburton, F. W. (1967). Objective personality and motivation test: A theoretical introduction and practical compendium. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chaiken, S. , Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Conrey, F. R. , Sherman, J. W. , Gawronski, B. , Hugenberg, K. , Groom, C. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The Quad-Model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 469–487. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cronbach, L. J. (Ed.) . (1970). Essentials of psychological testing. New York, NY: Harper & Row. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Derakshan, N. , Eysenck, M. W. , Myers, L. B. (2007). Emotional information processing in repressors: The vigilance-avoidance theory. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 1585–1614. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeWall, C. N. , Baumeister, R. F. (2007). From terror to joy: Automatic tuning to positive affective information following mortality salience. Psychological Science, 18, 984–990. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dislich, F. X. R. , Zinkernagel, A. , Ortner, T. M. , Schmitt, M. (2010). Convergence of direct, indirect, and objective risk-taking measures in gambling. Zeitschrift für Psychologie / Journal of Psychology, 218, 20–27. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Drosten, M. , Bode, R. C. , Kuhl, J. , Quirin, M. (2013). The IPANAT for motives: A standardized measure for implicit motives deriving from the Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test. Manuscript in preparation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Egloff, B. , & Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an implicit association test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441–1455. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Egloff, B. , Weck, F. , & Schmukle, S. C. (2008). Thinking about anxiety moderates the relationship between implicit and explicit anxiety measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 771–778. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. (1982). Emotion in the human face (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. , & Friesen, W. V. (Eds.). (1978). Facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R. H. , Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Forgas, J. P. , Bower, G. H. (1988). Affect in social judgments. Australian Journal of Psychology, 40, 125–145. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Freud, S. (2006). Hemmungen, Symptome und Angst [Inhibitions, symptoms, and anxiety] (6th ed.). Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Fischer (Original work published 1926). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gendolla, G. H. E. (2012). Implicit affect primes effort: Theory and research on cardiovascular response. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 86, 123–135. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goldman, R. N. , Greenberg, L. S. (2005). Depth of emotional experience and outcome. Psychotherapy Research, 15, 248–260. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. , Banaji, M. R. , Rudman, L. A. , Farnham, S. D. , Nosek, B. A. , Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3–25. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. , McGhee, D. E. , Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. , Poehlman, T. A. , Uhlmann, E. L. , Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Halbreich, U. , Asnis, G. M. , Shindledecker, R. , Zumoff, B. , Nathan, S. (1985). Cortisol secretion in endogenous depression. I. Basal plasma levels. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 904–908. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hass, R. G. , Katz, I. , Rizzo, N. , Bailey, J. (1992). When racial ambivalence evokes negative affect, using a disguised measure of mood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 786–797. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gawronski, B. , Gschwendner, T. , Le, H. , Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gschwendner, T. , Schmitt, M. (2005). On implicit-explicit consistency: The moderating role of individual differences in awareness and adjustment. European Journal of Personality, 19, 25–49. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jostmann, N. B. , Koole, S. L. , van der Wulp, N. Y. , Fockenberg, D. A. (2005). Subliminal affect regulation: The moderating role of action vs. state orientation. European Psychologist, 10, 209–217. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Koole, S. L. , Dijksterhuis, A. , van Knippenberg, A. (2001). What’s in a name: Implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 669–685. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koole, S. L. , Govorun, O. , Cheng, C. M. , Gallucci, M. (2009). Pulling yourself together: Meditation promotes congruence between implicit and explicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1220–1226. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koole, S. L. , Jostmann, N. B. (2004). Getting a grip on your feelings: Effects of action orientation and external demands on intuitive affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 974–990. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kuhl, J. (2000). A functional-design approach to motivation and self-regulation: The dynamics of personality systems and interactions. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 111–169). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lane, R. D. (2008). Neural substrates of implicit and explicit emotional processes: A unifying framework for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 214–231. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lane, R. D. , Sechrest, L. , Reidel, R. , Weldon, V. (1996). Impaired verbal and nonverbal emotion recognition in alexithymia. Psychosomatic Medicine, 58, 203–210. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • LeDoux, J. E. (Ed.). (1996). The emotional brain. New York, NY: Touchstone. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Moskowitz, G. B. , Gollwitzer, P. M. , Wasel, W. , & Schaal, B. (1999). Preconscious control of stereotype activation through chronic egalitarian goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 167–184. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ortner, T. M. , Proyer, R. T. , Kubinger, K. D. (2006). Theorie und Praxis objektiver Persönlichkeitstests [Theory and practice of objective personality tests]. Bern, Switzerland: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. , Cheng, C. M. , Govorun, O. , Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pruessner, J. C. , Wolf, O. T. , Hellhammer, D. H. , Buske-Kirschbaum, A. , von Auer, K. , Jobst, S. , … Kirschbaum, C. (1997). Free cortisol levels after awakening: a reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Science, 61, 2539–2549. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pyszczynski, T. , Greenberg, J. , Solomon, S. (1999). A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835–845. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. , Greenberg, J. (2003). In the wake of 9/11: The psychology of terror. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quirin, M. , Bode, R. C. , Kuhl, J. (2011). Recovering from negative events by boosting implicit positive affect. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 559–570. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quirin, M. , Bode, R. C. , Luckey, U. , Kuhl, J. (2013). Existential threat increases implicit but not explicit negative affect in soldiers. Manuscript in preparation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Quirin, M. , Kazén, M. , Kuhl, J. (2009). When nonsense sounds happy or helpless: The Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (IPANAT). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 500–516. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quirin, M. , Kazén, M. , Rohrmann, S. , Kuhl, J. (2009). Implicit but not explicit affectivity predicts circadian and reactive cortisol: Using the Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test. Journal of Personality, 77, 401–425. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quirin, M. , & Lane, R. D. (2012). The construction of emotional experience requires the integration of implicit and explicit emotional processes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 159–160. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K. , Voss, A. , Wentura, D. (2008). Counter-regulation in affective attentional biases: A basic mechanism that warrants flexibility in emotion and motivation. Emotion, 8, 34–46. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rusting, C. L. , DeHart, T. (2000). Retrieving positive memories to regulate negative mood: Consequences for mood-congruent memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 737–752. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sander, D. , Grandjean, D. , Scherer, K. R. (2005). A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion. Neural Networks, 18, 317–352. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scheffer, D. , Kuhl, J. , Eichstaedt, J. (2003). Der Operante Motiv-Test (OMT): Inhaltsklassen, Auswertung, psychometrische Kennwerte und Validierung [The operant motive test (OMT): Types of content, scoring, psychometric characteristics, and validation]. In F. Rheinberg, J. Stiensmeier-Pelster, (Eds.) Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept (pp. 151–168). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmukle, S. C. , Egloff, B. (2004). Does the Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety measure trait and state variance? European Journal of Personality, 18, 483–494. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmukle, S. C. , Egloff, B. (2005). A latent state-trait analysis of implicit and explicit personality measures. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21, 100–107. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schultheiss, O. C. , Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessment of implicit motives with a research version of the TAT: Picture profiles, gender differences, and relations to other personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 71–86. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, E. R. , DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108–131. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steyer, R. , Schmitt, M. , Eid, M. (1999). Latent state-trait theory and research in personality and individual differences. European Journal of Personality, 13, 389–408. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strack, F. , & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tellegen, A. , Watson, D. , Clark, L. A. (1999). On the dimensional and hierarchical structure of affect. Psychological Science, 10, 297–303. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D. , Clark, L. A. , Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, T. D. , Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181–192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Winkielman, P. , Berridge, K. C. , Wilbarger, J. L. (2005). Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 121–135. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wittenbrink, B. , Schwarz, N. (Eds.). (2007). Implicit measures of attitudes. New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar