Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.226

In an effort to remove a presumed confound of extrapersonal associations, Olson and Fazio (2004) introduced procedural modifications to attitude versions of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). We hypothesized that the procedural changes increased the likelihood that participants would explicitly evaluate the target concepts (e.g., rating Black and White faces as liked or disliked). Results of a mega-study covering 58 topics and six additional studies (Total N = 15,667) suggest that: (a) after personalizing, participants are more likely to explicitly evaluate target concepts instead of categorizing them according to the performance rules, (b) this effect appears to account for the personalized IAT’s enhanced correlations with self-report, (c) personalizing does not alter the relationship between the IAT and cultural knowledge, and (d) personalized and original procedures each capture unique attitude variation. These results provide an alternative interpretation of the impact of personalizing the IAT. Additional innovation may determine whether personalizing implicit cognition is viable.

References

  • Arkes, H.R. , Tetlock, P.E. (2004). Attributions of implicit prejudice, or “Would Jesse Jackson ‘fail’ the Implicit Association Test?” Psychological Inquiry, 15, 257–278. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Banaji, M.R. , Nosek, B.A. , Greenwald, A.G. (2004). No place for nostalgia in science: A response to Arkes and Tetlock. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 279–310. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Peters, K.R. , LeBel, E.P. (2008). What makes mental associations personal or extra-personal? Conceptual issues in the methodological debate about implicit attitude measures. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1002–1023. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Banaji, M.R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , McGhee, D.E. , Schwartz, J.L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Nosek, B.A. (2001). Health of the Implicit Association Test at age 3. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 85–93. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Nosek, B.A. , Banaji, M.R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Poehlman, T.A. , Uhlmann, E.L. , Banaji, M.R. (in press). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Karpinski, A. , Hilton, J.L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 774–788. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klauer, K.C. , Mierke, J. (2005). Task-set inertia, attitude accessibility, and compatibility-order effects: New evidence for a task-set switching account of the Implicit Association Test effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 208–217. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mierke, J. , Klauer, K.C. (2003). Method-specific variance in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1180–1192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. (2005). Moderators of the relationships between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565–584. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. (2007). Implicit-explicit relations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 65–69. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. , Greenwald, A.G. , Banaji, M.R. (2007). The Implicit Association Test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In J.A. Bargh, (Ed.), Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes (pp. 365–292). New York: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. , Hansen, J.J. (2008). The associations in our heads belong to us: Searching for attitudes and knowledge in implicit evaluation. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 553–594. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. , Smyth, F.L. (2007). A multitrait-multimethod validation of the Implicit Association Test: Implicit and explicit attitudes are related but distinct constructs. Experimental Psychology, 54, 14–29. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. , Smyth, F.L. , Hansen, J.J. , Devos, T. , Lindner, N.M. , Ranganath, K.A. et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 36–88. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olson, M.A. , Fazio, R.H. (2004). Reducing the influence of extra-personal associations on the Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 653–667. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar