Skip to main content
Freier Beitrag

Leistet voreiliges Schlussfolgern einen Beitrag zur Entstehung und Aufrechterhaltung von Wahn?

Ein systematisches und quantitatives Review

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1661-4747.57.2.125

Aus zahlreichen Untersuchungen geht hervor, dass Personen mit Wahn in verschiedener Hinsicht zu voreiligen Schlussfolgerungen (Jumping to Conclusions; JTC) neigen. Angeregt durch eine erste Studie von Huq, Garety und Hemsley (1988) wurde dies zuletzt häufig im Rahmen eines probabilistischen Paradigmas untersucht. In dieser Arbeit werden die Resultate aus den entsprechenden 25 Untersuchungen zusammengefasst, wobei zunächst geklärt wird, unter welchen Bedingungen Personen mit Wahn am stärksten zu voreiligen Schlussfolgerungen neigen. Ferner wird der bisher offenen Frage nachgegangen, ob JTC ursächlich oder aufrecht erhaltend an wahnhafter Symptomatik beteiligt sein könnte. Aus der Zusammenfassung der Studien geht hervor, dass JTC bei Personen mit Wahn vor allem dann auftritt, wenn es den Probanden selbst überlassen wird, die Menge gesammelter Information zu bestimmen. Dagegen sind die Unterschiede zwischen Probanden mit und ohne Wahn kleiner, wenn sie bei fest vorgegebener Informationsmenge lediglich um Wahrscheinlichkeitsangaben gebeten werden. Damit scheint JTC nicht auf Unterschiede im probabilistischen Denken zurückzuführen zu sein. JTC kovariiert mit dem Ausmaß aktueller wahnhafter Symptomatik. Dies legt die Annahme eines in erster Linie aufrechterhaltenden Einflusses voreiligen Schließens auf wahnhafte Überzeugungen nahe. Es liegen jedoch auch Hinweise auf eine mögliche ursächliche Beteiligung vor.


Does «Jumping to Conclusions» Contribute to the Development and Maintenance of Delusions? A Systematic and Quantitative Review

Many studies demonstrate that people with delusions tend to have a bias towards making hasty decisions (Jumping to Conclusions; JTC). Beginning with a study by Huq, Garety and Hemsley (1988) this bias was frequently explored in the context of a probabilistic paradigm. This review summarizes results of 25 studies while focussing on the question whether there are specific conditions that increase the likelihood of persons with delusions to jump to conclusions. Moreover, it is discussed whether JTC is involved in the development or maintenance of delusions. The results of this review reveal JTC to be more pronounced when the participants are allowed to determine the amount of information gathered. In contrast, differences between participants with and without delusions are smaller when they are merely asked for probability estimates. Thus, the bias does not seem to reflect differences in probabilistic reasoning. JTC is associated with the degree of acute delusional symptoms, which supports the suggestion that hasty decision-making is involved in the maintenance of delusions. However, there is also some evidence that it might contribute to the development of delusions.

Literatur

  • Beck, A.T. , Rush, A.J. , Shaw, B.F. , Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. Chichester: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bentall, R.P. , Corcoran, R. , Howard, R. , Blackwood, N. , Kinderman, P. (2001). Persecutory delusions: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 1143–1192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bentall, R.P. , Taylor, J.L. (2006). Psychological processes and paranoia: Implications for forensic behavioural science. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 24, 277–294. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bottlender, R. , Buchberger, A. , Hoff, P. , Möller, H.-P. (1999). Urteilsbildung und Wahn. Eine Studie zum Urteilsverhalten von wahnhaften, depressiven und gesunden Probanden. Nervenarzt, 70, 987–992. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brankovic, S.B. , Paunovic, V.R. (1999). Reasoning under uncertainty in deluded schizophrenic patients: A longitudinal study. European Psychiatry, 14, 76–83. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Colbert, S.M. , Peters, E. (2002). Need for closure and jumping-to-conclusions in delusion-prone individuals. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 27–31. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Conway, C.R. , Bollini, A.M. , Brevick, G.G. , Keefe, R.S.E. , Schiffman, S.S. , McEvoy, J.P. (2002). Sensory acuity and reasoning in delusional disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43, 175–178. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dudley, R.E.J. , Over, D.E. (2003). People with delusions jump to conclusions: A theoretical account of research findings on the reasoning of people with delusions. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 10, 263–274. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dudley, R.E.J. , Young, A.W. , John, C.H. , Over, D.E. (1997a). Normal and abnormal reasoning in people with delusions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 243–258. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dudley, R.E.J. , Young, A.W. , John, C.H. , Over, D.E. (1997b). The effect of self-referent material on the reasoning of people with delusions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 575–584. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fear, C.F. , Healy, D. (1997). Probabilistic reasoning in obsessive-compulsive and delusional disorders. Psychological Medicine, 27, 199–208. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fine, C. , Gardner, M. , Craigie, J. , Gold, I. (2007). Hopping, skipping or jumping to conclusions? Clarifying the role of the JTC bias in delusions. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12, 46–77. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fowler, D. , Garety, P.A. , Kuipers, E. (1995). Cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis. Theory and practice. Chichester: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fraser, J. , Morrison, A. , Wells, A. (2006). Cognitive processes, reasoning biases and persecutory delusions: A comparative study. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 34, 421–435. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Freeman, D. (2007). Suspicious minds: The psychology of persecutory delusions. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 425–457. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Freeman, D. , Garety, P.A. , Kuipers, E. , Colbert, S. , Jolley, S. , Fowler, D. et al. (2006). Delusions and decision-making style: Use of the Need for Closure Scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1147–1158. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Frith, C. , Corcoran, R. (1996). Exploring «theory of mind» in people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 26, 521–530. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garety, P.A. , Freeman, D. (1999). Cognitive approaches to delusions: A critical review of theories and evidence. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 113–154. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garety, P.A. , Freeman, D. , Jolley, S. , Dunn, G. , Bebbington, P.E. , Fowler, D.G. et al. (2005). Reasoning, emotions, and delusional conviction in psychosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 373–384. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garety, P.A. , Hemsley, D.R. , Wessely, S. (1991). Reasoning in deluded schizophrenic and paranoid patients. Biases in performance on a probabilistic inference task. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 179, 194–201. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hautzinger, M. (2000). Kognitive Verhaltenstherapie bei Depressionen. Weinheim: Beltz, Psychologie Verlags Union. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Huq, S.F. , Garety, P.A. , Hemsley, D.R. (1988). Probabilistic judgements in deluded and nondeluded subjects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 801–812. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • John, C.H. , Dodgson, G. (1994). Inductive reasoning in delusional thinking. Journal of Mental Health, 3, 31–49. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kinderman, P. , Bentall, R.P. (1997). Causal attributions in paranoia and depression: Internal, personal, and situational attributions for negative events. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 341–345. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kuglanski, A.W. , Webster, D.M. , Klem, A. (1993). Motivated resistance and openness to persuasion in the presence or absence of prior information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 861–876. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lincoln, T.M. (2006). Kognitive Verhaltenstherapie der Schizophrenie. Ein individuenzentrierter Ansatz zur Veränderung von Wahn, Halluzinationen und Negativsymptomatik. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lincoln, T.M. , Keller, E. (in press). Delusions and hallucinations in students compared to the general population. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McKay, R. , Langdon, R. , Coltheart, M. (2006). Need for closure, jumping to conclusions, and decisiveness in delusion-prone individuals. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 194, 422–426. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McKay, R. , Langdon, R. , Coltheart, M. (2007). Jumping to delusions? Paranoia, probabilistic reasoning and need for closure. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12, 362–376. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Menon, M. , Mizrahi, R. , Kapur, S. (2008). Jumping to conclusions and delusions in psychosis: Relationship and response to treatment. Schizophrenia Research, 98, 225–231. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Menon, M. , Pomarol-Clotet, E. , McKenna, P.J. , McCarthy, R.A. (2006). Probabilistic reasoning in schizophrenia: A comparison of the performance of deluded and nondeluded schizophrenic patients and exploration of possible cognitive underpinnings. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11, 521–536. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moritz, S. , Woodward, T.S. (2005). Jumping to conclusions in delusional and nondelusional schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 193–207. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moritz, S. , Woodward, T.S. (2007). Metacognitive training in schizophrenia: From basic research to knowledge translation and intervention. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20, 619–625. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moritz, S. , Woodward, T.S. , Hausmann, D. (2006). Incautious reasoning as a pathogenetic factor for the development of psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32, 327–331. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moritz, S. , Woodward, T.S. , Lambert, M. (2007). Under what circumstances do patients with schizophrenia jump to conclusions? A liberal acceptance account. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 127–137. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mortimer, A.M. , Bentham, P. , McKay, A.P. , Quemada, I. , Clare, L. , Eastwood, N. et al. (1996). Delusions in schizophrenia: A phenomenological and psychological exploration. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1, 289–303. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peters, E. , Garety, P.A. (2006). Cognitive functioning in delusions: A longitudinal analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 481–514. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peters, E. , Joseph, S.A. , Garety, P.A. (1999). Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: Introducing the PDI (Peters et al. Delusions Inventory). Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 553–576. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenthal, R. (1979). The «file-drawer-problem» and tolerance of null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dael, F. , Versmissen, D. , Janssen, I. , Myin-Germeys, I. , van Os, J. , Krabbendam, L. (2006). Data gathering: Biased in psychosis? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32, 341–351. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Hooren, S. , Versmissen, D. , Janssen, I. , Myin-Germeys, I. , Campo, J. , Mengelers, R. et al. (2008). Social cognition and neurocognition as independent domains in psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 103, 257–265. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Warman, D.M. (2008). Reasoning and delusion proneness: Confidence in decisions. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 196, 9–15. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Warman, D.M. , Lysaker, P.H. , Martin, J.M. , Davis, L. , Haudenschield, S.L. (2007). Jumping to conclusions and the continuum of delusional beliefs. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1255–1269. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Warman, D.M. , Martin, J.M. (2006). Jumping to conclusions and delusion proneness. The impact of emotionally salient stimuli. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 194, 760–765. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Young, H.F. , Bentall, R.P. (1997). Probabilistic reasoning in deluded, depressed and normal subjects: Effects of task difficulty and meaningful versus nonmeaningful material. Psychological Medicine, 27, 455–465. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ziegler, M. , Rief, W. , Werner, S.-M. , Mehl, S. , Lincoln, T.M. (in press). Hasty decision-making in a variety of tasks: Does it contribute to the development of delusions? Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar