Skip to main content
Log in

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins

  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated the contribution of genetic and environmental influences on the stability of aggressive behavior from early childhood to adolescence. Two developmental models, the simplex model and the common factor model, were tested to study the underlying processes of stability and change. Measures of aggressive behavior (AGG) were obtained from maternal CBCL data as part of a large ongoing longitudinal study of the Netherlands Twin Registers (NTR) and included data from 6488 three-year-old twin pairs, 5475 seven-year-old twin pairs, 2983 ten-year-old twin pairs, and 1509 twelve-year-old twin pairs. AGG showed moderate to high stability during childhood. The stability coefficients ranged from 0.41 to 0.77 across varying intervals. Averaged across boys and girls, genetic factors accounted for approximately 65% of the total stability. Longitudinal genetic analysis indicated a simplex model for genetic effects, which suggests a dynamic development process consisting of transmission of existing genetic effects interacting with new genetic influences. This is especially true at age 7, when the influence of new genetic factors was large. Shared environmental factors accounted for approximately 25% of phenotypic stability, and it seemed that a stable set of the same shared environmental factors underlay the development of AGG. Nonshared environmental factors, when important, are age specific. Sex-specific differences for stability were identified. For boys, genetic influences were greater, whereas for girls shared environmental factors were more important. These data support the idea that both genetic and environmental influences play a role in the stability of AGG from age 3 to 12.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Achenbach, T. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/ 4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/2-3 and 1992 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52: 317-322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, M., Boomsma, D. I., Rietveld, M. J. H., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Hudziak, J. A., and van den Oord, E. J. C. G. Disentangling genetic environmental, and rater effects on internalizing and externalizing problem behavior in 10-year-old twins. (submitted [a]).

  • Bartels, M., Hudziak, J. J., van den Oord, E. J. C. G., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Rietveld, M. J. H., and Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Cooccurrence of aggressive behavior and rule-breaking behavior at age 12: Multi-rater analyses. Behav. Genet. 33:607-622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, M., van den Oord, E. J., Hudziak, J. J., Rietveld, M. J. H., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., and Boomsma, D. I. Genetic and environmental mechanisms underlying stability and change in problem behaviors at ages 3, 7, 10, and 12. (submitted [b]).

  • Bergeman, C. S., and Seroczynski, A. D. (1998). Genetic and environmental influences on aggression and impulsivity. In M. Maes and E. F. Coccaro (Eds.), Neurobiology and clinical views on aggression and impulsivity (Vol. 5, pp. 63-80). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boomsma, D. I., and Molenaar, P. C. M. (1987). The genetic analysis of repeated measures. 1: Simplex models. Behav. Genet. 17:111-123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boomsma, D. I., Vink, J. M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., De Geus, E. J. C., Beem, L., and Mulder, E. J. C. M. (2002). Netherlands Twin Register: A focus on longitudinal research. Twin Res. 5:1-6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cardon, L. R., Fulker, D. W., DeFries, J. C., and Plomin, R. (1992). Continuity and change in general cognitive ability form 1 to 7 years of age. Dev. Psychol. 28:1-10.

    Google Scholar 

  • CBS (1993). Standaard Beroepenclassificatie 1992 [standardized classifications of occupations 1992]. Voorburg/Heerlen, The Netherlands: Central Bureau of Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Copeland, W., Stanger, C., and Hudziak, J. J. Parenting, family problems, and callous-unemotional traits predict internalizing and externalizing problems: A multi-group mediational analysis. (submitted).

  • Eley, T. C., Lichtenstein, P., and Stevenson, J. (1999). Sex differences in the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior: Results from two twin studies. Child Dev. 70:155-168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrington, D. P. (1995). The development of offending and antisocial behaviour from childhood: Key findings from the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 360:929-964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feinberg, M., and Hetherington, E. M. (2001). Differential parenting as a within-family variable. J. Family Psychol. 15:22-37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, H. H. (1991). A zygosity questionnaire for young twins: A research note. Behav. Genet. 21:257-270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L. (1954). A new approach to factor analysis: The radex. In P. F. Lazarsfeld (Ed.), Mathematical thinking in the social science (pp. 258-349). Glencoe, IL.: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath, A. C., Madden, P. A., and Martin, N. G. (1998). Assessing the effects of cooperation bias and attrition in behavioral genetic research using data-weighting. Behav. Genet. 28:415-427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstra, M. B., van der Ende, J., and Verhulst, F. C. (2000). Continuity and change of psychopathology from childhood into adulthood: A 14-year follow-up study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39:850-858.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudziak, J. A., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Bartel, M., Derks, E., and Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Individual differences in aggression in young children: Cross-sectional analyses in Dutch twins. Behav. Genet. 33:575-590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudziak, J. J., Rudiger, L. P., Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., and Todd, R. D. (2000). A twin study of inattentive, aggressive, and anxious/depressed behaviors. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39:469-476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, K. C., Prescott, C. A., and Kendler, K. S. (2002). Sex differences in the genetic and environmental influences on the development of antisocial behavior. Dev. Psychopathol. 14:395-416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kingston, L., and Prior, M. (1995). The development of patterns of stable, transient, and school-age onset aggressive behavior in young children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 34:348-358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koot, H. M. (1995). Longitudinal studies of general population and community samples. In F. Verhulst and H. M. Koot (Eds.), The epidemiology of child and adolescent psychopathology (pp. 337-365). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koot, H. M., van Den Oord, E. J., Verhulst, F. C., and Boomsma, D. I. (1997). Behavioral and emotional problems in young preschoolers: Cross-cultural testing of the validity of the Child Behavior Checklist/2-3. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 25: 183-196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, R. F., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A. Bleske, A., and Silva, P. A. (1998). Assortative mating for antisocial behavior: Developmental and methodological implications. Behav. Genet. 28: 173-186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., and Mrazek, D. A. (1999). Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament. Dev. Psychol. 35:189-204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Little, R. J. A., and Rubin, D. B. (1987). Statistical analysis with missing data. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loeber, R., and Hay, D. (1997). Key issues in the development of aggression and violence from childhood to early adulthood. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 48:371-410.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGue, M., Bacon, S., and Lykken, D. T. (1993). Personality stability and change in early adulthood: A behavioral genetic-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 29:96-109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles, D. R., and Carey, G. (1997). Genetic and environmental architecture of human aggression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72: 207-217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E. (1993). 'Life-cours-persistent’ and ‘adolescent-limited’ antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychol. Rev. 100:674-701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M. C., and Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., and Maes, H. H. (1999). Mx: Statistical Modeling: VCU Box 900126, Richmond, VA 23298: Dept. of Psychiatry (5th ed.).

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Connor, T. G., Neiderbiser, J. M., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., and Plomin, R. (1998). Genetic contributions to continuity, change, and co-occurrence of antisocial and depressive symptoms in adolescence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Disciplines 39:323-336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, N. L. (1991). Genetic and environmental factors in a developmental perspective. In D. Magnusson and L. R. Bergman (Eds.), Problems and methods in longitudinal research: Stability and change (pp. 236-249). Cambridge: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Hetherington, E. M., and Plomin, R. (2000). The relationship code: Deciphering genetic and social patterns in adolescent development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhee, S. H., and Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychol. Bull. 128:490-529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, M. J. H., Hudziak, J. A., Bartels, M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., and Boomsma, D. I. Heritability of attention problems in children. II. longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to 12. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry (in press).

  • Rietveld, M. J. H., VanderValk, J. C., Bongers, I. L., Stroet, T. M., Slagboom, P. E., and Boomsma, D. I. (2000). Zygosity diagnosis in young twins by parental report. Twin Res. 3:134-141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, S., Fulker, D. W., and Mrazek, D. A. (1995). Problem behavior in early and middle childhood: An initial behavior genetic analysis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Disciplines 36:1443-1458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanger, C., Achenbach, T. M., and Verhulst, F. C. (1997). Accelerated longitudinal comparisons of aggressive versus delinquent syndromes. Dev. Psychopathol. 9:43-58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J., Iacono, W. G., and McGue, M. (2000). Evidence for a genetic etiology of early-onset delinquency. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109:634-643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay, R. E. (2002). The origins of physical aggression. Int. Soc. Study Behav. Dev. Newslett. 2:4-6.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Oord, E. J., and Rowe, D. C. (1997). Continuity and change in children's social maladjustment: A developmental behavior genetic study. Dev. Psychol. 33:319-332.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Valk, J. C., van den Oord, E., Verhulst, F. C., and Boomsma, D. I. Genetic and environmental contributions to continuity and change of internalizing and externalizing problems during childhood. J. American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry (in press).

  • van der Valk, J. C., van den Oord, E. J., Verhulst, F. C., and Boomsma, D. I. (2001). Using parental ratings to study the etiology of 3-year-old twins' problem behaviors: Different views or rater bias? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42:921-931.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhulst, F., and van der Ende, J. (1995). The eight-year stability of problem behavior in an epidemiologic sample. Pediatr. Res. 38:612-617.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vierikko, E., Pulkkinen, L., Kaprio, J., Viken, R., and Rose, R. J. (2003). Sex differences in genetic and environmental effects on aggression. Aggress. Behav. 29:55-68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wothke, W. (2000). Longitudinal and multigroup modeling with missing data. In T. D. Little, K. U. Schnabel, and J. Baumert (Eds.), Modeling longitudinal and multilevel data: Practical issues, applied approaches and specific examples. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

van Beijsterveldt, C.E.M., Bartels, M., Hudziak, J.J. et al. Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins. Behav Genet 33, 591–605 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025735002864

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025735002864

Navigation