Skip to main content
Log in

Gender Differences in Attributions and Emotions in Helping Contexts

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Gender differences in the provision and receipt of emotional support may result from differences in the formation of responsibility and effort attributions in support-seeking interactions. Participants (N = 1,211, primarily middle-class, European American college students) read support-seeking scenarios that varied in support-seeker gender, responsibility for the problem, and effort to resolve the problem, as well as the problem itself, and completed measures of responsibility, effort attributions, and emotions (anger, sympathy). Results indicated qualified and subtle gender differences in attributions, emotions, and attribution--emotion associations, which are broadly consistent with the application of gendered moral orientations and instrumentality norms. These findings are discussed with respect to theorizing about gender differences in attribution processes and emotional support behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akiyama, H., Elliott, K., & Antonucci, T. C. (1996). Same-sex and cross-sex relationships. Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 51B(6), P374-P382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Auster, C. J., & Ohm, S. C. (2000). Masculinity and femininity in contemporary American society: A reevaluation using the Bem Sex-Role Inventory. Sex Roles, 43, 499–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Zur, H., & Zeidner, M. (1996). Gender differences in coping reactions under community crisis and daily routine conditions. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 331–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burda, P. C., Jr., Vaux, A., & Schill, T. (1984). Social support resources: Variation across sex and sex role. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 119–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caprara, G. V., Pastorelli, C., & Weiner, B. (1997). Linkagesbetween causal ascriptions, emotion, and behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 153–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, M. R., & Barbee, A. P. (2000). Social support. In C. Hendrick & S. S. Hendrick (Eds.), Close relationships:A sourcebook (pp. 273–286). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Crowley, M. (1986). Gender and helping behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 283–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social roletheory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123–174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • George, D. M., Carroll, P., Kersnick, R., & Calderon, K. (1998). Gender-related patterns of helping among friends. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 685–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, S. (1992). Message effects research. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, S., & Brashers, D. E. (1994). Random factors in ANOVA (Vol. 98). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaffee, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2000). Gender differences in moral orientation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 703–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jansz, J. (2000). Masculinity and restrictive emotionality. InA. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social psychologicalperspectives (pp. 166–186). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karasawa, K. (1991). The effects of onset and offset responsibility on affects and helping judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 482–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kojima, M. (1992). An analysis of attributional processes in helping behavior. Bulletin of the Tamagawa Guken Junior College for Women, 17, 57–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunkel, A. W., & Burleson, B. R. (1998). Social support and the emotional lives of men and women: An assessment of the different cultures perspective. In D. J. Canary & K. Dindia (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in communication (pp. 101–125). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunkel, A. W., & Burleson, B. R. (1999). Assessing explanations for sex differences in emotional support: A test of the different cultures and skill specialization accounts. Human Communication Research, 25, 307–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levant, R. F., Hirsch, L., Celentano, E., Cozza, T., Hill, S., MacEachern, M., et al. (1992). The male role: An investigation of norms and stereotypes. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 14, 325–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacGeorge, E. L. (1999). The influence of situational variation, associated attributions, and emotions on support providers' interaction goals [Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, 1999]. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 3201.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacGeorge, E. L. (2001). Support providers' interaction goals: The influence of attributions and emotions. Communication Monographs, 68, 72–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacGeorge, E. L., Clark, R. A., & Gillihan, S. (2001). Explaining sex differences in the evaluation of comforting messages: The mediating role of interaction goals. Communication Reports, 15, 17–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ptacek, J. T., Smith, R. E., & Dodge, K. L. (1994). Gender differences in coping with stress: When stressor and appraisals do not differ. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 421–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samter, W. (2001). How gender and cognitive complexity influence the provision of emotional support: A study of indirect effects. Communication Reports, 15, 5–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzer, R., & Weiner, B. (1991). Stigma controllability and coping as predictors of emotions and social support. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 133–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedants. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swim, J. K., & Sanna, L. J. (1996). He's skilled, she's lucky: A meta-analysis of observers' attributions for women's and men's successes and failures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 507–519.

    Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J. M. (1997). Changes in masculine and feminine traits over time: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 36, 305–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. T. (1997). Gendered lives: Communication, gender, and culture (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Workman, J. E., & Freeburg, E. W. (1999). An examination of date rape, victim dress, and perceiver variables within the context of attribution theory. Sex Roles, 41, 261–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zucker, G. S., & Weiner, B. (1993). Conservatism and perceptions of poverty: An attributional analysis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 925–943.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erina L. MacGeorge.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

MacGeorge, E.L. Gender Differences in Attributions and Emotions in Helping Contexts. Sex Roles 48, 175–182 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022459507110

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022459507110

Navigation