Skip to main content
Log in

Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Where American kindergartners are taughtletters and letter sounds, Germankindergartners are not; where American firstand second graders receive an eclectic blend ofwhole language, whole word and phonics-basedapproaches, their German counterparts aretaught by an intensive synthetic phonicsapproach. As a probe to the consequences ofthese pedagogical differences on the emergenceof phoneme awareness, this study administeredtwo tests of phoneme awareness tokindergarten-, first- and second-grade childrenin Germany and America, along with readingtests, the digit span test and a test of RANcolor naming ability. The American kindergartenchildren excelled on a phoneme identityjudgement and a phoneme deletion task that theGerman kindergartners found difficult. Theiradvantage held equally whether the manipulatedsound was a syllable onset or the initial partof a consonant cluster. The first and secondgraders surpassed the kindergartners in bothcountries; however, the German first and secondgraders equaled their American peers on bothtasks and both types of units. In addition,the German children were more accurate decodersof pseudowords by the end of second grade, andthe association between phoneme awareness andGerman decoding ability was weaker. Anincreased emphasis on phonics and the greatertransparency of the German alphabet arediscussed as possible factors in the decodingexcellence of the German second graders and itsdecreased association with phoneme awareness.The contrast between the American and Germankindergartners and the equivalence of the firstand second graders in the two countries areconsistent with a view that phoneme awarenessdevelops primarily as a product of literacyexposure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alegria, J. Pignot, E. & Morais, J. (1982). Phonetic analysis of speech and memory codes in beginning readers, Memory & Cognition 10: 451–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R.W. (1991). Proto-literacy, literacy, and the acquisition of phonological awareness, Learning and Individual Differences 3: 243–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R.W. (1994). The sound-to-spelling connection: Orthographic activation in auditory word recognition and its implications for the acquisition of phonological awareness and literacy skills. In V.W. Berninger (ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge. I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 219–242). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. & Shankweiler, D. (1991). Phonological Processes in Literacy. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J.A. (1994). Phonological sensitivity in novice readers and nonreaders, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 58: 134–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., Genesee, F. & Caravolas (1997). A cross-linguistic study of early literacy acquisition. In B. Blachman (ed.), Dyslexia and its treatment (pp. 45–62). Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. & Goswami, U. (1987). Beyond grapheme-phoneme correspondence, Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition 7: 439–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burhanpurkar, A. & Barron, R.W. (1997). Origins of phonological awareness in pre-readers: Roles of language, memory and proto-literacy. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC.

  • Byrne, B. (1998). The foundation of literacy: The child's acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1991). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children, Journal of Educational Psychology 83: 451–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M. & Bruck, M. (1993). The effect of oral and written language input on children's phonological awareness: a cross-linguistic study, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 55: 1–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connelly, V., Johnston, R.S. & Thompson (1999). The influence of instructional approaches on reading procedures. In G.B. Thompson & T. Nicholson (eds.), Learning to read: Beyond phonics and whole language (pp. 103–123). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E.V. (1995). The young word maker: A case study of innovation in the child's lexicon. In E. Wanner and L.R. Gleitman (eds.), Language acquisition, the state of the art (pp. 390–425). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I.Y., Katz, L. & Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children, Applied Psycholinguistics 9: 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M.B. & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children, Cortex 10: 186–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A.Y. & Oeney, B. (1999). A cross-linguistic comparison of phonological awareness and word recognition, Reading & Writing 11: 281–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. & Wilce, L.S. (1980). The influence of orthography on reader's conceptualization of the phonemic structure of word, Applied Psycholinguistics 1: 371–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis, Reading & Writing 8: 453–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C. & Peterson, D.K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical item, Reading Research Quarterly 33: 36–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A.E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 97–117). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., Wimmer, H. & Landerl, K. (1998). Differences in phonological recoding in Germanand English-speaking children, Scientific Studies of Reading 2: 31–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S.E., Hitch, G.J., Service, E. & Martin, A.J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children, Developmental Psychology 33: 966–979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S.E., Service, E., Hitch, G.J., Adams, A. & Martin, A.J. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship, Applied Cognitive Psychology 13: 65–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, R.S., Anderson, M. & Holligan, C. (1996). Knowledge of the alphabet and explicit awareness of phonemes in pre-readers: The nature of the relationship, Reading and Writing 8: 217–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I.Y., Shankweiler, S., Fischer, F.W. & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 18: 201–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C.J., Burgess, S.R., Anthony, J.J. & Barker, T.A. (1998). Development of phonological sensitivity in 2-to 5-year old children, Journal of Educational Psychology 90: 294–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukatela, K., Carello, C., Shankweiler, D. & Liberman, I.Y. (1995). Phonological awareness in illiterates: Observations from Serbo-Croatian, Applied Psycholinguistics 16: 463–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V.A. (1986). Phonological awareness: The role of reading experience, Cognition 24: 65–92. [Also published under the same title in: P. Bertelson (ed.) (1987) The onset of literacy. Cambridge: MIT Press.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J.L. & Walley, A.C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J.L. Metsala and L.C. Ehri (eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J. & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously?, Cognition 7: 323–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. & Taylor, S. (1998). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 71: 3–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naslund, J.C., Schneider, W. & Van Den Broek, P. (1997). Beginning reading in Germany and the US: A comparison of phonological segmentation, decoding, lexical access and comprehension. In C.K. Leong & R.M. Joshi (eds.), Cross-language studies of learning to read and spell (pp. 103–120). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Read, C., Zhang, Y., Nie, H. & Ding, B. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing, Cognition 24: 31–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosner, J. & Simon, D.P. (1971). The auditory analysis test: An initial report, Journal of Learning Disabilities 4: 384–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. & Näslund, J.C. (1993). The impact of early metalinguistic competencies and memory capacity on reading and spelling in elementary school: Results of the Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC), European Journal of Psychology of Education 8: 273–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Roth, E. & Ennemoser, M. (2000). Training phonological skills and letter knowledge in children-at risk for dyslexia: A comparison of three kindergarten intervention programs, Journal of Educational Psychology 92: 284–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scanlon, D.M. & Vellutino, F.R. (1997). A comparison of the instructional backgrounds and cognitive profiles of poor, average, and good readers who were initially identified as at risk for reading failure, Scientific Studies of Reading 1: 191–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy, Reading Research Quarterly 21: 360–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, G.B. & Johnston, R.S. (2000). Are nonword and other phonological deficits indicative of a failed reading process? Reading & Writing 12: 63–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J.K. & Davis, C. (1996). Individual difference variables that predict response to training in phonological awareness, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 63: 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. & Zukowski, A. (1996). Children's sensitivity to syllables, onsets, rimes, and phonemes, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 61: 193–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W.E. & Nesdale, A.R. (1985). Phonetic segmentation skill and beginning reading, Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 417–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K. & Torgesen, J.K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills, Psychological Bulletin 101: 192–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, J.K. & Rashotte, C. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study, Developmental Psychology 30: 73–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, J.K., Rashotte, C.A. & Hecht, S.A. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study, Developmental Psychology 33: 468–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walley, A.C. (1993). The role of vocabulary development in children's spoken word recognition and segmentation ability, Developmental Review 13: 286–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weschler, D. (1967). Preschool and primary scale of intelligence. Cleveland, Ohio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children, Cognition 51: 91–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. & Hummer, P. (1990). How German speaking first graders read and spell: Doubts on the importance of the logographic stage, Applied Psycholinguistics 11: 349–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., Landerl, K., Linortner, R. & Hummer, P. (1991). The relationship of phonemic awareness to reading acquisition. More consequence than precondition but still important. Cognition 40: 219–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H. & Landerl, K. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis and learning to read a more regular orthography. Unpublished manuscript, University of Salzburg.

  • Woodcock, R.W. (1998). Woodcock reading mastery tests C revised. Circle Pines, Minnesota: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yopp, H.K. (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests, Reading Research Quarterly 23: 159–177.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Virginia Mann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mann, V., Wimmer, H. Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing 15, 653–682 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020984704781

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020984704781

Navigation