Skip to main content
Log in

Gender Differences in Self-Perception and Negative Recall Biases

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated gender differences inthe accuracy of self-perceptions and whetherself-perception biases are related to negative recallbiases. Participants were 275 female and 213 malecollege students. Approximately 10% of the participantswere minorities (mostly African American and Asian). Ona masculine task, gender differences in self-perceptionswere found for three measures of accuracy: The accuracy of self-evaluations, calibration,and response bias. Females underestimated theirperformance, were less well calibrated, and showed amore conservative response bias than did males. Ashypothesized, no gender differences in the accuracy ofself-evaluations were found for feminine and neutraltasks. Participants' expectancies mediated the genderdifferences in post task self-evaluations ofperformance. In addition, evidence for a negative recallbias was found. Females were more likely than males torecall their mistakes even with performance and accuracyof self-evaluations controlled. The implications of females' greater self-perception biases onmasculine tasks are discussed and suggestions for futureresearch are made.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1982). Learned helplessness, depression, and the illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 1114–1126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, L., Willoughby, T. L., & Calkins, E. V. (1985). Self-evaluation in undergraduate medical education: A longitudinal perspective. Journal of Medical Education, 60, 21–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellew, M., & Hill, A. B. (1990). Negative recall bias as a predictor of susceptibility to induced depressive mood. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 471–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beyer, S. (1990). Gender differences in the accuracy of self-evaluations of performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 960–970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beyer, S. (1995). Maternal employment and children's academic achievement: Parenting style as mediating variable. Developmental Review, 15, 212–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beyer, S., & Bowden, E. M. (1997) Gender differences in self-perceptions: Convergent evidence from three measures of accuracy and bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 157–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boggiano, A. K., Main, D. S., & Katz, P. A. (1988). Children's preference for challenge: The role of perceived competence and control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 134–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1993). Convergence of stranger ratings of personality and intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and measured intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 546–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B., & Mathews, A. (1983). Negative self-schemata in clinical depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22, 173–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breslow, R., Kocsis, J., & Belkin, B. (1981). Contribution of the depressive perspective to memory function in depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 227–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. W., Andrews, B., Bifulco, A., & Veiel, H. (1990). Self-esteem and depression: I. Measurement issues and prediction of onset. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 25, 200–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. W., Bifulco, A., & Andrews, B. (1990). Self-esteem and depression: III. Aetiological issues. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 25, 235–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (rev. ed.). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colvin, C. R., & Block, J. (1994). Do positive illusions foster mental health? An examination of the Taylor and Brown formulation. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 3–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crocker, J., Alloy, L. B., & Kayne, N. T. (1988). Attributional style, depression, and perceptions of consensus for events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 840–846.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutrona, C. E., Cole, V., Colangelo, N., Assouline, S. G., & Russell, D. W. (1994). Perceived parental social support and academic achievement: An attachment theory perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 369–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, G. C., & Lishman, W. A. (1984). Depression, recognition-memory and hedonic tone: A signal detection analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 376–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D. (1995). Trait importance and modifiability as factors influencing self-assessment and self-enhancement motives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1297–1306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1995). The science and politics of comparing women and men. American Psychologist, 50, 145–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 5–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 429–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabriel, M. T., Critelli, J. W., & Ee, J. S. (1994). Narcissistic illusions in self-evaluations of intelligence and attractiveness. Journal of Personality, 62, 143–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glass, D. C., McKnight, D., & Valdimarsdottir, H. (1993). Depression, burnout, and perceptions of control in hospital nurses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 147–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H. (1983). Perception and recall of interpersonal feedback: Negative bias in depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7, 399–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grolnick, W. S., & Slowiaczek, M. L. (1994). Parents' involvement in children's schooling: A multidimensional conceptualization and motivational model. Child Development, 65, 237–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurman, E. B., & Balban, M. (1990). Self-evaluations of physical attractiveness as a function of self-esteem and defensiveness. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 5, 575–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., Miklowitz, D. J., & Dyck, D. G. (1986). Stability and severity parameters of depressive self-schema encoding. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 4, 23–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannover, B. (1991). Zur Unterrepräsentanz von Mädchen in Naturwissenschaften und Technik: Psychologische Prädiktoren der Fach-und Berufswahl. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 5, 169–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1993). Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 904–915.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S., & Connell, J. P. (1984). A model of children's achievement and related self-perceptions of competence, control, and motivational orientation. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 3). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hochhaus, L. (1972). A table for the calculation of d' and β. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 375–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S., & Frost, L. A. (1993). Meta-analysis in the psychology of women. In F. L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janman, K. (1987). A-level expectancies and university aspirations of males and females. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 57, 289–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janoff-Bulman, R. (1989). The benefits of illusions, the threat of disillusionment, and the limitations of inaccuracy. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 158–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception: Individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 206–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245–1267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy. Psychological Review, 103, 490–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). The psychology of being “right”: The problem of accuracy in social perception and cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 395–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuiper, N. A., Olinger, L. J., MacDonald, M. R., & Shaw, B. F. (1985). Self-schema processing of depressed and nondepressed content: The effects of vulnerability to depression. Social Cognition, 3, 77–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurman, J., & Sriram, N. (1997). Self-enhancement, generality of self-evaluation, and affectivity in Israel and Singapore. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 421–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, C. A., Charleston, S. I., & Zieles, A. S. (1996). Individual-related and gender-related differences in indoor wayfinding. Environment and Behavior, 28, 204–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., Mischel, W., Chaplin, W. C., & Barton, R. (1980). Social competence and depression: The role of illusory self-perceptions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 203–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein, S., Fischhoff, B., & Phillips, L. D. (1982). Calibration of probabilities: The state of the art to 1980. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindeman, M., Sundvik, L., & Rouhiainen, P. (1995). Under-or overestimation of self? Person variables and self-assessment accuracy. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 10, 123–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, G. G., & Lishman, W. A. (1975). Effect of depression on the speed of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Psychological Medicine, 5, 173–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., Crane, M., Bernstein, S., & Siladi, M. (1982). Self-schemas and gender. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 38–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowall, J. (1984). Recall of pleasant and unpleasant words in depressed participants. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 401–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mura, R. (1987). Sex-related differences in expectations of success in undergraduate mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18, 15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, J. F., Lynch, P. B., & Bakal, D. A. (1989). Dysthymic and hypomanic self-referent effects associated with depressive illness and recovery. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 13, 195–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 259–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, D. (1984). The illusion of incompetence among academically competent children. Child Development, 55, 2000–2016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, D. (1987). Socialization of perceived academic competence among highly competent children. Child Development, 58, 1308–1320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powel, W. D., & Nusbaum, N. (1990, June). Development of accurate error monitoring. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Western Psychological Association, Los Angeles, CA.

  • Pyszczynski, T., Hamilton, J. C., Herring, F. H., & Greenberg, J. (1989). Depression, self-focused attention, and the negative memory bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 351–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, J., & Mulhern, G. (1995). Gender differences in self-estimated IQ: The need for care in interpreting group data. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 189–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Risucci, D. A., Tortolani, A. J., & Ward, R. J. (1989). Ratings of surgical residents by self, supervisors, and peers. Surgery, Gynecology, and Obstetrics, 169, 519–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, T.-A. (1991). Gender and the influence of evaluations on self-assessments in achievement settings. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 297–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, T.-A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1989). Sex differences in reactions to evaluative feedback. Sex Roles, 21, 725–747.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, T.-A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1994). Gender comparisons on responsiveness to others' evaluations in achievement settings. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 221–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rude, S. S., Krantz, S. E., & Rosenhan, D. L. (1988). Distinguishing the dimensions of valence and belief consistency in depressive and nondepressive information processing. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 12, 391–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rustemeyer, R., & Jubel, A. (1996). Geschlechtsspezifische Unterschiede im Unterrichtsfach Mathematik hinsichtlich der Fähigkeitseinschätzung, Leistungserwartung, Attribution sowie im Lernaufwand und im Interesse. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 10, 13–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shrauger, J. S., & Kelly, R. J. (1988). Global self-evaluation and changes in self-description as a function of information discrepancy and favorability. Journal of Personality, 56, 709–728.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shrauger, J. S., & Terbovic, M. L. (1976). Self-evaluation and assessments of performance by self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 564–572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, C. R. (1989). Reality negotiation: From excuses to hope and beyond. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 130–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stangor, C. (1988). Stereotype accessibility and information processing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 694–708.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swann, W. B., Jr., & Read, S. J. (1981). Self-verification processes: How we sustain our self-conceptions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 351–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 21–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Neter, E., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-evaluation processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1278–1287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, N. T. (1976). Self-esteem and sources of cognitive bias in the evaluation of past performance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 966–975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winer, B. J., Brown, D. R., & Michels, K. M. (1991). Statistical principles in experimental design (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Beyer, S. Gender Differences in Self-Perception and Negative Recall Biases. Sex Roles 38, 103–133 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018768729602

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018768729602

Keywords

Navigation