Skip to main content
Log in

Environmental reference systems for large-scale spaces

  • Published:
Spatial Cognition and Computation

Abstract

Human navigation in well-known environments is guided by stored memory representations of spatial information. In three experiments (N = 43) we investigated the role of different spatial reference systems when accessing information about familiar objects at different locations in the city in which the participants lived. Our results indicate that two independent reference systems underly the retrieval of spatial knowledge. Environmental characteristics, e.g., the streets at an intersection, determine which headings are easier to imagine at a given location and lead to differences in accessibility of spatial information (orientation-specific behavior). In addition, access to spatial information depends on the relative direction of a location with respect to the imagined heading, such that information about locations imagined in front of oneself is easier to access than about locations towards the back. This influence of an egocentric reference system was found for environmental knowledge as well as map-based knowledge. In light of these reference system effects, position-dependent models of spatial memory for large-scale environments are discussed. To account for the simultaneous effect of an environmental and an egocentric reference system, we present a 2-level model of spatial memory access.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Berthoz, A. (1991). Reference Frames for the Perception and Control of Movement. In J. Paillard (ed.), Brain and Space. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 81–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach, E. and Luzzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space, Cortex 14: 129–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R.W. (1979). Memory for Urban Geography, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 31: 147–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, N. and Tversky, B. (1990). Searching Imagined Environments, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 119: 63–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R.G. (1999). Human Wayfinding and Cognitive Maps. In R.G. Golledge (ed.), Wayfinding Behavior: Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Processes. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, pp. 5–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R.G. and Stimson, R.J. (1997). Spatial Behavior: A Geographic Perspective.New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hintzman, D.L., O'Dell, C.S. and Arndt, D.R. (1981). Orientation in Cognitive Maps, Cognitive Psychology 13(2): 149–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirtle, S.C. and Jonides, J. (1985). Evidence of Hierarchies in Cognitive Maps, Memory and Cognition 13: 208–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ittelson, W.H. (1970). Perception of the Large-scale Environment, Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences, Series II 32(7): 807–815.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatzky, R.L. (1998). Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Representations: Definitions, Distinctions, and Interconnection. In C. Freksa, C. Habel and K.F. Wender (eds.), Spatial Cognition: An Interdisciplinary Approach to Representing and Processing Spatial Knowledge. Berlin: Springer, pp. 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S.M. (1994). Image and Brain. Cambridge: MIT-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M., Jankovic, I.N. and Palij, M. (1982). Principles of Spatial Problem Solving, Journal of Experimental Psychology General 111(2): 157–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S.C. (1996). Frames of Reference and Molyneux's Question: Crosslinguistic Evidence. In P. Bloom, M.A. Peterson, L. Nadel and M.F. Garrett (eds.), Language and Space: Language, Speech and Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 109–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loomis, J.M., da Silva, J.A., Fujita, N. and Fukusima, S.S. (1992). Visual Space Perception and Visually Directed Action, Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance 18(4): 906–921.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, T.P., Hardy, J.K. and Hirtle, S.C. (1989). Subjective Hierarchies in Spatial Memory, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15: 211–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montello, D.R. (1991). Spatial Orientation and the Angularity of Urban Routes: A Field Study, Environment and Behavior 23(1): 47–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poucet, B. (1993). Spatial Cognitive Maps in Animals: New Hypotheses on Their Structure and Neural Mechanisms, Psychological Review 100(2): 163–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Presson, C.C. and Hazelrigg, M.D. (1984). Building Spatial Representations through Primary and Secondary Learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 10: 716–722.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieser, J.J. (1989). Access to Knowledge of Spatial Structure at Novel Points of Observation, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15: 1157–1165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., McNamara, T.P., Shelton, A.L. and Carr, W.S. (1998). Mental Representations of Large and Small Spatial Layouts are Orientation-Dependent, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition 24: 215–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schölkopf, B. and Mallot, H.A. (1995). View-Based Cognitive Mapping and Path Planning, Adaptive Behavior 3(3): 311–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shelton, A.L. and McNamara, T.P. (1997). Multiple Views of Spatial Memory, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 4: 102–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shelton, A.L. and McNamara, T.P. (submitted). Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory.

  • Shepard, R.N. and Cooper, L.A. (1982). Mental Images and Their Transformations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sholl, M.J. (1987). Cognitive Maps as Orienting Schemata, Journal of Experimental Psycho-logy: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15: 110–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sholl, M.J. (1995). The Representation and Retrieval of Map and Environment Knowledge, Geographical Systems 2: 177–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sholl, M.J. and Nolin, T.L. (1997). Orientation Specificity in Representations of Place, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 23(6): 1494–1507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, A.L. and Coupe, P. (1978). Distortions in Judged Spatial Relations, Cognitive Psychology 10: 422–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarr, M.J. (1995). Rotating Objects to Recognize Them: A Case Study on the Role of Viewpoint Dependency in the Recognition of Three-Dimensional Objects, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 2(1): 55–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (1981). Distortions in Memory for Maps, Cognitive Psychology 13: 407–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, R.F. (1999). Representing a Stable Environment by Egocentric Updating and Invariant Representation, Spatial Cognition and Computation 1(4): 431–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, S (in preparation). The Effects of Egocentric and Allocentric Frames of Reference on the Mental Representation of Extrapersonal Space.

  • Werner, S., Krieg-Brückner, B. and Herrmann, T. (2000). Modelling Navigational Knowledge by Route Graphs. In C. Freksa and C. Habel (eds.), Spatial Cognition II. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Werner, S., Schmidt, K. Environmental reference systems for large-scale spaces. Spatial Cognition and Computation 1, 447–473 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010095831166

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010095831166

Navigation