Skip to main content
Log in

The Declarative/Procedural Model of Lexicon and Grammar

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Our use of language depends upon two capacities: a mental lexicon of memorized words and a mental grammar of rules that underlie the sequential and hierarchical composition of lexical forms into predictably structured larger words, phrases, and sentences. The declarative/procedural model posits that the lexicon/grammar distinction in language is tied to the distinction between two well-studied brain memory systems. On this view, the memorization and use of at least simple words (those with noncompositional, that is, arbitrary form-meaning pairings) depends upon an associative memory of distributed representations that is subserved by temporal-lobe circuits previously implicated in the learning and use of fact and event knowledge. This “declarative memory” system appears to be specialized for learning arbitrarily related information (i.e., for associative binding). In contrast, the acquisition and use of grammatical rules that underlie symbol manipulation is subserved by frontal/basal-ganglia circuits previously implicated in the implicit (nonconscious) learning and expression of motor and cognitive “skills” and “habits” (e.g., from simple motor acts to skilled game playing). This “procedural” system may be specialized for computing sequences. This novel view of lexicon and grammar offers an alternative to the two main competing theoretical frameworks. It shares the perspective of traditional dual-mechanism theories in positing that the mental lexicon and a symbol-manipulating mental grammar are subserved by distinct computational components that may be linked to distinct brain structures. However, it diverges from these theories where they assume components dedicated to each of the two language capacities (that is, domain-specific) and in their common assumption that lexical memory is a rote list of items. Conversely, while it shares with single-mechanism theories the perspective that the two capacities are subserved by domain-independent computational mechanisms, it diverges from them where they link both capacities to a single associative memory system with broad anatomic distribution. The declarative/procedural model, but neither traditional dual- nor single-mechanism models, predicts double dissociations between lexicon and grammar, with associations among associative memory properties, memorized words and facts, and temporal-lobe structures, and among symbol-manipulation properties, grammatical rule products, motor skills, and frontal/basal-ganglia structures. In order to contrast lexicon and grammar while holding other factors constant, we have focused our investigations of the declarative/procedural model on morphologically complex word forms. Morphological transformations that are (largely) unproductive (e.g., in go—went, solemn—solemnity) are hypothesized to depend upon declarative memory. These have been contrasted with morphological transformations that are fully productive (e.g., in walk—walked, happy—happiness), whose computation is posited to be solely dependent upon grammatical rules subserved by the procedural system. Here evidence is presented from studies that use a range of psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic approaches with children and adults. It is argued that converging evidence from these studies supports the declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Alexander, G. E., Crutcher, M. D., & DeLong, M. R. (1990). Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: Parallel substrates for motor oculomotor 'prefrontal' and 'limbic' functions. In H. B. M. Uylings & C. G. V. Eden & J. P. C. DeBruin & M. A. Corner & M. G. P. Feenstra (Eds.), Progress on brain research (Vol. 85, pp. 119-146). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., & Strick, P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9, 357-381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, M. P. (1997). Aphasia: Clinical and anatomic aspects. In T. E. Feinberg & M. J. Farah (Eds.), Behavioral neurology and neuropsychology (pp. 133-150). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, S. R. (1992). A-morphous morphology (Vol. 62). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Appell, J., Kertesz, A., & Fisman, M. (1982). A study of language functioning in Alzheimer's patients. Brain and Language, 17, 73-91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, S. E., Hyman, B. T., Flory, J., Damasio, A. R., & Hoesen, G. W. V. (1991). The topographical and neuroanatomical distribution of neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques in the cerebral cortex of patients with Alzheimer's disease. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 103-116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badecker, W., & Caramazza, A. (1987). The analysis and morphological errors in a case of acquired dyslexia. Brain and language, 32, 278-305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badecker, W., & Caramazza, A. (1991). Morphological composition in the lexical output system. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 335-367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, S. E., & Rugg, M. D. (1989). Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of faces. Neuropsychologia, 27, 913-922.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, S. E., & Rugg, M. D. (1990). Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of pictures. Brain and Cognition, 14, 201-212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1989). Functionalism and the competition model. In B. MacWhinney & E. Bates (Eds.), The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing (pp. 3-73). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayles, K. A. (1982). Language function in senile dementia. Brain and Language, 16, 265-280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, W. W., Winn, P., Adams, R. L., Allen, E. W., Wilson, D. A., Prince, J. R., Olson, K. A., Dean, K., & Littleford, D. (1994). Preserved cognitive skills in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology, 51, 1040-1046.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, W. W., Zavadil, K. D., Bailly, R. C., Rixen, G. J., Zavadil, L. E., Farnham, N., & Fisher, L. (1988). Preserved musical skill in a severely demented patient. International Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 10(4), 158-164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, T., Trauner, D., & Doherty, S. (1990). Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams Syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Supplement, 6, 115-125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergida, R., O'Craven, K., Savoy, R. L., & Ullman, M. T. (1998) fMRI double dissociations in frontal and temporal regions between regular and irregular past tense production. Paper presented at the 5th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, California.

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1992). The underlying nature of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 3-66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bromberg, H., Ullman, M. T., Coppola, M., Marcus, G., Kelly, K., & Levine, K. (1994). A dissociation of memory and grammar: Evidence from Williams Syndrome. 18th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, Massachusetts.

  • Bybee, J. L., & Moder, C. L. (1983). Morphological classes as natural categories. Language, 59, 251-270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J. L., & Slobin, D. I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language, 58g, 265-289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappa, S., & Ullman, M. T. (1998). A neural dissociation in Italian verbal morphology. Paper presented at the 5th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, California.

  • Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clahsen, H. (1999). Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(6), 991-1060.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clahsen, H., & Almazan, M. (1998). Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition, 68, 167-198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cottrell, G. W., & Plunkett, K. (1991). Learning the past tense in a recurrent Network: Acquiring the mapping from meaning to sounds, Proceedings of the 13th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 328-333). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curran, T., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). Amnesia: Cognitive neuropsychological aspects. In T. E. Feinberg & M. J. Farah (Eds.), Behavioral neurology and neuropsychology (pp. 463-471). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cushman, L. A., & Caine, E. D. (1987). A controlled study of processing of semantic and syntactic information in Alzheimer's disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2, 283-292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daugherty, K., & Seidenberg, M. (1992, April). Rules or connections? The past tense revisited. Paper presented at the Milwaukee Rules Conference, Milwaukee, Wisconsin.

  • De Renzi, E. (1989). Apraxia. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 2, pp. 245-263). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubois, B., Boller, F., Pillon, B., & Agid, Y. (1991). Cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 5, pp. 195-240). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J., Bates, E., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J., & Grossman, M. (1997). Semantic memory impairments. In I. E. Feinberg & M. J. Farah (Eds.), Behavioral neurology and neuropsychology (pp. 473-477). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1985). Precis of the modularity of mind. Behavioral and brain sciences, 8, 1-42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, P. T., Mintum, M. A., Reiman, E. M., & Raichle, M. E. (1988). Enhanced detection of focal brain responses using intersubject averaging and changing-distribution analysis of subtracted PET images. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 8, 642-653.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A., & von Cramon, D. Y. (1998). First-pass versus second-pass parsing processes in a Wernicke's and a Broca's aphasic: Electrophysiological evidence for a double dissociation. Brain and Language, 62(3), 311-341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fujiwara, M., & Ullman, M. T. (1999). The computation of default suffixation in Japanese adjectival past tense formation. Paper presented at the 12th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, CUNY, New York.

  • Gabrieli, J. D. E., Corkin, S., Mickel, S. F., & Growdon, J. H. (1993). Intact acquisition and long-term retention of mirror-tracing skill in Alzheimer's disease and in global amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 899-910.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H. (1993). Understanding aphasia. San Diego, California: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, K. S., & Hodges, J. R. (1997). Differentiating the roles of the hippocampal complex and the neocortex in long-term memory storage: Evidence from the study of semantic dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 11, 77-89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel, A. M. (1995). Building action repertoires: Memory and learning functions of the basal ganglia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 733-741.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, M., Say, T., Kleingers, M., Mu¨nte, T. F., & Clahsen, H. (1998). Human brain potentials to violations in morphologically complex Italian words. Neuroscience Letters, 241, 83-86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Gollomp, S., Stern, M. B., Reivich, M., Morrison, D., Alavi, A., & Hurtig, H. I. (1993). Cognitive and physiological substrates of impaired sentence processing in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 480-498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Gollomp, S., Stern, M. B., Vernon, G., & Hurtig, H. I. (1991). Sentence comprehension and praxis deficits in Parkinson's disease. Neurology, 41, 1620-1626.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M., Carvell, S., & Peltzer, L. (1993). The sum and substance of it: The appreciation of mass and count qualifiers in Parkinson's disease. Brain and Language, 44, 351-384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Stern, M. B., Gollomp, S., & Hurtig, H. I. (1992). Sentence comprehension in Parkinson's disease: The role of attention and memory. Brain and Language, 42, 347-384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M., Payer, F., Onishi, K., D'Esposito, M., Morrison, D., Sadek, A., & Alavi, A. (1998). Language comprehension and regional cerebral defects in frontotemporal degeneration and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 50, 157-163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagoort, P., & Kutus, M. (1995). Electrophysiological insights into language deficits. In R. J. Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 10, pp. 105-134). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection, The view from building 20. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M., & Mohanan, K. P. (1985). Segmental phonology of modern English. Linguistic Inquiry, 16, 57-116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, M., & Elman, J. (1995). Learning and morphological change. Cognition, 56, 61-98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, M., Elman, J. L., & Daugherty, K. G. (1995). Default generalization in connectionist networks. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 601-630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y., Yeo, R. A., & Marder, E. (1990). Procedural memory in Parkinson's disease: Impaired motor but not visuoperceptual Learning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 323-339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hier, D. B., Hagenlocker, K., & Shindler, A. G. (1985). Language disintegration in dementia: Effects of etiology and severity. Brain and Language, 25, 117-133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoard, J. E., & Sloat, C. (1973). English irregular verbs. Language, 49, 107-120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (1997). Semantic memory disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 68-72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., & Funnell, E. (1992). Semantic dementia. Brain, 115, 1783-1806.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, J. E., & Strick, P. L. (1993). Multiple output channels in the basal ganglia. Science, 259, 819-821.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurst, J. A., Baraister, M., Auger, E., Graham, F., & Norell, S. (1990). An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32, 352-355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Illes, J. (1989). Neurolinguistic features of spontaneous language production dissociate three forms of neurogenerative disease: Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's. Brain and Language, 37, 628-642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Illes, J., Metter, E. J., Hanson, W. R., & Iritani, S. (1988). Language production in Parkinson's disease: Acoustic and linguistic considerations. Brain and Language, 33, 146-160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Indefrey, P., Brown, C., Hagoort, P., Herzog, H., Sach, M., & Seitz, R. J. (1997). A PET study of cerebral activation patterns induced by verb inflection. NeuroImage, 5, S548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irigaray, L. (1973). Le Langage des Dements. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, J. J., Lockwood, A. H., Kemmerer, D. L., Van Valin Jr., R. D., Murphy, B. W., & Khalak, H. G. (1996). A positron emission tomographic study of regular and irregular verb morphology in English. Language, 72, 451-497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Impairments in verb morphology after brain injury: A connectionist model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 96, p7592-7597.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kempler, D., Curtiss, S., & Jackson, C. (1987). Syntactic preservation in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 343-350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P., Friedman, J., & Feldman, L. S. (1990). Syntax comprehension deficits in Parkinson's disease. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178, 360-365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P., Kako, E., Friedman, J., Tajchman, G., Feldman, L. S., & Jiminez, E. B. (1992). Speech production, syntax comprehension, and cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. Brain and Language, 43, 169-189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ling, C. X., & Marinov, M. (1993). Answering the connectionist challenge: A symbolic model of learning the past tenses of English verbs. Cognition, 49, 235-296.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). Lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101, 676-703.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B., & Bates, E. (Eds.). (1989). The cross-linguistic study of sentence processing. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B., & Leinbach, J. (1991). Implementations are not conceptualizations: Revising the verb learning model. Cognition, 40, 121-157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchman, V. A. (1993). Constraints on plasticity in a connectionist model of the English past tense. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 215-234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchman, V. A. (1997). Children's productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighborhood structure. Cognitive Science, 21, 283-304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F., Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R., & Pinker, S. (1995). German inflection: The exception that proves the rule. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 189-256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M. T., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57, 1-165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marin, O. S. M., Saffran, E. M., & Schwartz, M. F. (1976). Dissociations of language in aphasia: Implications for normal function. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280, pp. 868-884.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (1997). Dissociating types of mental computation. Nature, 387, 592-594.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungeleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1996). Neural correlates of categoryspecific knowledge. Nature (London), 379, 649-652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mishkin, M., Malamut, B., & Bachevalier, J. (1984). Memories and habits: Two neural systems. In G. Lynch & J. L. McGaugh & N. W. Weinburger (Eds.), Neurobiology of learning and memory (pp. 65-77). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdoch, B. E., Chenery, H. J., Wilks, V., & Boyle, R.(1987). Language disorders in dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Brain and Language, 31, 122-137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natsopoulos, D., Katsarou, Z., Bostantzopoulou, S., Grouios, G., Mentenopoulos, G., & Logothetis, J. (1991). Strategies in comprehension of relative clauses by Parkinsonian patients. Cortex, 27, 255-268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nebes, R. D. (1989). Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 377-394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neville, H., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I., & Garrett, M. F. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 151-165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1981). Computer science as empirical inquiry: Symbol and search. In J. Haugeland (Ed.), Mind design: Philosophy psychology artificial intelligence (pp. 35-66). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, A., Izvorski, R., Davis, L., Neville, H., & Ullman, M. T. (1999). Distinct electrophysiological patterns in the processing of regular and irregular verbs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Supplement, 47.

  • Newman, A., Neville, H., & Ullman, M. T. (1998). Neural processing of inflectional morphology: An event-related potential study of English past tense. Paper presented at the 5th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, California.

  • Nicholas, M., Obler, L. K., Albert, M. L., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1985). Empty speech in Alzheimer's disease and fluent aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 405-410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nobre, A. C., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (1994). Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe. Nature (London), 372, 260-263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obler, L. (1981). Review of Le Langage des dements. The Hague: Mouton, 1973. In Brain and Language, 12, 375-386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penke, M., Weyerts, H., Gross, M., Zander, E., Mu¨nte, T. F., & Clahsen, H. (1997). How the brain processes complex words: an event-related potential study of German verb inflections. Cognitive Brain Research, 6, 37-52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science, 253, 530-535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: The ingredients of language. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1988). On Language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73-193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1991). Regular and irregular morphology and the psychological status of rules of grammar. Berkeley Linguistics Society, 17, 230-251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1992). Regular and irregular morphology and the psychological status of rules of grammar. Paper presented at the 17th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: General Session and Parasession on the Grammar of Event Structure, Berkeley, California.

  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56-115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1991). U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multilayered perceptron: Implications for child language acquisition. Cognition, 38, 43-102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1993). From rote learning to system building: Acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition, 48, 21-69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prasada, S., & Pinker, S. (1993). Generalization of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 1-56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prasada, S., Pinker, S., & Snyder, W. (1990, November 16-18). Some evidence that irregular forms are retrieved from memory but regular forms are rule-generated. Paper presented at the 31st Annual Meeting of the Psychonomics Society, New Orleans, Louisiana.

  • Preuss, T. M. (1995). The argument from animals to humans in cognitive neuroscience. In M. S. Gazzaniga (tEd.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 1227-1241). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, B. H., Gurvit, H., Weintraub, S., Geula, C., Leimkuhler, E., & Mesulam, M. (1993). Neuropsychological patterns and language deficits in 20 consecutive cases of autopsyconfirmed Alzheimer's disease. Archives of Neurology, 50, 931-937.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhee, J., Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. T. (1999). A magnetoencephalographic study of English past tense production. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Supplement.

  • Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., & Weiskrantz, L. (Eds.). (1998). The prefrontal cortex: Executive and cognitive functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochon, E., Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1994). Sentence comprehension in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Brain and Language, 46, 329-349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Garfield, L., & Milliken, J. A. (1970). Homographic entries in the internal lexicon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 487-492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart, & PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructures of cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 216-271). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Bradford/MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saint-Cyr, J. A., Taylor, A. E., & Lang, A. E. (1988). Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. Brain, 111, 941-959.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (Eds.). (1994). Memory systems 1994. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. F., Marin, O. S. M., & Saffran, E. M. (1979). Dissociations of language function in dementia: A case study. Brain and Language, 7, 277-306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. (1992). Connectionism without tears. In S. Davis (Ed.), Connectionism: Theory and Practice (pp. 84-137). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (1997). Language acquisition and use: Learning and applying probabilistic constraints. Science, 275, 1599-1603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & Daugherty, K. G. (1992). The psychological reality of grammatical rules: Linguistic, historical, chronometric, psychophysical, computational, developmental, neurological, and genetic evidence-NOT!!!. Paper presented at the Reality of Linguistic Rules Conference, Milwaukee, Wisconsin.

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & Hoeffner, J. H. (1998). Evaluating behavioral and neuroimaging data on past tense production. Language, 74, 104-122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P. (1995). Memory and frontal lobe function. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 803-813). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Basile, L. F. H., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (1997). Source localization of the N400 response in a sentence-reading paradigm using evoked magnetic fields and magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Research, 762, 29-39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., Knowlton, B., & Musen, G. (1993). The structure and organization of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 453-495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 93, 13515-13522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemberger, J. P., & MacWhinney, B. (1988). Are inflected forms stored in the lexicon? In M. Hammond & M. Noonan (Eds.), Theoretical Morphology: Approaches in modern linguistics (pp. 101-116). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, W. A., & Amaral, D. G. (1994). Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: Cortical afferants. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 350, 497-533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T. (1993). The computation of inflectional morphology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts.

  • Ullman, M. T. (1999a). Acceptability ratings of regular and irregular past tense forms: Evidence for a dual-system model of language from word frequency and phonological neighbourhood effects. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 47-67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T. (1999b). The functional neuroanatomy of inflectional morphology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1041-1042.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T. (in press). Evidence that lexical memory is part of the temporal lobe declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the frontal/basal-ganglia procedural system. Brain and Language.

  • Ullman, M. T. (unpublished manuscript) Consistency and blocking in English past tense: Evidence for a dual-system model.

  • Ullman, M. T., Bergida, R., & O'Craven, K. (1997). Distinct fMRI activation patterns for regular and irregular past tense. NeuroImage, 5, S549.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J. H., Koroshetz, W. J., & Pinker, S. (1997). A neural dissociation within language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 266-276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Pinker, S., Coppola, M., Locascio, J., & Growdon, J. H. (1994). The neural structures subserving language: Evidence from inflectional morphology. Paper presented at the 1st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, California.

  • Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Pinker, S., Coppola, M., Locascio, J., & Growdon, J. H. (1993). Neural modularity in language: Evidence from Alzheimer's and Parkinson's diseases. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 19, 1806.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T., & Gopnik, M. (1994). Inflectional morphology in familial language impair-ment. Paper presented at the 19th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, Massachusetts.

  • Ullman, M. T., & Gopnik, M. (1999). Inflectional morphology in a family with inherited specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 51-117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T., Hickok, G., & Pinker, S. (1995). Irregular and regular inflectional morphology in an aphasic. Brain and Cognition, 28, 88-89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T., Izvorski, R., Love, T., Yee, E., Swinney, D., & Hickok, G. (in press). Neural correlates of lexicon and grammar: Evidence from the production, reading, and judgment of inflection in aphasia. Brain and Language.

  • van der Lely, H. J. K., & Ullman, M. T. (1996). The computation and representation of past-tense morphology in specifically language impaired and normally developing children. Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 804-815.

  • van der Lely, H. K. J., & Ullman, M. T. (in press). Past tense morphology in specifically language impaired and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes.

  • Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K., Alcock, K., Fletcher, P., & Passingham, R. (1995). Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 92, 930-933.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K. E., Price, C. J., Ashburner, J., Alcock, K. J., Connelly, A., Frackowiak, R. S., Friston, K. J., Pembrey, M. E., Mishkin, M., Gadian, D. G., & Passingham, R. E. (1998). Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95(21), 12695-12700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vennemann, T. (1971). Natural generative phonology. Paper presented at the Linguistics Society of America Annual Meeting, St. Louis, Missouri.

  • Waters, G. S., Caplan, D., & Rochon, E. (1995). Processing capacity and sentence comprehension in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 1-30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weyerts, H., Penke, M., Dohrn, U., Clahsen, H., & Münte, T. F. (1997). Brain potentials indicate differences between regular and irregular German plurals. Neuroreport, 8, 957-962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitaker, H. (1976). A case of the isolation of the language function. In H. Whitaker & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Studies in neurolinguistics (Vol. 2, pp. 1-58). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558-584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (1990). Hippocampal and prefrontal cortex contributions to learning and memory: Analysis of lesion and aging effects on maze learning in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 544-551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. B., & Penney, J. B. (1993). Biochemical and functional organization of the basal ganglia. In J. Jankovic & E. Tolosa (Eds.), Parkinson's disease and movement disorders, 2nd ed, (pp. 1-11). Baltimore, Maryland: Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zola, S. (1997). Amnesia: neuroanatomic and clinical aspects. In T. E. Feinberg & M. J. Farah (Eds.), Neurology and neuropsychology (pp. 447-461). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ullman, M.T. The Declarative/Procedural Model of Lexicon and Grammar. J Psycholinguist Res 30, 37–69 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005204207369

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005204207369

Navigation