Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T12:30:34.969Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Labelling Improves False Belief Understanding. A Training Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 June 2013

Elisabet Serrat Sellabona*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Girona (Spain)
Carles Rostan Sánchez
Affiliation:
Universidad de Girona (Spain)
Eduard Vallès Majoral
Affiliation:
Universidad de Girona (Spain)
Moisès Esteban Guitart
Affiliation:
Universidad de Girona (Spain)
Francesc Sidera Caballero
Affiliation:
Universidad de Girona (Spain)
Jèssica Serrano Ortiz
Affiliation:
Universidad de Girona (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Elisabet Serrat Sellabona. Departamento de Psicología. Universidad de Girona. Pl. Sant Domènec, 9. 17071 Girona (Spain). Phone: +34–972418754. Fax: +34–972418315. E-mail: elisabet.serrat@udg.edu

Abstract

A total of 104 children aged between 41 and 47 months were selected to study the relationship between language and false belief understanding. Participants were assigned to four different training conditions: discourse, labelling, control (all with deceptive objects), and sentential complements (involving non-deceptive objects). Post-test results showed an improvement in children’s false belief understanding in the discourse and the labelling conditions, but not in the sentential complements with non-deceptive objects or the control group. Furthermore, the most remarkable improvement in false belief understanding occurred in the labelling group. These results suggest that some types of linguistic experience promote the development of false belief understanding, provided that differing perspectives are confronted.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This research was partially supported by a grant from Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia (SEJ2006–12039) and a grant from the Universidad de Girona (SING2010B/9). The authors would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments and suggestions to improve the quality of the paper.

References

Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 13111320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311 Google Scholar
Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. A. (2005). Why language matters for the theory of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0001 Google Scholar
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York; NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cutting, A., & Dunn, J. (2006). Conversations with siblings and with friends: Links between relationship quality and social understanding. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 7387. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151005X70337 Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. (2005). Can language acquisition give children a point of view? In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp.186219). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0001 Google Scholar
de Villiers, J., & de Villiers, P. (2000). Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs. In Mitchell, P. & Riggs, K. J. (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and the mind (pp. 191228). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J., & Pyers, J. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17, 10371060. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00073-4 Google Scholar
Doherty, M. (2000). Children’s understanding of homonymy: Metalinguistic awareness and false belief. Journal of Child Language, 27, 367392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900004153 Google Scholar
Dunn, L., Padilla, E., Lugo, D., & Dunn, L. (1986). Test de Vocabulario de Imágenes Peabody. Adaptación Hispanoamericana. [Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Latin American adaptation] . Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1986). The development of children’s knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. American Psychologist, 41, 418425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.4.418 Google Scholar
Gale, E., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Pyers, J. (1996) Language and theory of mind in oral deaf children. In Stringfellow, A., Cahana-Amitay, D., Hughes, E., & Zukowski, A. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th annual Boston university conference on language development (Vol. 1, pp. 213224). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 2637. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb03192.x Google Scholar
Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science, 6, 346359. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00289 Google Scholar
Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843855. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131954 Google Scholar
Harris, P. (2005). Conversation, pretense, and theory of mind. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 7083). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0001 Google Scholar
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, D. (2005a). Language and the development of cognitive flexibility: Implications for theory of mind. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 144162). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0008 Google Scholar
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005b). On the possible socio-communicative roots of cognitive flexibility. In Homer, B. D. & Tamis-Lemonda, C. S. (Eds.), The development of social understanding and communication (pp. 5381). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6, 449467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00300 Google Scholar
Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74, 11301144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00597 Google Scholar
Malle, B. F. (2002). The relation between language and theory of mind in development and evolution. In Givón, T., & Malle, B. F. (Eds.), The evolution of language out of pre-language. (pp. 265284). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2007). Preschoolers’ understanding of multiple orientations to reality: The Adjectives task. Cognitive Development, 22, 289297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.008 Google Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622646. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x Google Scholar
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Hood, S., Leone, T., & Rohrer, L. (2004). Interference control in a new rule use task: Age-related changes, labeling, and attention. Child Development, 75, 15941609. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00759.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Lurye, L. E., & Liebermann, D. P. (2008). The effect of labeling on preschool children’s performance in the dimensional change card sort task. Cognitive Development, 23, 395408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.06.001 Google Scholar
Müller, U., Jacques, S., Brocki, K., & Zelazo, P. (2009). The executive functions of language in preschool children. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 5368). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511581533 Google Scholar
Olson, D. R. (1988). On the origins of beliefs and other intentional states in children. In Astington, J. W., Harris, P. L., & Olson, D. R. (Eds.), Developing theories of mind (pp. 414426). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Stummer, S., Sprung, M., & Doherty, M. (2002). Theory of mind finds its piagetian perspective: Why alternative naming comes with understanding belief. Cognitive Development, 17, 14511472. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00127-2 Google Scholar
Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and Theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 459474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01303.x Google Scholar
Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2000). Insights into theory of mind from deafness and autism. Mind and Language, 15, 123145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00126 Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Rowlandson, K., Rumsey, C., & Garnham, A. (2003). How language relates to belief, desire, and emotion understanding. Cognitive Development, 18, 139158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00002-9 Google Scholar
Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78, 376396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x Google Scholar
Slade, L., & Ruffman, T. (2005). How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of sintax, semantics, working memory and false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 117141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151004X21332 Google Scholar
Tardif, T., & Wellman, H. M. (2000). Acquisition of mental state language in Mandarin- and Cantonese-speaking children. Developmental Psychology, 36, 2543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.36.1.25 Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x Google Scholar
Towse, J. N., Redbone, J., Houston-Price, C., & Cook, S. (2000). Understanding the dimensional change card sort. Cognitive Development, 15, 347365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00021-6 Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5 Google Scholar