Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T19:46:52.732Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Language history questionnaire (LHQ 2.0): A new dynamic web-based research tool*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 November 2013

PING LI*
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
FAN ZHANG
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
ERLFANG TSAI
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
BRENDAN PULS
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
*
Address for correspondence: Ping Li, Department of Psychology and Center for Brain, Behavior, and Cognition, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USApul8@psu.edu

Abstract

The language history questionnaire (LHQ) is an important tool for assessing the linguistic background of bilinguals or second language learners and for generating self-reported proficiency in multiple languages. Previously we developed a generic LHQ based on the most commonly asked questions in published studies (Li, Sepanski & Zhao, 2006). Here we report a new web-based interface (LHQ 2.0) that has more flexibility in functionality, more accuracy in data recording, and more privacy for users and data. LHQ 2.0 achieves flexibility, accuracy, and privacy by using dynamic web-design features for enhanced data collection. It allows investigators to dynamically construct individualized LHQs on the fly and allows participants to complete the LHQ online in multiple languages. Investigators can download and delete the LHQ results and update their user and experiment information on the web. Privacy issues are handled through the online assignment of a unique ID number for each study and password-protected access to data.

Type
Research Notes
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We thank Shin-Yi Fang, Angela Grant, and Benjamin Zinszer and other members of the Brain, Language, and Computation Lab at the Pennsylvania State University for using, testing, and providing feedback on LHQ 2.0. Thanks also go to Yanping Dong, Barbara Malt, Li Sheng, Xiaowei Zhao, and two anonymous reviewers for providing valuable comments, and to Shin-Yi Fang, Shiwen Feng, Kyra Swick, Jing Yang, and Helen Zhao for providing the Chinese versions, Jennifer Legault for the French version, Angela Grant for the Italian version, Ceriz Bicalho Costa for the Portuguese version, and Pablo Requena for the Spanish version. We also thank the Center for Linguistics and Applied Linguistics at Guangdong University of Foreign Studies, China, where revision of the manuscript was conducted. Preparation of the manuscript was supported in part by a grant from the National Science Foundation (#BCS-1057855) and a grant from the National Science Council of Taiwan (#NSC 102-2911-I-003-301).

References

Bedore, L. M., Pena, E. D., Summers, C. L., Boerger, K. M., Resendiz, M. D., Greene, K., Bohman, T., & Gillam, R. B. (2012). The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 616629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chandrasekaran, B., Krishnan, A., & Gandour, J. T. (2009). Relative influence of musical and linguistic experience on early cortical processing of pitch contours. Brain and Language, 108, 19.Google Scholar
Crinion, J. T., Green, D. W., Chung, R., Ali, N., Grogan, A., Price, G. R., Mechelli, A., & Price, C. J. (2009). Neuroanatomical markers of speaking Chinese. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 41084115.Google Scholar
Dunn, A. L., & Fox Tree, J. E. (2009). A quick, gradient bilingual dominance scale. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 273289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Weissberger, G. H., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R. I., & Cera, C. M. (2012). Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 594615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Google Scholar (2013). Language history questionnaire: A web-based interface for bilingual research. http://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&q=language+history+questionnaire&btnG=&as_sdt=1%2C39&as_sdtp= (retrieved September 8, 2013; cited by 73).Google Scholar
Krishnan, A., Gandour, J. T., & Bidelman, G. M. (2010). The effects of tone language experience on pitch processing in the brainstem. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23, 8195.Google Scholar
Li, P., Sepanski, S., & Zhao, X. (2006). Language history questionnaire: A web-based interface for bilingual research. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 202210.Google Scholar
Wilson, R., & Dewaele, J.-M. (2010). The use of web questionnaires in second language acquisition and bilingualism research. Second Language Research, 26, 103123.Google Scholar