Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T13:24:46.475Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Differential Contribution of Cognitive and Psychomotor Functions to the Age-Related Slowing of Speech Production

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2011

Claudia Rodríguez-Aranda*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Tromsø, Norway
Mona Jakobsen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Tromsø, Norway
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Claudia Rodríguez-Aranda, Department of Psychology, University of Tromsø, N-9037 Tromsø, Norway. E-mail: claudia.rodriguez-aranda@uit.no

Abstract

Healthy elderly adults are slower to initiate and produce speech. However, the sources of the slowing remain poorly understood. The present study evaluates to which extent psychomotor and cognitive changes influence age-related slowing in speech generation. Four verbal tasks varying in degree of difficulty and cognitive demands were used to evaluate 30 young and 30 healthy elderly. Speed of word production was measured by reaction times and pronunciation durations. Stroop test and Digits backwards were used as cognitive predictors while the Purdue Pegboard and Finger Tapping were used as psychomotor predictors. The relative contribution of cognitive and psychomotor functioning was evaluated by hierarchical regression analyses and based on the processing speed hypothesis. Results showed that Vocabulary and psychomotor execution significantly explained a portion of the variance in RTs depending on type of verbal task. These variables explained 36% of the total variance in reading, 26% in naming, 31% in phonemic fluency and 47% in semantic fluency. Also, Vocabulary and psychomotor functions strongly predicted pronunciation speed. Conversely, tests related to executive functions and working memory were not significant predictors. These data demonstrate the importance of the interplay between Vocabulary and psychomotor decline on speed of language production among healthy elderly adults. (JINS, 2011, 17, 807–821)

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Balota, D.A., Duchek, J.M. (1988). Age-related differences in lexical access, spreading activation, and simple pronunciation. Psychology and Aging, 3, 8493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelsohn, M., Mock, J., Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, A.L. (1967). Problems of test construction in the field of aphasia. Cortex, 3, 3258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, W.D., Feldman, S. (1985). Multiple regression in practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birren, J.E., Fisher, L.M. (1992). Aging and slowing of behavior: Consequences for cognition and survival. In T.B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Psychology and aging (pp. 137). Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska.Google Scholar
Bopp, K.L., Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60, 223233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borden, G.J., Harris, K.S., Raphael, L.J. (2003). Speech science primer. Physiology, acoustics, and perception of speech (4th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Brébion, G. (2001). Language processing, slowing, and speed/accuracy trade-off in the elderly. Experimental Aging Research, 27, 137150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryan, J., Luszcz, M.A. (2000). Measures of fluency as predictors of incidental memory among older adults. Psychology and Aging, 15, 483489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Barbarotto, R. (1999). Gender affects word retrieval of certain categories in semantic fluency tasks. Cortex, 35, 273278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cauthen, N.R. (1977). Extension of the Wechsler Memory Scale norms to older age groups. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 208211.3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connor, L.T., Spiro, A., Obler, L.K., Albert, M.L. (2004). Change in object naming ability during adulthood. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59, 203209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conover, W.J., Iman, R.L. (1982). Analysis of covariance using the rank transformation. Biometrics, 38, 715724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desrosiers, J., Hebert, R., Bravo, G., Dutil, E. (1995). The Purdue Pegboard Test – Normative data for people aged 60 and over. Disability and Rehabilitation, 17, 217224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feyereisen, P., Demaeght, N., Samson, D. (1998). Why do picture naming latencies increase with age: General slowing, greater sensitivity to interference, or task-specific deficits? Experimental Aging Research, 24, 2151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.F., McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fozo, M.S., Watson, B.C. (1998). Task complexity effect on vocal reaction time in aged speakers. Journal of Voice, 12, 404414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaser, W.R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42, 61105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godefroy, O., Roussel, M., Despretz, P., Quaglino, V., Boucart, M. (2010). Age-related slowing: Perceptuomotor, decision, or attention decline? Experimental Aging Research, 36, 169189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golden, C.J. (1978). Stroop color and word test. A manual for clinical and experimental uses. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting.Google Scholar
Goodglass, H. (1980). Naming disorders in aphasia and aging. In L.K. Obler & M.L. Albert (Eds.), Language and communication in the elderly: Clinical, therapeutic, and experimental issues (pp. 3745). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Heggstad, K. (1982). Norsk frekvensordbok (Norwegian Frequency Dictionary). Bergen: Universitetsforlaget.Google Scholar
Hodgson, C., Ellis, A.W. (1998). Last in, first to go: Age of acquisition and naming in the elderly. Brain and Language, 64, 146163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, V.M. (2009). Bottom-up processing and reading comprehension in experienced adult readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 32, 309326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huisman, M., Kunst, A.E., Andersen, O., Bopp, M., Borgan, J.K., Borrell, C., Mackenbach, J.P. (2004). Socioeconomic inequalities in mortality among elderly people in 11 European populations. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 58, 468475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kear-Colwell, J.J., Heller, M. (1978). A normative study of the Wechsler Memory Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 437442.3.0.CO;2-K>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemper, S., Mitzner, T.L. (2001). Language production and comprehension. In J.E. Birren & K.W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (5th ed., pp. 378398). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kemper, S., Rash, S., Kynette, D., Norman, S. (1990). Telling stories: The structure of adults’ narratives. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2, 205228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linville, S.E. (2001a). Aging and speaking rate. In: Vocal aging (pp. 157167). San Diego, CA: Singular Thomson Learning.Google Scholar
Linville, S.E. (2001b). Vocal aging. San Diego, CA: Singular Thomson Learning.Google Scholar
Lopez-Higes, R., Rubio Valdehita, S., Martin Aragoneses, M.T., Del Rio, D. (2010). Interindividual variability in vocabulary, sentence comprehension and working memory in the elderly: Effects of cognitive deterioration. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13, 7587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loonstra, A.S., Tarlow, A.R., Sellers, A.H. (2001). COWAT metanorms across age, education, and gender. Applied Neuropsychology, 8, 161166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCallum, R.C., Zhang, S.B., Preacher, K.J., Rucker, D.D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 1940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mortensen, L., Meyer, A.S., Humphreys, G.W. (2006). Age-related effects on speech production: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 238290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Multhaup, K.S., Balota, D.A., Cowan, N. (1996). Implications of aging, lexicality, and item length for the mechanisms underlying memory span. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 112120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramig, L.A. (1983). Effects of physiological aging on speaking and reading rates. Journal of Communication Disorders, 16, 217226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R.M., Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, B.A., Fuhrer, R., Marmot, M., Richards, M. (2010). Does retirement influence cognitive performance? The Whitehall II Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, [Epub ahead of print].Google ScholarPubMed
Robert, C., Mathey, S. (2007). Aging and lexical inhibition: The effect of orthographic neighborhood frequency in young and older adults. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62, 340342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez-Aranda, C. (2003). Reduced writing and reading speed and age-related changes in verbal fluency tasks. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 203215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez-Aranda, C., Waterloo, K., Sparr, S., Sundet, K.S. (2006). Age-related psychomotor slowing as an important component of verbal fluency. Evidence from healthy individuals and Alzheimer's patients. Journal of Neurology, 253, 14141427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T.A. (1993). Speed and knowledge as determinants of adult age differences in verbal tasks. Journal of Gerontology, 48, 2936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T.A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaie, K.W., Willis, S.L. (2002). Biological development. The aging body. In Adult development and aging (5th ed., pp. 390430). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Seo, E.H., Lee, D.Y., Choo, I.H., Kim, S.G., Kim, K.W., Youn, J.C., Woo, J.I. (2008). Normative study of the Stroop Color and Word Test in an educationally diverse elderly population. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23, 10201027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Span, M.M., Ridderinkhof, K.R., Van der Molen, M.W. (2004). Age-related changes in the efficiency of cognitive processing across the life span. Acta Psychologica, 117, 155183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stine-Morrow, E.A., Miller, L.M., Hertzog, C. (2006). Aging and self-regulated language processing. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 582606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, E., Sherman, A.M., Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B.G., Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Tiffin, J. (1968). Purdue pegboard: Examiner manual. Chicago: Science Research Associates.Google Scholar
Tombaugh, T.N., Kozak, J., Rees, L. (1999). Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 167177.Google ScholarPubMed
Tun, P.A., Lachman, M.E. (2008). Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: Education, sex, and task complexity matter. Developmental Psychology, 44, 14211429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanier, M. (1991). Test de Stroop. Montréal, Canada: Centre de recherche Institut de réadaptation de Montréal.Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P. (2003). Aging and vocabulary scores: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 18, 332339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J. (2002). Aging, executive control, and attention: A review of meta-analyses. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 26, 849857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhaeghen, P., De Meersman, L. (1998). Aging and the Stroop effect: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 13, 120126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wilson, R.S., Bienias, J.L., Evans, D.A., Bennett, D.A. (2004). Religious orders study: Overview and change in cognitive and motor speed. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 11, 280303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wingfield, A., Stine-Morrow, E.A. (2000). Language and speech. In F.I.M. Craik & T.A. Salthouse (Eds.), Handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 359416). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar