Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T05:47:54.253Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is “not just right experience” (NJRE) in obsessive-compulsive disorder part of an autistic phenotype?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 November 2016

Josselyn Hellriegel*
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Caroline Barber
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Maheshi Wikramanayake
Affiliation:
National OCDs Specialist Service, Herts. Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, Queen Elizabeth II Hospital, Welwyn Garden City, Herts, UK
Naomi A. Fineberg
Affiliation:
National OCDs Specialist Service, Herts. Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, Queen Elizabeth II Hospital, Welwyn Garden City, Herts, UK
William Mandy
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr. Josselyn Hellriegel, Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1N 6BT, UK.(Email: j.hellriegel@ucl.ac.uk)

Abstract

Objective

Harm avoidance (HA) and “not just right experience” (NJRE) have been proposed to be 2 core motivational processes underlying obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). The objective of this study was to explore whether NJRE demarcates a neurodevelopmental OCD subgroup distinct from HA related to autistic traits and/or to a broader phenotype of cognitive rigidity and sensory processing difficulties associated with an earlier age of OCD onset.

Methods

A correlational design investigated whether NJRE and HA are distinct entities in OCD and explored their relationship to autism spectrum disorder (ASD) traits measured by the Autism Quotient (AQ), sensory processing, set-shifting, and age of OCD onset in an OCD sample (N=25).

Results

NJRE was only moderately (r=.34) correlated to HA and not significant in this study. Consistent with predictions, NJRE was associated with sensory processing difficulties and an earlier age of OCD onset. No significant relationships were found between NJRE and ASD traits as measured by the AQ or set-shifting difficulties.

Conclusions

These preliminary findings suggest a lack of evidence demonstrating NJRE as a manifestation of core autistic traits as measured by the AQ. However, NJRE was associated with sensory abnormalities and an earlier age of OCD onset. The role of NJRE as a developmental, and possibly neurodevelopmental, risk factor for OCD possibly warrants further investigation.

Type
Original Research
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

We would like to express our appreciation to the Cambridge Cognition team, who helpfully provided consultation in selecting outcome variables to address the research question. Furthermore, we would also like to thank Dr. Laura Summerfeldt for providing us with the Obsessive-Compulsive Trait Core Dimensions Questionnaire, which she developed.

Grant: Graduate School Research Projects Fund Reference 3.12/13.RP.LMS.

References

1. Chik, HM, Calamari, JE, Rector, NA, Riemann, BC. What do low-dysfunctional beliefs obsessive-compulsive disorder subgroups believe? J Anxiety Disord. 2010; 24(8): 837846.Google Scholar
2. Ecker, W, Gönner, S. Incompleteness and harm avoidance in OCD symptom dimensions. Behav Res Ther. 2008; 46(8): 895904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Rasmussen, SA, Eisen, JL. The epidemiology and differential diagnosis of obsessive compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 1992; 53(Suppl): 410.Google Scholar
4. Coles, ME, Heimberg, RG, Frost, RO, Steketee, G. Not just right experiences and obsessive-compulsive features: experimental and self-monitoring perspectives. Behav Res Ther. 2005; 43(2): 153167.Google Scholar
5. Ferrão, YA, Shavitt, RG, Prado, H, et al. Sensory phenomena associated with repetitive behaviors in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an exploratory study of 1001 patients. Psychiatry Res. 2012; 197(3): 253258.Google Scholar
6. Rosario, MC, Prado, HS, Borcato, S, et al. Validation of the University of São Paulo Sensory Phenomena Scale: initial psychometric properties. CNS Spectr. 2009; 14(6): 315323.Google Scholar
7. Smith, AH, Wetterneck, CT, Hart, JM, Short, MB, Björgvinsson, T. Differences in obsessional beliefs and emotion appraisal in obsessive compulsive symptom presentation. J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord. 2012; 1(1): 5461.Google Scholar
8. Starcevic, V, Berle, D, Brakoulias, V, et al. Functions of compulsions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2011; 45(6): 449457.Google Scholar
9. Do Rosario-Campos, MC, Leckman, JF, Mercadante, MT, et al. Adults with early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2001; 158(11): 18991903.Google Scholar
10. Leckman, JF, Walker, DE, Goodman, WK, Pauls, DL, Cohen, DJ. “Just right” perceptions associated with compulsive behavior in Tourette’s syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 1994; 151(5): 675680.Google ScholarPubMed
11. Leckman, JF, Grice, DE, Barr, LC, et al. Tic-related vs. non-tic-related obsessive compulsive disorder. Anxiety. 1994–1995; 1(5): 208215.Google Scholar
12. Coles, ME, Pinto, A, Mancebo, MC, Rasmussen, SA, Eisen, JL. OCD with comorbid OCPD: a subtype of OCD? J Psychiatr Res. 2008; 42(4): 289296.Google Scholar
13. Ecker, W, Kupfer, J, Gönner, S. Incompleteness as a link between obsessive-compulsive personality traits and specific symptom dimensions of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2014; 21(5): 394402.Google Scholar
14. Miguel, EC, Coffey, BJ, Baer, L, Savage, CR, Rauch, SL, Jenike, MA. Phenomenology of intentional repetitive behaviors in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette’s disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 1995; 56(6): 246255.Google Scholar
15. Miguel, EC, do Rosário-Campos, MC, Prado, HS, et al. Sensory phenomena in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette’s disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2000; 61(2): 150156.Google Scholar
16. Bejerot, S. An autistic dimension: a proposed subtype of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Autism. 2007; 11(2): 101110.Google Scholar
17. Rieke, EF, Anderson, D. Adolescent/adult sensory profile and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Am J Occup Ther. 2009; 63(2): 138145.Google Scholar
18. Lawrence, NS, Wooderson, S, Mataix-Cols, D, David, R, Speckens, A, Phillips, ML. Decision making and set shifting impairments are associated with distinct symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology. 2006; 20(4): 409419.Google Scholar
19. Kloosterman, PH, Summerfeldt, LJ, Parker, JDA, Holden, JJA. The obsessive-compulsive trait of incompleteness in parents of children with autism spectrum disorders. J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord. 2013; 2(2): 176182.Google Scholar
20. Summerfeldt, LJ. Understanding and treating incompleteness in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychol. 2004; 60(11): 11551168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992.Google Scholar
22. Summerfeldt, LJ, Kloosterman, PH, Parker, JDA, Antony, MM, Swinson, RP. Assessing and validating the obsessive-compulsive-related construct of incompleteness. Poster presented at: 62nd Annual Convention of the Canadian Psychological Association; June 21-23, 2001; Ste-Foy, Quebec.Google Scholar
23. Brown, C, Dunn, W. Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2002.Google Scholar
24. Horder, J, Wilson, CE, Mendez, MA, Murphy, DG. Autistic traits and abnormal sensory experiences in adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014; 44(6): 14611469.Google Scholar
25. Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL, Lushene, R, Vagg, PR, Jacobs, GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983.Google Scholar
26. Barnes, LL, Harp, D, Jung, WS. Reliability generalization of scores on the Spielberger state-trait anxiety inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2002; 62(4): 603618.Google Scholar
27. Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, Skinner, R, Martin, J, Clubley, E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001; 31(1): 517.Google Scholar
28. Bishop, SL, Seltzer, MM. Self-reported autism symptoms in adults with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012; 42(11): 23542363.Google Scholar
29. Montgomery, SA, Asberg, M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry. 1979; 134(4): 382389.Google Scholar
30. Carmody, T, Rush, AJ, Bernstein, I, et al. The Montgomery Åsberg and the Hamilton ratings of depression: a comparison of measures. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006; 16(8): 601611.Google Scholar
31. Bech, P. The Bech–Rafaelsen Melancholia Scale (MES) in clinical trials of therapies in depressive disorders: a 20-year review of its use as outcome measure. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2002; 106(4): 252264.Google Scholar
32. Goodman, WK, Price, LH, Rasmussen, SA, et al. The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. I. Development, use, and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989; 46(11): 10061011.Google Scholar
33. Deacon, BJ, Abramowitz, JS. The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: factor analysis, construct validity, and suggestions for refinement. J Anxiety Disord. 2005; 19(5): 573585.Google Scholar
34. Lowe, C, Rabbitt, P. Test/re-test reliability of the CANTAB and ISPOCD neuropsychological batteries: theoretical and practical issues. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery. International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction. Neuropsychologia. 1998; 36(9): 915923.Google Scholar
35. Ozonoff, S, Cook, I, Coon, H, et al. Performance on Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe function in people with autistic disorder: evidence from the Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism network. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004; 34(2): 139150.Google Scholar
36. Robbins, TW, James, M, Owen, AM, et al. A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998; 4(5): 474490.Google Scholar
37. Pietrefesa, AS, Coles, ME. Moving beyond an exclusive focus on harm avoidance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: considering the role of incompleteness. Behav Ther. 2008; 39(3): 224231.Google Scholar
38. Ketelaars, C, Horwitz, E, Sytema, S, et al. Brief report: adults with mild autism spectrum disorders (ASD): scores on the autism spectrum quotient (AQ) and comorbid psychopathology. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008; 38(1): 176180.Google Scholar
39. Murphy, DGM, Beecham, J, Craig, M, Ecker, C. Autism in adults. New biological findings and their translational implications to the cost of clinical services. Brain Res. 2011; 1380: 2233.Google Scholar
40. Quednow, BB, Kühn, KU, Stelzenmuelle, R, Hoenig, K, Maier, W, Wagner, M. Effects of serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants on auditory startle response in patients with major depression. Psychopharmacol (Berl). 2004; 175(4): 399406.Google Scholar
41. Ronald, A, Simonoff, E, Kuntsi, J, Asherson, P, Plomin, R. Evidence for overlapping genetic influences on autistic and ADHD behaviours in a community twin sample. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008; 49(5): 535542.Google Scholar
42. Taylor, S, McKay, D, Crowe, KB, Abramowitz, JS, Conelea, CA, Calamari, JE, Sica, C. The sense of incompleteness as a motivator of obsessive-compulsive symptoms: an empirical analysis of concepts and correlates. Behav Ther. 2014; 45(2): 254262.Google Scholar
43. Happé, F, Ronald, A. The “fractionable autism triad”: a review of evidence from behavioural, genetic, cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychol Rev. 2008; 18(4): 287304.Google Scholar
44. Pooni, J, Ninteman, A, Bryant-Waugh, R, Nicholls, D, Mandy, W. Investigating autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in early onset eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord. 2012; 45(4): 583591.Google Scholar
45. LeCouteur, A, Lord, C, Rutter, M. The Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2003.Google Scholar
46. Skuse, D, Warrington, R, Bishop, D, et al. The developmental, dimensional and diagnostic interview (3di): a novel computerized assessment for autism spectrum disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004; 43(5): 548558.Google Scholar
47. Bodfish, JW, Symons, FJ, Parker, DE, Lewis, MH. Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: comparisons to mental retardation. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000; 30(3): 237243.Google Scholar
48. Mandy, WPL, Charman, T, Skuse, DH. Testing the construct validity of proposed criteria for DSM-5 autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012; 51(1): 4150.Google Scholar
49. Hill, EL. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review. 2004; 24(2): 189233.Google Scholar
50. Dunn, W. The impact of sensory processing abilities on the daily lives of young children and their families: a conceptual model. Infants & Young Children. 1997; 9(4): 2335.Google Scholar
51. Blakemore, SJ, Tavassoli, T, Calò, S, et al. Tactile sensitivity in Asperger syndrome. Brain Cogn. 2006; 61(1): 513.Google Scholar
52. Smith, PJ, Need, AC, Cirulli, ET, Chiba-Falek, O, Attix, DK. A comparison of the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB) with “traditional” neuropsychological testing instruments. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2013; 35(3): 319328.Google Scholar
53. Geller, D, Biederman, J, Jones, J, et al. Is juvenile obsessive-compulsive disorder a developmental subtype of the disorder? A review of the pediatric literature. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998; 37(4): 420427.Google Scholar