Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-17T10:18:43.880Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mothers’ emotional expressivity in urban and rural societies: Salience and links with young adolescents’ emotional wellbeing and expressivity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 November 2021

Ruyi Ding
Affiliation:
Tsinghua Shenzhen International Graduate School, Tsinghua University, Shenzhen, China
Shuang Bi
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology, College of Public Administration, Guangdong University of Foreign Studies
Yuhan Luo
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
Tuo Liu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Division for Psychological Methods and Statistics, Carl von Ossietzky University Oldenburg, Oldenburg, Germany
Pusheng Wang
Affiliation:
Tsinghua Shenzhen International Graduate School, Tsinghua University, Shenzhen, China
Wei He*
Affiliation:
Nanshan Educational Science Institute of Shenzhen, Shenzhen, China
Shiguang Ni*
Affiliation:
Tsinghua Shenzhen International Graduate School, Tsinghua University, Shenzhen, China
*
Corresponding authors: Wei He and Shiguang Ni, emails: psyhewei@163.com, ni.shiguang@sz.tsinghua.edu.cn
Corresponding authors: Wei He and Shiguang Ni, emails: psyhewei@163.com, ni.shiguang@sz.tsinghua.edu.cn

Abstract

This research aims to investigate the salience of mothers’ emotional expressivity and its links with adolescents’ emotional wellbeing and expressivity in an urban society endorsing more individualism and a rural society ascribing to more collectivism. By comparing Chinese urban (N = 283, Mage = 14.13) and rural (N = 247, Mage = 14.09) adolescents, this research found that urban mothers’ expression of positive-dominant and positive-submissive emotions (PD and PS) were more common while expression of negative-dominant (ND) emotions was less common than rural mothers’. PD and PS had significant links with urban and rural adolescents’ increased emotional expressivity and self-esteem, however, only significantly related to urban adolescents’ decreased depression but not with rural adolescents’. ND had significant links with both urban and rural adolescents’ expression of negative emotions, however, only significantly correlated with urban adolescents’ less level of self-esteem and rural adolescents’ more expression of positive emotions. No significant difference was found in the salience of urban and rural mothers’ expression of negative-submissive (NS) emotions, which positively related to both urban and rural adolescents’ depression and emotional expressivity. Moreover, we found that adolescents’ emotional wellbeing (i.e., self-esteem and depression) mediated the relationship between mothers’ emotional expressivity and adolescents’ expressivity in both societies. Overall, the study findings document that the salience of mothers’ emotional expressivity and its relations with adolescents’ emotional adjustment differ between urban and rural societies.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aktar, E., & Bögels, S. M. (2017). Exposure to parents’ negative emotions as a developmental pathway to the family aggregation of depression and anxiety in the first year of life. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 20(4), 369390. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-017-0240-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bai, S., Repetti, R. L., & Sperling, J. B. (2016). Children’s expressions of positive emotion are sustained by smiling, touching, and playing with parents and siblings: A naturalistic observational study of family life. Developmental Psychology, 52(1), 88101. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039854 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barry, R. A., & Kochanska, G. (2010). A longitudinal investigation of the affective environment in families with young children: From infancy to early school age. Emotion, 10(2), 237249. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018485 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedwell, J. S., Cohen, A. S., Spencer, C. C., & Simpson, S. D. (2019). Emotion experience and expressive suppression scale: Psychometric properties and relationships with depression and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 142, 145152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.02.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2017). Individualism and collectivism. The International Encyclopedia of Intercultural Communication, 19. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118783665.ieicc0107 Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Suwalsky, J. T. D., Venuti, P., de Falco, S., de Galperín, C. Z., Gini, M., & Tichovolsky, M. H. (2012). Emotional relationships in mothers and infants. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(2), 171197. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110388563 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, R. H. (2002). Environment and parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting; Vol. 2. Biology and Ecology of Parenting (2nd ed., pp. 281314). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brand, A. E., & Klimes-Dougan, B. (2010). Emotion socialization in adolescence: The roles of mothers and fathers. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2010(128), 85100. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.270 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camras, L., Kolmodin, K., & Chen, Y. (2008). Mothers’ self-reported emotional expression in Mainland Chinese, Chinese American and European American families. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(5), 459463. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408093665 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camras, L. A., Bakeman, R., Chen, Y., Norris, K., & Cain, T. R. (2006). Culture, ethnicity, and children’s facial expressions: A study of European American, mainland Chinese, Chinese American, and adopted Chinese girls. Emotion, 6(1), 103114. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.103 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaplin, T. M., & Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 735. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030737 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S. H., & Zhou, Q. (2019a). Cultural values, social status, and Chinese American immigrant parents’ emotional expressivity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 50(3), 381395. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022118817653 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S. H., & Zhou, Q. (2019b). Longitudinal relations of cultural orientation and emotional expressivity in Chinese American immigrant parents: Sociocultural influences on emotional development in adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 55(5), 11111123. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000681 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S. H., Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., & Wang, Y. (2011). Parental expressivity and parenting styles in chinese families: Prospective and unique relations to children’s psychological adjustment. Parenting, 11(4), 288307. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2011.613725 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S. H., Zhou, Q., Main, A., & Lee, E. H. (2015). Chinese American immigrant parents’ emotional expression in the family: Relations with parents’ cultural orientations and children’s emotion-related regulation. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 21(4), 619629. https://doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Fu, R., & Zhao, S. (2015). Culture and socialization. In Grusec, J. E. & Hastings, D. P. (Eds.), Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research (pp. 451471). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chen, X., & Li, D. (2012). Parental encouragement of initiative-taking and adjustment in Chinese children from rural, urban, and urbanized families. Journal of Family Psychology, 26(6), 927. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030708 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9(2), 233255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiorri, C., Marsh, H. W., Ubbiali, A., & Donati, D. (2016). Testing the factor structure and measurement invariance across gender of the Big Five Inventory through exploratory structural equation modeling. Journal of Personality Assessment, 98(1), 8899. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2015.1035381 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, A. (2018). Emotional Well-Being of Children and Adolescents: Recent Trends and Relevant Factors. (No. 169). Paris: OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/41576fb2-en Google Scholar
Cole, P. , M., & Tan, P. Z. (2015). Emotion socialization from a cultural perspective. In Hastings, P. D. (Ed.), Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research (pp. 499519). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2008). The revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R). In Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G., & Saklofske, D. H. (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment: Volume 2 — Personality Measurement and Testing (pp. 179198). Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781849200479.n9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cotar-Konrad, S. (2016). Family emotional expressiveness and family structure. Psihologija, 49(4), 319333. https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI1604319C CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curran, P. G. (2016). Methods for the detection of carelessly invalid responses in survey data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66, 419. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.07.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior problems and discipline revisited: Nonlinear ffects and variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8(3), 161175. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0803_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2007). Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: a general analytical framework using moderated path analysis. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (2020). Findings, issues, and new directions for research on emotion socialization. Developmental psychology, 56(3), 664. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000906 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Gershoff, E. T., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, A. J., Losoya, S. H., Guthrie, I. K., & Murphy, B. C. (2001). Mother’s emotional expressivity and children’s behavior problems and social competence: Mediation through children’s regulation. Developmental Psychology, 37(4), 475490. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.4.475 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Liew, J., & Pidada, S. U. (2001). The relations of parental emotional expressivity with quality of Indonesian children’s social functioning. Emotion, 1(2), 116136. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.2.116 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enders, C., & Bandalos, D. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 8(3), 430457. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem0803_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedlmeier, W., Corapci, F., & Cole, P. M. (2011). Emotion socialization in cross-cultural perspective. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(7), 410427. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00362.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gershoff, E. T., Grogan-Kaylor, A., Lansford, J. E., Chang, L., Zelli, A., Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (2010). Parent discipline practices in an international sample: Associations ith child behaviors and moderation by perceived normativeness. Child Development, 81(2), 487502. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01409.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (2009). Linking social change and developmental change: Shifting pathways of human development. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 401418. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014726 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (1998). Mapping the domain of expressivity: Multimethod evidence for a hierarchical model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 170191. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.170 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J., John, O. P., & Richards, J. M. (2000). The dissociation of emotion expression from emotion experience: A personality perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(6), 712726. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200268006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halberstadt, A. G. (1986). Family socialization of emotional expression and nonverbal communication styles and skills. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(4), 827836. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.4.827 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halberstadt, A. G., Cassidy, J., Stifter, C. A., Parke, R. D., & Fox, N. A. (1995). Self-expressiveness within the family context: Psychometric support for a new measure. Psychological Assessment, 7(1), 93103. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.1.93 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halberstadt, A. G., & Eaton, K. L. (2002). A meta-analysis of fmily expressiveness and children’s emotion expressiveness and understanding. Marriage & Family Review, 34(1–2), 3562. https://doi.org/10.1300/J002v34n01_03 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hareli, S., Kafetsios, K., & Hess, U. (2015). A cross-cultural study on emotion expression and the learning of social norms. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01501 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helwig, C. C., To, S., Wang, Q., Liu, C., & Yang, S. (2014). Judgments and reasoning about parental discipline involving induction and psychological cntrol in China and Canada. Child Development, 85(3), 11501167. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12183 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, J. L., Liu, M., & Bowling, N. A. (2015). Insufficient effort responding: Examining an insidious confound in survey data. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(3), 828845. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038510 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaworska, N., & MacQueen, G. (2015). Adolescence as a unique developmental period. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN, 40(5), 291. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.150268 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, D. C., Abbey, B. B., & Cumberland, A. (1998). The development of display rule knowledge: Linkages with family expressiveness and social competence. Child Development, 69(4), 12091222. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06168.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagitcibasi, C. (1997). Individualism and collectivism. In Berry, J. W., Segall, M. H., & Kagitcibasi, C. (Eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology. Vol. 3: Social Behavior and Applications (2nd ed., pp. 149). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (2003). Socialization for competence: Cultural models of infancy. Human Development, 46, 288311. https://doi.org/10.1159/000071937 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohout, F. J., Berkman, L. F., Evans, D. A., & Cornoni-Huntley, J. (1993). Two shorter forms of the CES-D depression symptoms index. Journal of Aging and Health, 5(2), 179193. https://doi.org/10.1177/089826439300500202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konrad, S. Č. (2016). Family emotional expressiveness and family structure. Psihologija, 49(4), 319333. https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI1604319C CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyeong, Y., Cheung, R. Y., & Cheung, C. S. (2021). The role of family expressiveness in American and Chinese adolescents’ emotional experiences. Social Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12515 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lansford, J. E., Godwin, J., Al-Hassan, S. M., Bacchini, D., Bornstein, M. H., Chang, L., Chen, B.-B., Deater-Deckard, K., Di Giunta, L., Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Pastorelli, C., Skinner, A. T., Sorbring, E., Steinberg, L., Tapanya, S., Alampay, L. P., Uribe Tirado, L. M., & Zelli, A. (2018). Longitudinal associations between parenting and youth adjustment in twelve cultural groups: Cultural normativeness of parenting as a moderator. Developmental Psychology, 54(2), 362377. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000416 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, C. C. (2015). Gratitude and depression in young adults: The mediating role of self-esteem and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 3034. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.017 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindsey, E. W., Caldera, Y. M., & Rivera, M. (2013). Mother–child and father–child emotional expressiveness in Mexican-American families and toddlers’ peer interactions. Early Child Development and Care, 183(3–4), 378393. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2012.711589 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., Lu, S., & Chen, Y. (2013). Spatio-temporal change of urbanerural equalized development patterns in China and its driving factors. Journal of Rural Studies, 32(320), e330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrurstud.2013.08.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.98.2.224 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Grayson, D. (2005). Goodness of fit evaluation in structural equation modeling. In Maydeu-Olivares, A. & McArdle, J. (Eds.), Psychometrics. A Festschrift for Roderick P. McDonald (pp. 275340). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D. (2001). Culture and emotion. In Matsumoto, D. (Ed.), The Handbook of Culture and Psychology (pp. 171194). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., Seung, H. Y., & Fontaine, J. (2008). Mapping expressive differences around the world. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39(1), 5574. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022107311854 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCord, B. L., & Raval, V. V. (2016). Asian Indian immigrant and White American maternal emotion socialization and child socio-emotional functioning. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(2), 464474. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-015-0227-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meade, A. W., & Craig, S. B. (2012). Identifying careless responses in survey data. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 437455. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028085 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michalik, N. M., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Ladd, B., Thompson, M., & Valiente, C. (2007). Longitudinal relations among parental emotional expressivity and sympathy and prosocial behavior in adolescence. Social Development, 16(2), 286309. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00385.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milfont, T. L., & Fischer, R. (2010). Testing measurement invariance across groups: applications in cross-cultural research. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(1), 111130. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.857 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milojevich, H. M., & Haskett, M. E. (2018). Three-year trajectories of emotional expressiveness among maltreating mothers: The role of life changes. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27(1), 141153. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0858-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morelen, D., Jacob, M. L., Suveg, C., Jones, A., & Thomassin, K. (2013). Family emotion expressivity, emotion regulation, and the link to psychopathology: Examination across race. British Journal of Psychology, 104(2), 149166. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2012.02108.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morelen, D., Zeman, J., Perry-Parrish, C., & Anderson, E. (2011). Children’s emotion regulation across and within nations: A comparison of Ghanaian, Kenyan, and American youth. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 415431. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.2011.02050.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 361388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, J. A., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Leerkes, E. M., Marcovitch, S., & Blankson, A. N. (2012). Maternal expressive style and children’s emotional development. Infant and Child Development, 21(3), 267286. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.748 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberle, E. (2018). Early adolescents’ emotional well-being in the classroom: The role of personal and contextual assets. Journal of School Health, 88(2), 101111. https://doi.org/10.1111/josh.12585 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 372. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollastri, A. R., Raftery-Helmer, J. N., Cardemil, E. V., & Addis, M. E. (2018). Social context, emotional expressivity, and social adjustment in adolescent males. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 19(1), 6977. https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000081 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: The state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Developmental Review, 41, 7190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raval, V. V., & Walker, B. L. (2019). Unpacking ‘culture’: Caregiver socialization of emotion and child functioning in diverse families. Developmental Review, 51, 146174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.11.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F., & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described. Review of General Psychology, 7(4), 331363. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.4.331 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.10.1515/9781400876136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutkowski, L., & Svetina, D. (2014). Assessing the hypothesis of measurement invariance in the context of large-scale international surveys. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 74(1), 3157. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164413498257 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryu, E., & Cheong, J. (2017). Comparing indirect effects in different groups in single-group and multi-group structural equation models. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00747 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sallquist, J., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Gaertner, B. M., Eggum, N. D., & Zhou, N. (2009). Mothers’ and children’s positive emotion: Relations and trajectories across four years. Social Development, 19(4), 799821. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00565.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoemann, A. M., Boulton, A. J., & Short, S. D. (2017). Determining power and sample size for simple and complex mediation models. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(4), 379386. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617715068 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steenkamp, J. E. M., & Baumgartner, H. (1998). Assessing measurement invariance in cross-national consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 25, 7890.10.1086/209528CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stocker, C. M., Richmond, M. K., Rhoades, G. K., & Kiang, L. (2007). Family emotional processes and adolescents’ adjustment. Social Development, 16(2), 310325. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00386.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stride, C. B., Gardner, S., Catley, N., & Thomas, F. (2015). Mplus code for mediation, moderation, and moderated mediation models. Retrieved from http://www.offbeat.group.shef.ac.uk/FIO/mplusmedmod.htm Google Scholar
Tao, A., Zhou, Q., & Wang, Y. (2010). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: Prospective relations to Chinese children’s psychological adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(2), 135144. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018974 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, H. J., Chan, G. C., Scott, J. G., Connor, J. P., Kelly, A. B., & Williams, J. (2015). Association of different forms of bullying victimisation with adolescents’ psychological distress and reduced emotional wellbeing. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 50(4), 371379. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867415600076 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Shepard, S. A., Fabes, R. A., Cumberland, A. J., Losoya, S. H., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). The relations of mothers’ negative expressivity to children’s experience and expression of negative emotion. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25(2), 215235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2004.02.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandenberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 3(1), 470. https://doi.org/10.1177/109442810031002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q., Pomerantz, E. M., & Chen, H. (2007). The role of parents? Control in early adolescents? Psychological functioning: A longitudinal investigation in the United States and China. Child Development, 78(5), 15921610. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01085.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Z., , W., & Qin, R. (2013). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with trait positive affect and positive emotional expressivity. Biological Psychology, 93(1), 190196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.12.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weıchold, K., & Barber, B. L. (2009). Introduction to social change and human development. Internatıonal Society for the Study of Behavioural Development, 1, 142.Google Scholar
Yang, W., Xiong, G., Garrido, L. E., Zhang, J. X., Wang, M. C., & Wang, C. (2018). Factor structure and criterion validity across the full scale and ten short forms of the CES-D among Chinese adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 30(9), 1186. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000559 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, Y., & Wang, Q. (2019). Culture in emotional development. In Handbook of Emotional Development (pp. 569593). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-17332-6_22 CrossRefGoogle Scholar